Artigos de revistas sobre o tema "Visual perception"

Siga este link para ver outros tipos de publicações sobre o tema: Visual perception.

Crie uma referência precisa em APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, e outros estilos

Selecione um tipo de fonte:

Veja os 50 melhores artigos de revistas para estudos sobre o assunto "Visual perception".

Ao lado de cada fonte na lista de referências, há um botão "Adicionar à bibliografia". Clique e geraremos automaticamente a citação bibliográfica do trabalho escolhido no estilo de citação de que você precisa: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

Você também pode baixar o texto completo da publicação científica em formato .pdf e ler o resumo do trabalho online se estiver presente nos metadados.

Veja os artigos de revistas das mais diversas áreas científicas e compile uma bibliografia correta.

1

Atasoy, Seda Nur. "Visual perception management in advertisements". New Trends and Issues Proceedings on Humanities and Social Sciences 2, n.º 1 (19 de fevereiro de 2016): 397–409. http://dx.doi.org/10.18844/gjhss.v2i1.324.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

Cooper, John, Nicholas J. Wade e Michael Swanston. "Visual Perception". Leonardo 24, n.º 5 (1991): 631. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1575684.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Ng, Jason S. "Visual Perception". Optometry and Vision Science 95, n.º 12 (dezembro de 2018): 1166–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/opx.0000000000001332.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

KEARINS, JUDITH. "Visual Perception". Australian Occupational Therapy Journal 14, n.º 1 (27 de agosto de 2010): 5–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1630.1967.tb00189.x.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Boselie, F. "Visual perception". Acta Psychologica 75, n.º 2 (novembro de 1990): 174–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(90)90088-w.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

Sahan, Melek. "On three dimensional perceptions in the visual age". Global Journal of Arts Education 5, n.º 1 (14 de abril de 2015): 10. http://dx.doi.org/10.18844/gjae.v5i1.14.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
Aim of the study is to examine the values of the three dimensional perception and collect information about that. Starting point is the observation of differences in two and three dimensional perception abilities of individuals having education in visual arts. It has been observed that some students, who have high level of competency in two dimensional perception, do not have the same competency in three dimensional perception. This is considered important since it creates two and three dimensional perceptions and differences of expression between them. We tried to emphasize the importance of the subject particularly in terms of visual arts education in the current visual age. The study is a document review. In this study we addressed the characteristics of the age we are living in, perception, visual perception, and two and three dimensional perception. We summarized the differences in visual perception and included three dimensional comprehension. By examining the related literature, we came to the conclusion that the two and three dimensional perception processes and their requirements are different. Keywords: sculpture, three dimensional perceptions, visual perception, visual age.
7

Taneja, MK. "Visual speech perception". Indian Journal of Otology 25, n.º 2 (2019): 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.4103/indianjotol.indianjotol_67_19.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

McSorley, Eugene. "Review: Visual Perception". Perception 30, n.º 4 (abril de 2001): 523–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p3004rvw.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Klingenhoefer, Steffen, e Bart Krekelberg. "Perisaccadic visual perception". Journal of Vision 17, n.º 9 (24 de agosto de 2017): 16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/17.9.16.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

Albright, T. D., e G. R. Stoner. "Visual motion perception." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 92, n.º 7 (28 de março de 1995): 2433–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.92.7.2433.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
11

Mascalzoni, Elena, e Lucia Regolin. "Animal visual perception". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2, n.º 1 (20 de agosto de 2010): 106–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.97.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
12

Revonsuo, Antti. "Visual perception and subjective visual awareness". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, n.º 6 (dezembro de 1998): 769–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x98461752.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
Pessoa et al. fail to make a clear distinction between visual perception and subjective visual awareness. Their most controversial claims, however, concern subjective visual awareness rather than visual perception: visual awareness is externalized to the “personal level,” thus denying the view that consciousness is a natural biological phenomenon somehow constructed inside the brain.
13

Hardcastle, Valerie Gray. "Visual perception is not visual awareness". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, n.º 5 (outubro de 2001): 985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01360119.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
O'Regan & Noë mistakenly identify visual processing with visual experience. I outline some reasons why this is a mistake, taking my data and arguments mainly from the literature on subliminal processing.
14

Torales, Julio, Enrique De Doménico Meyer e Eliana Duarte Fariña. "Schizophrenia and visual perception disturbances". Revista Virtual de la Sociedad Paraguaya de Medicina Interna 7, n.º 2 (30 de setembro de 2020): 96–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.18004/rvspmi/2312-3893/2020.07.02.96.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
15

Teraoka, Ryo, e Wataru Teramoto. "Touch-contingent visual motion perception: tactile events drive visual motion perception". Experimental Brain Research 235, n.º 3 (3 de dezembro de 2016): 903–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4850-y.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
16

Legrain, Valéry, Louise Manfron, Marynn Garcia e Lieve Filbrich. "Does Body Perception Shape Visuospatial Perception?" Perception 47, n.º 5 (15 de março de 2018): 507–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0301006618763269.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
How we perceive our body is shaped by sensory experiences with our surrounding environment, as witnessed by poor performance in tasks during which participants judge with their hands crossed the temporal order between two somatosensory stimuli, one applied on each hand. This suggests that somatosensory stimuli are not only processed according to a somatotopic representation but also a spatiotopic representation of the body. We investigated whether the perception of stimuli occurring in external space, such as visual stimuli, can also be influenced by the body posture and somatosensory stimuli. Participants performed temporal order judgements on pairs of visual stimuli, one in each side of space, with their hands uncrossed or crossed. In Experiment 1, participants’ hands were placed either near or far from the visual stimuli. In Experiment 2, the visual stimuli were preceded, either by 60 ms or 360 ms, by tactile stimuli applied on the hands placed near the visual stimuli. Manipulating the time interval was intended to activate either a somatotopic or a spatiotopic representation of somatic inputs. We did not obtain any evidence for an influence of body posture on visual temporal order judgment, suggesting that body perception is less relevant for processing extrabody stimuli than the reverse.
17

Cuijpers, R. H., A. M. L. Kappers e J. J. Koenderink. "Visual perception of collinearity". Perception & Psychophysics 64, n.º 3 (abril de 2002): 392–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03194712.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
18

Duchesneau, André P. "Alexithymia and Visual Perception". Perceptual and Motor Skills 83, n.º 1 (agosto de 1996): 291–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1996.83.1.291.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
60 subjects were administered the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20, a screening test for alexithymia, and categorized by the usual scoring procedure. Each subject was then given the six Performance subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised which address aspects of visual perception, organization, and construction. Analysis indicated that the alexithymic group scored significantly lower on two tests of visual perception, Picture Completion and Digit Symbol of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised.
19

Ittelson, William H. "Visual perception of markings". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3, n.º 2 (junho de 1996): 171–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03212416.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
20

Heider, Daniel. "Suárez on Visual Perception". Scientia et Fides 5, n.º 1 (19 de abril de 2017): 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.12775/setf.2017.003.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
21

NAGAMACHI, Mibuo. "Visual Perception and Visualization." Journal of the Visualization Society of Japan 11, n.º 43 (1991): 201–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.3154/jvs.11.201.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
22

Zahorik, Pavel. "Auditory/visual distance perception". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 137, n.º 4 (abril de 2015): 2374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4920626.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
23

Stryker, Michael P. "Elements of visual perception". Nature 360, n.º 6402 (novembro de 1992): 301–2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/360301a0.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
24

Mirabel, Sylvie. "Visual function and perception". Current Opinion in Ophthalmology 3, n.º 5 (outubro de 1992): 581–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00055735-199210000-00005.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
25

Schomaker, Judith, e Martijn Meeter. "Novelty Enhances Visual Perception". PLoS ONE 7, n.º 12 (5 de dezembro de 2012): e50599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050599.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
26

Zhang, J., e S. Y. Wu. "Structure of visual perception." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 87, n.º 20 (1 de outubro de 1990): 7819–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.87.20.7819.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
27

Brown, Angela M. "Visual Perception: Essential Readings." Optometry and Vision Science 79, n.º 5 (maio de 2002): 283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-200205000-00005.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
28

Celesia, Gastone G. "Visual Perception and Awareness". Journal of Psychophysiology 24, n.º 2 (janeiro de 2010): 62–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000014.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
The study of visual processing and abnormalities due to lesions of cortical structures sheds light on visual awareness/consciousness and may help us to better understand consciousness. We report on clinical observations and psychophysical testing of achromatopsia/prosopagnosia, visual agnosia, and blindsight. Achromatopsia and prosopagnosia reveal that visual cortices have functionally specialized processing systems for color, face perception, and their awareness, and that furthermore these systems operate independently. Dysfunction is limited to some aspects of visual perception; someone with achromatopsia, although not conscious of color, is aware of the objects’ form, motion, and their relationship with sound and other sensory percepts. Perceptual awareness is modular, with neuronal correlates represented by multiple separate specialized structures or modules. Visual agnosia shows that awareness of a complete visual percept is absent, though the subject is aware of single visual features such as edges, motion, etc., an indication that visual agnosia is a disruption of the binding process that unifies all information into a whole percept. Blindsight is characterized by the subject’s ability to localize a visual target while denying actually seeing the target. Blindsight is mediated by residual islands of the visual cortex, which suggests that sensory modules responsible for awareness can function only when structurally intact. We conclude (1) that perceptual awareness (consciousness?) is modular, and (2) that perceptual integration is also modular, which suggests that integration among distinct cortical regions is a parallel process with multiple communication pathways. Any hypothesis about consciousness must include these observations about the presence of multiple parallel, but spatially and temporally different, mechanisms.
29

Beauchamp, Ross. "THEORIES OF VISUAL PERCEPTION". Optometry and Vision Science 68, n.º 9 (setembro de 1991): 758. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-199109000-00019.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
30

Griffin, John R., Todd F. Birch, Gordon F. Bateman e Paul N. De Land. "Dyslexia and Visual Perception". Optometry and Vision Science 70, n.º 5 (maio de 1993): 374–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-199305000-00006.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
31

Jolij, Jacob, e Maaike Meurs. "Music Alters Visual Perception". PLoS ONE 6, n.º 4 (21 de abril de 2011): e18861. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018861.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
32

Stuchlíková, Iva, Pavel Kindlman e Frantisek Man. "Anxiety in visual perception". Anxiety, Stress & Coping 9, n.º 1 (janeiro de 1996): 87–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615809608249394.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
33

Troscianko, Tom, Christopher P. Benton, P. George Lovell, David J. Tolhurst e Zygmunt Pizlo. "Camouflage and visual perception". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364, n.º 1516 (6 de novembro de 2008): 449–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0218.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
How does an animal conceal itself from visual detection by other animals? This review paper seeks to identify general principles that may apply in this broad area. It considers mechanisms of visual encoding, of grouping and object encoding, and of search. In most cases, the evidence base comes from studies of humans or species whose vision approximates to that of humans. The effort is hampered by a relatively sparse literature on visual function in natural environments and with complex foraging tasks. However, some general constraints emerge as being potentially powerful principles in understanding concealment—a ‘constraint’ here means a set of simplifying assumptions. Strategies that disrupt the unambiguous encoding of discontinuities of intensity (edges), and of other key visual attributes, such as motion, are key here. Similar strategies may also defeat grouping and object-encoding mechanisms. Finally, the paper considers how we may understand the processes of search for complex targets in complex scenes. The aim is to provide a number of pointers towards issues, which may be of assistance in understanding camouflage and concealment, particularly with reference to how visual systems can detect the shape of complex, concealed objects.
34

Boynton, Geoffrey M. "Attention and visual perception". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15, n.º 4 (agosto de 2005): 465–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.06.009.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
35

Hartmann, Joel A., William A. Wolz, David P. Roeltgen e Felice L. Loverso. "Denial of visual perception". Brain and Cognition 16, n.º 1 (maio de 1991): 29–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(91)90083-k.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
36

Slater, Alan M. "Visual perception at birth". Infant Behavior and Development 9 (abril de 1986): 346. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(86)80352-6.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
37

Hel, Francois. "Visual Perception Entropy Measure". SMPTE Motion Imaging Journal 128, n.º 10 (novembro de 2019): 51–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.5594/jmi.2019.2941361.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
38

Zakia, Richard D. "Photography and Visual Perception". Journal of Aesthetic Education 27, n.º 4 (1993): 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3333501.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
39

Langley, Keith. "Visual perception and encoding". Spatial Vision 18, n.º 4 (2005): 375–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568568054389598.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
40

Matthews, Julia. "Neurobiology of Visual Perception". Psychoanalytic Quarterly 63, n.º 2 (abril de 1994): 392–403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21674086.1994.11927419.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
41

Nakayama, K. "Binocular visual surface perception." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93, n.º 2 (23 de janeiro de 1996): 634–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.634.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
42

Faubert, Jocelyn. "Visual perception and aging." Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 56, n.º 3 (2002): 164–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087394.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
43

ffytche, D. H., J. D. Blom e M. Catani. "Disorders of visual perception". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 81, n.º 11 (22 de outubro de 2010): 1280–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2008.171348.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
44

Doi, S. A. R. "Visual perception — A hypothesis". Medical Hypotheses 35, n.º 2 (junho de 1991): 148–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-9877(91)90039-2.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
45

Vos, P. G. "Theories of visual perception". Acta Psychologica 75, n.º 1 (outubro de 1990): 91–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(90)90068-q.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
46

Wagemans, Johan. "Visual perception: An introduction". Acta Psychologica 81, n.º 1 (outubro de 1992): 91–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(92)90016-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
47

Johnson, Scott P. "Development of visual perception". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2, n.º 5 (22 de novembro de 2010): 515–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.128.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
48

Intraub, Helene. "Rethinking visual scene perception". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 3, n.º 1 (6 de maio de 2011): 117–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.149.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
49

Porto, André. "SINGULARITY AND VISUAL PERCEPTION". Revista Dissertatio de Filosofia 58 (13 de março de 2024): 218–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.15210/dissertatio.v58i.26546.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
This paper deals with the mutations in Wittgenstein’s treatment of the notions of “generality”and of “singularity”, from his first philosophy, in the Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus, to his later maturephilosophy represented by the Philosophical Investigations. As we shall see, Wittgenstein’s philosophicalhandling of the notion of “visual perception” plays a key role in those conceptual transformations.
50

Ishai, Alumit, e Dov Sagi. "Visual Imagery Facilitates Visual Perception: Psychophysical Evidence". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9, n.º 4 (julho de 1997): 476–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.476.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Resumo:
Visual imagery is the invention or recreation of a perceptual experience in the absence of retinal input.The degree to which the same neural representations are involved in both visual imagery and visual perception is unclear. Previous studies have shown that visual imagery interferes with perception (Perky effect). We report here psychophysical data showing a direct facilitatory effect of visual imagery on visual perception. Using a lateral masking detection paradigm of a Gabor target, flanked by peripheral Gabor masks, observers performed imagery tasks that were preceded by perceptual tasks. We found that both perceived and imaginary flanking masks can reduce contrast detection threshold. At short target-to-mask distances imagery induced a threshold reduction of 50% as compared with perception, while at long target-to-mask distances imagery and perception had similar facilitatory effect. The imagery-induced facilitation was specific to the orientation of the stimulus, as well as to the eye used in the task. These data indicate the existence of a stimulus-specific short-term memory system that stores the sensory trace and enables reactivation of quasi-pictorial representations by topdown processes. We suggest that stimulus parameters dominate the imagery-induced facilitation at short target-to-mask distances, yet the topdown component contributes to the effect at long target-to-mask distances.

Vá para a bibliografia