Siga este link para ver outros tipos de publicações sobre o tema: Human information processing.

Artigos de revistas sobre o tema "Human information processing"

Crie uma referência precisa em APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, e outros estilos

Selecione um tipo de fonte:

Veja os 50 melhores artigos de revistas para estudos sobre o assunto "Human information processing".

Ao lado de cada fonte na lista de referências, há um botão "Adicionar à bibliografia". Clique e geraremos automaticamente a citação bibliográfica do trabalho escolhido no estilo de citação de que você precisa: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

Você também pode baixar o texto completo da publicação científica em formato .pdf e ler o resumo do trabalho online se estiver presente nos metadados.

Veja os artigos de revistas das mais diversas áreas científicas e compile uma bibliografia correta.

1

CALLISTA ROY, SISTER. "Human Information Processing". Annual Review of Nursing Research 6, n.º 1 (setembro de 1988): 237–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0739-6686.6.1.237.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

Callaway, Enoch, Roy Halliday, Hilary Naylor, Lovelle Yano e Karen Herzig. "Drugs and Human Information Processing". Neuropsychopharmacology 10, n.º 1 (fevereiro de 1994): 9–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.1994.2.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Velmans, Max. "Is human information processing conscious?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14, n.º 4 (dezembro de 1991): 651–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00071776.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
AbstractInvestigations of the function of consciousness in human information processing have focused mainly on two questions: (1) Where does consciousness enter into the information processing sequence, and (2) how does conscious processing differ from preconscious and unconscious processing? Input analysis is thought to be initially “preconscious” and “pre-attentive” - fast, involuntary, and automatic. This is followed by “conscious,” “focal-attentive” analysis, which is relatively slow, voluntary, and flexible. It is thought that simple, familiar stimuli can be identified preconsciously, but conscious processing is needed to identify complex, novel stimuli. Conscious processing has also been thought to be necessary for choice, learning and memory, and the organization of complex, novel responses, particularly those requiring planning, reflection, or creativity.The present target article reviews evidence that consciousness performs none of these functions. Consciousness nearly alwaysresultsfrom focal-attentive processing (as a form of output) but does not itselfenter intothis or any other form of human information processing. This suggests that the term “conscious process” needs reexamination. Consciousnessappearsto be necessary in a variety of tasks because they require focal-attentive processing; if consciousness is absent, focal-attentive processing is absent. From afirst-person perspective, however, conscious statesarecausally effective. First-person accounts arecomplementaryto third-person accounts. Although they can be translated into third-person accounts, they cannot be reduced to them.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

Petrie, Rachel X. A., e Ian J. Deary. "Smoking and human information processing". Psychopharmacology 99, n.º 3 (novembro de 1989): 393–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00445565.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Spangenberg, J. F. A., e F. J. N. Nijhuis. "Human information processing in science". Scientometrics 18, n.º 5-6 (maio de 1990): 389–407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02020153.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

Rogers, Tracy. "Human information processing under stress". Journal of Emergency Management 21, n.º 2 (16 de maio de 2023): 141–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.5055/jem.0756.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
This article is a review of literature focused on how human beings process information under stress. Three major theories on information processing are reviewed: cue utilization theory, attentional control theory, and working memory capacity theory. Different conditions that can cause an individual to feel stress are examined, how stress affects information processing, ways which stress may be beneficial, and different ways to mitigate stress, so they may process information more accurately and efficiently. Throughout the article, examples of how stress can affect incident commanders responding to a disaster situation are used to illustrate research findings.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Ishii, Hiroshi. "The Dawn of Kansei Information Processing. Human Interface and 'Kansei' Information Processing." Journal of the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineers 52, n.º 1 (1998): 56–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.3169/itej.52.56.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Lynn, Christopher W., Lia Papadopoulos, Ari E. Kahn e Danielle S. Bassett. "Human information processing in complex networks". Nature Physics 16, n.º 9 (15 de junho de 2020): 965–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41567-020-0924-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

PAAS, FRED G. W. C., e JOS J. ADAM. "Human information processing during physical exercise". Ergonomics 34, n.º 11 (novembro de 1991): 1385–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00140139108964879.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

Robertson, Ivan T. "Human information-processing strategies and style". Behaviour & Information Technology 4, n.º 1 (janeiro de 1985): 19–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01449298508901784.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
11

Fedzechkina, Maryia, Becky Chu e T. Florian Jaeger. "Human Information Processing Shapes Language Change". Psychological Science 29, n.º 1 (1 de dezembro de 2017): 72–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797617728726.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Human languages exhibit both striking diversity and abstract commonalities. Whether these commonalities are shaped by potentially universal principles of human information processing has been of central interest in the language and psychological sciences. Research has identified one such abstract property in the domain of word order: Although sentence word-order preferences vary across languages, the superficially different orders result in short grammatical dependencies between words. Because dependencies are easier to process when they are short rather than long, these findings raise the possibility that languages are shaped by biases of human information processing. In the current study, we directly tested the hypothesized causal link. We found that learners exposed to novel miniature artificial languages that had unnecessarily long dependencies did not follow the surface preference of their native language but rather systematically restructured the input to reduce dependency lengths. These results provide direct evidence for a causal link between processing preferences in individual speakers and patterns in linguistic diversity.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
12

Callaway, Enoch. "The Biology of Human Information Processing". Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 18, n.º 4 (outubro de 1986): 315–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02791072.1986.10472363.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
13

Rugg, Michael D. "Psychophysiological approaches to human information processing". Biological Psychology 22, n.º 2 (abril de 1986): 208–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(86)90073-6.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
14

Wilkinson, Thomas M. "Human Information Processing, Health Information Technology, and Medical Outcomes". JAMA 302, n.º 13 (7 de outubro de 2009): 1417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1417.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
15

Ramsey, N. F. "Neurophysiological factors in human information processing capacity". Brain 127, n.º 3 (7 de novembro de 2003): 517–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh060.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
16

Jayles, Bertrand, Ramón Escobedo, Roberto Pasqua, Christophe Zanon, Adrien Blanchet, Matthieu Roy, Gilles Tredan, Guy Theraulaz e Clément Sire. "Collective information processing in human phase separation". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 375, n.º 1807 (27 de julho de 2020): 20190801. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0801.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
In our digital societies, individuals massively interact through digital interfaces whose impact on collective dynamics can be important. In particular, the combination of social media filters and recommender systems can lead to the emergence of polarized and fragmented groups. In some social contexts, such segregation processes of human groups have been shown to share similarities with phase separation phenomena in physics. Here, we study the impact of information filtering on collective segregation behaviour of human groups. We report a series of experiments where groups of 22 subjects have to perform a collective segregation task that mimics the tendency of individuals to bond with other similar individuals. More precisely, the participants are each assigned a colour (red or blue) unknown to them, and have to regroup with other subjects sharing the same colour. To assist them, they are equipped with an artificial sensory device capable of detecting the majority colour in their ‘environment’ (defined as their k nearest neighbours, unbeknownst to them), for which we control the perception range, k = 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13. We study the separation dynamics (emergence of unicolour groups) and the properties of the final state, and show that the value of k controls the quality of the segregation, although the subjects are totally unaware of the precise definition of the ‘environment’. We also find that there is a perception range k = 7 above which the ability of the group to segregate does not improve. We introduce a model that precisely describes the random motion of a group of pedestrians in a confined space, and which faithfully reproduces and allows interpretation of the results of the segregation experiments. Finally, we discuss the strong and precise analogy between our experiment and the phase separation of two immiscible materials at very low temperature. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Multi-scale analysis and modelling of collective migration in biological systems’.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
17

Spivey, Michael J. "Redesigning our theories of human information processing". Discourse, Cognition and Communication 15, n.º 3 (12 de dezembro de 2007): 261–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/idj.15.3.08spi.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
18

KITAGAWA, NORIMICHI, e CHARLES SPENCE. "Audiotactile multisensory interactions in human information processing". Japanese Psychological Research 48, n.º 3 (setembro de 2006): 158–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.2006.00317.x.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
19

Dickhaut, John, Vernon Smith, Baohua Xin e Aldo Rustichini. "Human economic choice as costly information processing". Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 94 (outubro de 2013): 206–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2013.08.001.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
20

Sanders, Andries F. "Human information processing: Measures, mechanisms and models". Acta Psychologica 76, n.º 2 (abril de 1991): 199–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(91)90048-5.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
21

Ren, Fuji. "Affective Information Processing and Recognizing Human Emotion". Electronic Notes in Theoretical Computer Science 225 (janeiro de 2009): 39–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.entcs.2008.12.065.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
22

Duncan, James R. "Human Information Processing, Health Information Technology, and Medical Outcomes—Reply". JAMA 302, n.º 13 (7 de outubro de 2009): 1417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1418.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
23

Alvandi, Ebrahim Oshni. "Emotions and Information Processing". International Journal of Synthetic Emotions 2, n.º 1 (janeiro de 2011): 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jse.2011010101.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
An animate system standing in nature and trying to investigate its surroundings for different purposes does a type of cognitive processing. Emotions as mental states are leading human cognitive features that attract life by interactions processed in the world. This paper examines how this cognitive feature process works. By researching history and theories related to emotions and their generation, it becomes clear that information processing is discussed as a tool for their processes. Three different styles of information processing are evaluated for emotional processes. The pragmatic notion of information processing fits as a processing tool in modeling emotions and artificial emotions and explains the emotional process.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
24

Rikani, Azadeh A., Zia Choudhry, Adnan Maqsood Choudhry, Nasir Rizvi e Huma Ikram. "Spatial information processing by the human visual system". El Mednifico Journal 2, n.º 3 (25 de julho de 2014): 309. http://dx.doi.org/10.18035/emj.v2i3.156.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
25

Lynn, Christopher W., Lia Papadopoulos, Ari E. Kahn e Danielle S. Bassett. "Author Correction: Human information processing in complex networks". Nature Physics 16, n.º 12 (7 de julho de 2020): 1238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41567-020-0985-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
26

Driver, Michael J., Katarina Svensson, Roy P. Amato e Larry E. Pate. "A Human-Information-Processing Approach to Strategic Change". International Studies of Management & Organization 26, n.º 1 (março de 1996): 41–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00208825.1996.11656673.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
27

No authorship indicated. "Review of Eye Movements and Human Information Processing." Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 31, n.º 10 (outubro de 1986): 820. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/024205.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
28

Lounasmaa, O. V., M. Hamalainen, R. Hari e R. Salmelin. "Information processing in the human brain: magnetoencephalographic approach." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93, n.º 17 (20 de agosto de 1996): 8809–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.17.8809.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
29

Truta, Meda, e Mihai Popescu. "Intelligent Control System Based on Human Information Processing". IFAC Proceedings Volumes 34, n.º 3 (maio de 2001): 117–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1474-6670(17)34336-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
30

Howes, Andrew, Richard L. Lewis e Satinder Singh. "Utility Maximization and Bounds on Human Information Processing". Topics in Cognitive Science 6, n.º 2 (20 de março de 2014): 198–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/tops.12089.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
31

Seo, Chang-Jin. "Artificial Vision System using Human Visual Information Processing". Journal of Digital Convergence 12, n.º 11 (28 de novembro de 2014): 349–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.14400/jdc.2014.12.11.349.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
32

Coles, MichaelG H. "Psychophysiology and contemporary models of human information processing". Biological Psychology 20, n.º 3 (maio de 1985): 189–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(85)90066-3.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
33

Ramanujan, S., e RB Cooper. "A human information processing perspective on software maintenance". Omega 22, n.º 2 (março de 1994): 185–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0305-0483(94)90078-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
34

Czigler, István, Gergely Csibra e Ágnes Ambró. "Age and Information Processing". European Psychologist 2, n.º 3 (janeiro de 1997): 247–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.2.3.247.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
This paper reviews our recent studies on the effects of aging on human information processing. In these studies the event-related potentials of the brain (ERPs) recorded in visual discrimination tasks were compared in younger and older groups of subjects in four experiments. We obtained a slight age-related delay of the NA component of the ERP. This component is a correlate of elementary pattern-identification processes. Obvious latency differences appeared on the anterior positivity, selection negativity, and N2b components in tasks where the target stimuli were defined by two stimulus characteristics. These components are correlates of attentional processes, i.e., the results support the view emphasizing age-related decline of the attentional processes. In the elderly the late positivity was less sensitive to stimulus probability, and in the older groups this component was more evenly distributed over the scalp. These results are considered as an indication that the structure of stimulus sequences was less efficiently represented in the older subjects.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
35

Yang, C. Y. David, e Jon D. Fricker. "Using Human Information Processing Principles to Design Advanced Traveler Information Systems". Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 1759, n.º 1 (janeiro de 2001): 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3141/1759-01.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
36

Fagan, Joseph, e Robert E. Ployhart. "The information processing foundations of human capital resources: Leveraging insights from information processing approaches to intelligence". Human Resource Management Review 25, n.º 1 (março de 2015): 4–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2014.09.003.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
37

Diamant, Emanuel. "Modeling human-like intelligent image processing: An information processing perspective and approach". Signal Processing: Image Communication 22, n.º 6 (julho de 2007): 583–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.image.2007.05.007.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
38

Chakarov, Vihren E., Krassimir T. Atanassov e Anthony G. Shannon. "Generalized Net Model of Auditory Information Processing". Journal of Advanced Computational Intelligence and Intelligent Informatics 11, n.º 5 (20 de junho de 2007): 452–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.20965/jaciii.2007.p0452.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
In a series of papers we have described the human body, its separate systems, and their behavior using Generalized Nets (GNs). Here we construct a GN-model that represents auditory information processing - the first on our research on the structure and behavior of the human nervous system.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
39

Jackson, Russell E., e Dustin P. Calvillo. "Evolutionary Relevance Facilitates Visual Information Processing". Evolutionary Psychology 11, n.º 5 (dezembro de 2013): 147470491301100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/147470491301100506.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Visual search of the environment is a fundamental human behavior that perceptual load affects powerfully. Previously investigated means for overcoming the inhibitions of high perceptual load, however, generalize poorly to real-world human behavior. We hypothesized that humans would process evolutionarily relevant stimuli more efficiently than evolutionarily novel stimuli, and evolutionary relevance would mitigate the repercussions of high perceptual load during visual search. Animacy is a significant component to evolutionary relevance of visual stimuli because perceiving animate entities is time-sensitive in ways that pose significant evolutionary consequences. Participants completing a visual search task located evolutionarily relevant and animate objects fastest and with the least impact of high perceptual load. Evolutionarily novel and inanimate objects were located slowest and with the highest impact of perceptual load. Evolutionary relevance may importantly affect everyday visual information processing.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
40

Burgin, Mark, Eugene Eberbach e Rao Mikkilineni. "Processing Information in the Clouds". Proceedings 47, n.º 1 (7 de maio de 2020): 25. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/proceedings2020047025.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Cloud computing makes the necessary resources available to the appropriate computation to improve scaling, resiliency, and the efficiency of computations. This makes cloud computing a new paradigm for computation by upgrading its artificial intelligence (AI) to a higher order. To explore cloud computing using theoretical tools, we use cloud automata as a new model for computation. Higher-level AI requires infusing features of the human brain into AI systems such as incremental learning all the time. Consequently, we propose computational models that exhibit incremental learning without stopping (sentience). These features are inherent in reflexive Turing machines, inductive Turing machines, and limit Turing machines.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
41

Burgin, Mark, Eugene Eberbach e Rao Mikkilineni. "Processing Information in the Clouds". Proceedings 47, n.º 1 (7 de maio de 2020): 25. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/proceedings47010025.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Cloud computing makes the necessary resources available to the appropriate computation to improve scaling, resiliency, and the efficiency of computations. This makes cloud computing a new paradigm for computation by upgrading its artificial intelligence (AI) to a higher order. To explore cloud computing using theoretical tools, we use cloud automata as a new model for computation. Higher-level AI requires infusing features of the human brain into AI systems such as incremental learning all the time. Consequently, we propose computational models that exhibit incremental learning without stopping (sentience). These features are inherent in reflexive Turing machines, inductive Turing machines, and limit Turing machines.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
42

K.N., Sridevi, e Prakasha S. "Doc-To-Tokens based Pre-Processing in Information Retrieval System". Webology 18, SI05 (30 de outubro de 2021): 570–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.14704/web/v18si05/web18247.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
43

Venturino, Michael. "Timesharing, Cognition, and Human Performance in Complex Information Systems". Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting 30, n.º 9 (setembro de 1986): 880–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193128603000906.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Applications of advanced technology to modern systems have resulted in the requirement for the human operator to process large volumes of information at rapid rates. Such systems may impose substantial attentional and timesharing requirements on the operator, often exceeding human information processing capabilities. Consequently, there is a critical need to investigate and understand the human information processing system in order to identify generalizable, empirically derived principles of human timesharing capabilities. Of particular interest are current theoretical approaches to human attention and information processing limitations that suggest ways to improve timesharing performance. Two of these approaches, automatic information processing theory and multiple resource theory are especially relevant to the timesharing problem.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
44

Massaro, Dominic W., e Donald A. Norman. "Memory and Attention: An Introduction to Human Information Processing". American Journal of Psychology 107, n.º 4 (1994): 597. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1423002.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
45

Hatamoto, K., M. Nagamachi, K. Ito e T. Tsuji. "Human visual information processing on formation of subjective contours". Japanese journal of ergonomics 25, Supplement (1989): 112–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.5100/jje.25.supplement_112.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
46

Godlewska, Magdalena. "Smart Document-Centric Processing of Human Oriented Information Flows". Computing and Informatics 37, n.º 3 (2018): 673–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.4149/cai_2018_3_673.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
47

Rappelsberger, P., N. Vath, S. Weiss, E. Möller, G. Grießbach, H. Witte e B. Schack. "EEG Frequency and Phase Coupling during Human Information Processing". Methods of Information in Medicine 40, n.º 02 (2001): 106–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0038-1634470.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
AbstractNeuronal activity during information processing is represented by oscillations within local or widespread neuronal networks. These oscillations may be recorded by the EEG (electroencephalogram). The oscillatory interaction between neuronal ensembles may be at one single frequency or at different frequencies due to non-linear coupling. The investigation of momentary coherence and phase enables the examination of synchronized oscillatory network activity during fast-changing cognitive processes. On this basis information transfer from occipital areas towards frontal areas could be described during processing of visual presented words. Non-linear phase coupling between oscillations with different frequencies during memory processing was detected by means of cross-bicoherence.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
48

Foss, Donald J. "Review of Human Information Processing: Measures, Mechanisms, and Models." Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 36, n.º 3 (março de 1991): 263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/029570.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
49

Sobol, M. G., e G. Klein. "New graphics as computerized displays for human information processing". IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics 19, n.º 4 (1989): 893–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/21.35357.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
50

Strawser, Jerry R. "Human Information Processing the Consistency of Audit Risk Judgments". Accounting and Business Research 21, n.º 81 (dezembro de 1990): 67–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00014788.1990.9729405.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Oferecemos descontos em todos os planos premium para autores cujas obras estão incluídas em seleções literárias temáticas. Contate-nos para obter um código promocional único!

Vá para a bibliografia