Siga este link para ver outros tipos de publicações sobre o tema: Connectionism.

Artigos de revistas sobre o tema "Connectionism"

Crie uma referência precisa em APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, e outros estilos

Selecione um tipo de fonte:

Veja os 50 melhores artigos de revistas para estudos sobre o assunto "Connectionism".

Ao lado de cada fonte na lista de referências, há um botão "Adicionar à bibliografia". Clique e geraremos automaticamente a citação bibliográfica do trabalho escolhido no estilo de citação de que você precisa: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

Você também pode baixar o texto completo da publicação científica em formato .pdf e ler o resumo do trabalho online se estiver presente nos metadados.

Veja os artigos de revistas das mais diversas áreas científicas e compile uma bibliografia correta.

1

MARATSOS, M. P. "Problems of Connectionism: Connections and Symbols." Science 242, n.º 4883 (2 de dezembro de 1988): 1316–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.242.4883.1316.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

Vassallo, Marta, Davide Sattin, Eugenio Parati e Mario Picozzi. "Problems of Connectionism". Philosophies 9, n.º 2 (25 de março de 2024): 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/philosophies9020041.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
The relationship between philosophy and science has always been complementary. Today, while science moves increasingly fast and philosophy shows some problems in catching up with it, it is not always possible to ignore such relationships, especially in some disciplines such as philosophy of mind, cognitive science, and neuroscience. However, the methodological procedures used to analyze these data are based on principles and assumptions that require a profound dialogue between philosophy and science. Following these ideas, this work aims to raise the problems that a classical connectionist theory can cause and problematize them in a cognitive framework, considering both philosophy and cognitive sciences but also the disciplines that are near to them, such as AI, computer sciences, and linguistics. For this reason, we embarked on an analysis of both the computational and theoretical problems that connectionism currently has. The second aim of this work is to advocate for collaboration between neuroscience and philosophy of mind because the promotion of deeper multidisciplinarity seems necessary in order to solve connectionism’s problems. In fact, we believe that the problems that we detected can be solved by a thorough investigation at both a theoretical and an empirical level, and they do not represent an impasse but rather a starting point from which connectionism should learn and be updated while keeping its original and profoundly convincing core.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Naidu, Som. "Connectionism". Distance Education 33, n.º 3 (novembro de 2012): 291–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01587919.2012.723321.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

Fowler, David, e David W. Brooks. "Connectionism". Journal of Chemical Education 68, n.º 9 (setembro de 1991): 748. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed068p748.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Gasser, Michael. "Connectionism and Universals of Second Language Acquisition". Studies in Second Language Acquisition 12, n.º 2 (junho de 1990): 179–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0272263100009074.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
This article examines the implications of connectionist models of cognition for second language theory. Connectionism offers a challenge to the symbolic models which dominate cognitive science. In connectionist models all knowledge is embodied in a network of simple processing units joined by connections which are strengthened or weakened in response to regularities in input patterns. These models avoid the brittleness of symbolic approaches, and they exhibit rule-like behavior without explicit rules. A connectionist framework is proposed within which hypotheses about second language acquisition can be tested. Inputs and outputs are patterns of activation on units representing both form and meaning. Learning consists of the unsupervised association of pattern elements with one another. A network is first trained on a set of first language patterns and then exposed to a set of second language patterns with the same meanings. Several simulations of constituent-order transfer within this framework are discussed.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

Beaman, C. Philip. "Neurons amongst the symbols?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23, n.º 4 (agosto de 2000): 468–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00233359.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Page's target article presents an argument for the use of localist, connectionist models in future psychological theorising. The “manifesto” marshalls a set of arguments in favour of localist connectionism and against distributed connectionism, but in doing so misses a larger argument concerning the level of psychological explanation that is appropriate to a given domain.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Milojevic, Miljana, e Vanja Subotic. "The exploratory status of postconnectionist models". Theoria, Beograd 63, n.º 2 (2020): 135–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/theo2002135m.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
This paper aims to offer a new view of the role of connectionist models in the study of human cognition through the conceptualization of the history of connectionism - from the simplest perceptrons to convolutional neural nets based on deep learning techniques, as well as through the interpretation of criticism coming from symbolic cognitive science. Namely, the connectionist approach in cognitive science was the target of sharp criticism from the symbolists, which on several occasions caused its marginalization and almost complete abandonment of its assumptions in the study of cognition. Criticisms have mostly pointed to its explanatory inadequacy as a theory of cognition or to its biological implausibility as a theory of implementation, and critics often focused on specific shortcomings of some connectionist models and argued that they apply on connectionism in general. In this paper we want to show that both types of critique are based on the assumption that the only valid explanations in cognitive science are instances of homuncular functionalism and that by removing this assumption and by adopting an alternative methodology - exploratory mechanistic strategy, we can reject most objections to connectionism as irrelevant, explain the progress of connectionist models despite their shortcomings and sketch the trajectory of their future development. By adopting mechanistic explanations and by criticizing functionalism, we will reject the objections of explanatory inadequacy, by characterizing connectionist models as generic rather than concrete mechanisms, we will reject the objections of biological implausibility, and by attributing the exploratory character to connectionist models we will show that practice of generalizing current to general failures of connectionism is unjustified.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Lones, Michael A., Alexander P. Turner, Luis A. Fuente, Susan Stepney, Leo S. D. Caves e Andy M. Tyrrell. "Biochemical connectionism". Natural Computing 12, n.º 4 (20 de outubro de 2013): 453–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11047-013-9400-y.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Berkeley, Istvan S. N. "The Curious Case of Connectionism". Open Philosophy 2, n.º 1 (12 de agosto de 2019): 190–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/opphil-2019-0018.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
AbstractConnectionist research first emerged in the 1940s. The first phase of connectionism attracted a certain amount of media attention, but scant philosophical interest. The phase came to an abrupt halt, due to the efforts of Minsky and Papert (1969), when they argued for the intrinsic limitations of the approach. In the mid-1980s connectionism saw a resurgence. This marked the beginning of the second phase of connectionist research. This phase did attract considerable philosophical attention. It was of philosophical interest, as it offered a way of counteracting the conceptual ties to the philosophical traditions of atomism, rationalism, logic, nativism, rule realism and a concern with the role symbols play in human cognitive functioning, which was prevalent as a consequence of artificial intelligence research. The surge in philosophical interest waned, possibly in part due to the efforts of some traditionalists and the so-called black box problem. Most recently, what may be thought of as a third phase of connectionist research, based on so-called deep learning methods, is beginning to show some signs of again exciting philosophical interest.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

Castro Moreno, Carmen Cayetana. "Secuencia y delimitación en el procesamiento textual alemán: modelo conexionista." Futhark. Revista de Investigación y Cultura, n.º 7 (2012): 85–114. http://dx.doi.org/10.12795/futhark.2012.i07.03.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
In order to simulate human speech, connectionism has developed several techniques for representation of frames and scripts. These models are able to solve some of the shortcomings of classical cognitive systems. Specifically, classical systems are unable to control when a chunk of knowledge has to be activated and what part of the chunk should be more activated. Instead, connectionist systems can use degrees in mobilizing to try this case. However, connectionism has also some limitations if considered as models of human mind, but they also are a point of interest to the application in the prosecution textual. Besides, we examinate the plausibility of any of these approaches used in computers and their implications in our own connectionist model.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
11

CHATER, NICK, e MARTIN REDINGTON. "Connectionism, theories of learning, and syntax acquisition: where do we stand?" Journal of Child Language 26, n.º 1 (fevereiro de 1999): 217–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000998223749.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
12

Phaf, R. Hans. "Connectionism and psychology: A psychological perspective on new connectionist research". Acta Psychologica 85, n.º 2 (abril de 1994): 178–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(94)90033-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
13

Seidenberg, Mark S. "Connectionist Models and Cognitive Theory". Psychological Science 4, n.º 4 (julho de 1993): 228–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00266.x.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Connectionist models have come to play an important role in cognitive science and in cognitive neuroscience, yet their role in explaining behavior is not necessarily obvious and has generated considerable debate. Connectionism is a body of tools and ideas that can be used in different ways. It can be treated as a form of simulation modeling in which the goal is to implement preexisting theories. In this approach, connectionist models function as a kind of statistical tool, a way of analyzing a complex set of data. Connectionism can also be seen as providing a small set of general theoretical principles that apply in a variety of domains. Construed in this way, it contributes to the development of theories that are explanatory, not merely descriptive.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
14

Dobson, Richard, Peter M. Todd, D. Gareth Loy e David Rosenboom. "Music and Connectionism". Notes 49, n.º 4 (junho de 1993): 1497. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/899412.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
15

McLaughlin, Brian. "TYE ON CONNECTIONISM". Southern Journal of Philosophy 26, S1 (26 de março de 2010): 185–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-6962.1988.tb00472.x.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
16

SCHWARZ, GEORG. "Connectionism, Processing, Memory". Connection Science 4, n.º 3-4 (janeiro de 1992): 207–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540099208946616.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
17

Kassirer, Jerome P., e Richard I. Kopelman. "Cognition and Connectionism". Hospital Practice 25, n.º 2 (15 de fevereiro de 1990): 15–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21548331.1990.11703906.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
18

McNaughton, Neil. "Evolution and connectionism". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13, n.º 2 (junho de 1990): 402–3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00079449.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
19

Zucker, Steven W. "Does connectionism suffice?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, n.º 2 (julho de 1985): 301–2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00020823.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
20

Smolensky, Paul. "Connectionism and implementation". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10, n.º 3 (setembro de 1987): 492–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00023712.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
21

Sterelny, Kim. "Connectionism Rules, OK?" Dialogue 32, n.º 3 (1993): 545–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0012217300012312.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Those familiar with Paul Churchland's earlier work will expect A Neuro-computational Perspective to be lively, provocative and interesting. They will not be disappointed. Churchland is best known for his sceptical view of belief-desire psychology. He suspects this theory is hopelessly false. This welcome collection of his essays includes this work but also his papers on the subjective aspects of the mind and his more recent adventures in philosophy of science. Three themes unify the collection: an anti-sententialist view of cognition, an emphasis on the plasticity of the human mind and a strong endorsement of connectionist models of human cognitive processes.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
22

Cordeschi, Roberto. "Early-connectionism machines". AI & Society 14, n.º 3-4 (setembro de 2000): 314–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf01205514.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
23

Sánchez, V. D. A. "Connectionism in perspective". Neurocomputing 2, n.º 3 (junho de 1991): 135–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0925-2312(91)90059-k.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
24

Biggs, Norman. "Combinatorics and connectionism". Discrete Mathematics 124, n.º 1-3 (janeiro de 1994): 19–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0012-365x(92)00048-v.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
25

Clapin, Hugh. "Connectionism isn't magic". Minds and Machines 1, n.º 2 (maio de 1991): 167–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00361035.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
26

Bechtel, William. "Currents in connectionism". Minds and Machines 3, n.º 2 (maio de 1993): 125–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00975529.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
27

O'Brien, Gerard J. "Is connectionism commonsense?" Philosophical Psychology 4, n.º 2 (janeiro de 1991): 165–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09515089108573024.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
28

Plunkett, Kim. "Connectionism and development". Infant Behavior and Development 19 (abril de 1996): 178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(96)90233-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
29

Smith, Linda B. "Teleology in connectionism". Developmental Science 5, n.º 2 (maio de 2002): 178–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00218_4.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
30

DiPalma, Louis P., e David J. Hamilton. "Music and Connectionism". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 96, n.º 2 (agosto de 1994): 1218–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.410341.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
31

Garton, Brad. "Music and connectionism". Artificial Intelligence 79, n.º 2 (dezembro de 1995): 387–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(95)90015-2.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
32

Marcus, Gary F. "Rethinking Eliminative Connectionism". Cognitive Psychology 37, n.º 3 (dezembro de 1998): 243–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0694.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
33

Banan, Shahryar, Muhammad Ridwan e Abdurahman Adisaputera. "A Study of Connectionism Theory". Budapest International Research and Critics Institute (BIRCI-Journal): Humanities and Social Sciences 3, n.º 3 (13 de agosto de 2020): 2335–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.33258/birci.v3i3.1181.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
The development of connectionism represents a paradigm shift in science. Connectionism has its root in cognitive and computational neuroscience. Likening the brain to a computer, connectionism tries to describe human mental abilities in terms of artificial neural networks. A neural network consists of a large number of nodes and units which are joined together to form an interconnection network. Within these interconnections, knowledge is distributed. Therefore learning is a processing by-product. This article is about the concept of connectionism, what it accounts for and what it doesn't take into account. Finally, different approaches to connectionism are discussed.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
34

HAHN, ULRIKE. "Language acquisition also needs non-connectionist models". Journal of Child Language 26, n.º 1 (fevereiro de 1999): 217–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000998283747.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Rethinking innateness is a timely volume which forcefully demonstrates the importance of modelling in understanding development, ‘innateness’, and the nature of change. It provides an inspiring vision of what developmental psychology could one day be like, linking behaviour and biology via connectionist models. However, Rispoli's worry about the book's potential for detrimental polarization does not seem unfounded. One aspect of the book that deserves comment in this respect is the focus on connectionism to the exclusion of other types of model. It is unclear from Rethinking innateness itself whether this exclusion of other approaches merely stems from the legitimate desire to write a focused book, or whether it possibly reflects an actively held view that connectionism is the one true approach to modelling development. Regardless of the authors' intentions, the role of connectionism in relation to other computational approaches is an issue which is particularly pertinent to the study of language acquisition.To clarify straightaway, I not only concur with Elman et al. on the central role of modelling, but also strongly believe that the task of the cognitive scientist is not complete until one has an account of how a particular process is realised in a neural architecture. These two commitments give connectionist models a central role. But they do not make connectionism the exclusive modelling tool for the study of language acquisition, nor necessarily the best path to currently pursue. The reasons for this are twofold.The first is a matter of research strategy. It is possible that more rapid progress and greater success might be made if one starts with high-level models which give less immediate regard to matters of implementation.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
35

O'Loughlin, Claire F., e Annette Karmiloff-Smith. "Evaluating connectionism: A developmental perspective". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26, n.º 5 (outubro de 2003): 614–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x03310131.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
This commentary questions the applicability of the Newell Test for evaluating the utility of connectionism. Rather than being a specific theory of cognition (because connectionism can be used to model nativist, behaviorist, or constructivist theories), connectionism, we argue, offers researchers a collection of computational and conceptual tools that are particularly useful for investigating and rendering specific fundamental issues of human development. These benefits of connectionism are not well captured by evaluating it against Newell's criteria for a unified theory of cognition.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
36

Hadley, Robert F. "The Problem of Rapid Variable Creation". Neural Computation 21, n.º 2 (fevereiro de 2009): 510–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/neco.2008.07-07-572.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Both Marcus ( 2001 ) and Jackendoff ( 2002 ) have emphasized the importance of finding credible explanations for the occurrence of variables within cognitive representations. Marcus, in particular, has argued that a prevailing form of connectionist modeling, eliminative connectionism, cannot adequately explain crucial forms of human generalization. Eliminative connectionism eschews the use of explicitly represented variables, and the latter, Marcus contends, play an essential role in the forms of generalization that he considers. Recently, van der Velde and de Kamps ( 2006 ) proposed a neural blackboard architecture, which they assert to have satisfied the variable representation needs that Marcus and Jackendoff identified. However, this letter argues that closely related variants of Marcus's generalization examples possess variable requirements that are incompatible with the van der Velde and de Kamps approach. Moreover, it is argued here that these newly proposed variants present a severe challenge not only for eliminative connectionism but for all network training methods that require iterative tuning of synaptic strengths. The letter focuses on generalization cases that necessitate either virtually instantaneous creation of variables or very rapid deployment of preexisting variables within highly novel contexts.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
37

Thaler, Stephen. "Lessons from connectionism in differentiating knowledge types". e-mentor 2014, n.º 55 (3) (30 de junho de 2014): 81–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.15219/em55.1112.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
38

Mills, Stephen. "Smolensky’s Interpretation of Connectionism". Irish Philosophical Journal 7, n.º 1 (1990): 104–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/irishphil199071/25.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
39

Lycan, William G. "Connectionism and the Mental". Noûs 25, n.º 2 (abril de 1991): 207. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2215585.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
40

Plunkett, Kim, e Chris Sinha. "Connectionism and developmental theory". British Journal of Developmental Psychology 10, n.º 3 (setembro de 1992): 209–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.1992.tb00575.x.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
41

Fetzer, James H. "What makes connectionism different?" Pragmatics and Cognition 2, n.º 2 (1 de janeiro de 1994): 327–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/pc.2.2.09fet.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
42

Goldstein, Laurence, e Hartley Slater. "Wittgenstein, Semantics and Connectionism". Philosophical Investigations 21, n.º 4 (outubro de 1998): 293–314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9205.00074.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
43

Plunkett, Kim. "Language Acquisition and Connectionism". Language and Cognitive Processes 13, n.º 2-3 (junho de 1998): 97–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/016909698386483.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
44

Tienson, John L. "AN INTRODUCTION TO CONNECTIONISM". Southern Journal of Philosophy 26, S1 (26 de março de 2010): 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-6962.1988.tb00460.x.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
45

Graham, George. "CONNECTIONISM IN PAVLOVIAN HARNESS". Southern Journal of Philosophy 26, S1 (26 de março de 2010): 73–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-6962.1988.tb00465.x.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
46

HARRIS, CATHERINE L. "Connectionism and Cognitive Linguistics". Connection Science 2, n.º 1-2 (janeiro de 1990): 7–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540099008915660.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
47

CHRISTIANSEN, MORTEN H., e NICK CHATER. "Connectionism, Learning and Meaning". Connection Science 4, n.º 3-4 (janeiro de 1992): 227–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540099208946617.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
48

North, Geoffrey. "A celebration of connectionism". Nature 328, n.º 6126 (julho de 1987): 107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/328107a0.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
49

Dulany, Donelson E. "Consciousness, connectionism, and intentionality". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, n.º 1 (fevereiro de 1999): 154–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99281794.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
50

Marcus, Gary F. "Can connectionism save constructivism?" Cognition 66, n.º 2 (maio de 1998): 153–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00018-3.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Oferecemos descontos em todos os planos premium para autores cujas obras estão incluídas em seleções literárias temáticas. Contate-nos para obter um código promocional único!

Vá para a bibliografia