Literatura científica selecionada sobre o tema "Cognitive science"

Crie uma referência precisa em APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, e outros estilos

Selecione um tipo de fonte:

Consulte a lista de atuais artigos, livros, teses, anais de congressos e outras fontes científicas relevantes para o tema "Cognitive science".

Ao lado de cada fonte na lista de referências, há um botão "Adicionar à bibliografia". Clique e geraremos automaticamente a citação bibliográfica do trabalho escolhido no estilo de citação de que você precisa: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

Você também pode baixar o texto completo da publicação científica em formato .pdf e ler o resumo do trabalho online se estiver presente nos metadados.

Artigos de revistas sobre o assunto "Cognitive science"

1

Alberto Greco. "Cognitive science and cognitive sciences". Journal of Cognitive Science 13, n.º 4 (dezembro de 2012): 471–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.17791/jcs.2012.13.4.471.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

Favela, Luis H., e Jonathan Martin. "“Cognition” and Dynamical Cognitive Science". Minds and Machines 27, n.º 2 (7 de dezembro de 2016): 331–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11023-016-9411-4.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Aasim, Shahzad. "Cognitive Dimensions: Where Science Meets Art". International Journal of Science and Research (IJSR) 8, n.º 6 (5 de junho de 2019): 2422–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.21275/sr24221151213.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

Ross, Don. "Economics, cognitive science and social cognition". Cognitive Systems Research 9, n.º 1-2 (março de 2008): 125–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2007.06.010.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Sofronova, Lidia. "Historical Cognition and Cognitive Sciences: New in Russian Historiography". ISTORIYA 12, n.º 8 (106) (2021): 0. http://dx.doi.org/10.18254/s207987840016952-6.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
The article presents an analytical review of the recent literature on cognitive history, especially the Russian collective monograph “Cognitive Sciences and Historical Cognition”, published in 2020. It traces the patterns typical for interdisciplinary research not only within the humanitarian disciplines, but also at the “borders” between the humanities and the “natural sciences”. The article highlights the paradoxical and productive nature of the “mutual interventions” of cognitive science and the humanities, which contribute to overcoming “atomism” both within the humanities and at the “frontier” between them and the natural science disciplines.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

Boring, Ronald Laurids. "Cognitive science". XRDS: Crossroads, The ACM Magazine for Students 10, n.º 2 (dezembro de 2003): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/1027328.1027329.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Wimer Brakel, Linda A. "Cognitive Science". Psychoanalytic Quarterly 64, n.º 2 (abril de 1995): 417–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21674086.1995.11927458.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Dartnall, Terry, Steve Torrance, Mark Coulson, Stephen Nunn, Brendan Kitts, R. F. Port, T. van Gelder, Donald Peterson e Philip Gerrans. "Cognitive science". Metascience 5, n.º 1 (março de 1996): 95–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02988881.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Stern, Elsbeth. "Cognitive Psychology and Cognitive Science". Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 36, n.º 6 (junho de 1991): 485–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/029811.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

Leite, Diego Azevedo. "Neo-mechanistic explanatory integration for cognitive science: the problem of reduction remains". Sofia 8, n.º 1 (5 de setembro de 2019): 124–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.47456/sofia.v8i1.23198.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
One of the central aims of the neo-mechanistic framework for the neural and cognitive sciences is to construct a pluralistic integration of scientific explanations, allowing for a weak explanatory autonomy of higher-level sciences, such as cognitive science. This integration involves understanding human cognition as information processing occurring in multi-level human neuro-cognitive mechanisms, explained by multi-level neuro-cognitive models. Strong explanatory neuro-cognitive reduction, however, poses a significant challenge to this pluralist ambition and the weak autonomy of cognitive science derived therefrom. Based on research in current molecular and cellular neuroscience, the framework holds that the best strategy for integrating human neuro-cognitive theories is through direct reductive explanations based on molecular and cellular neural processes. It is my aim to investigate whether the neo-mechanistic framework can meet the challenge. I argue that leading neo-mechanists offer some significant replies; however, they are not able yet to completely remove strong explanatory reductionism from their own framework.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.

Teses / dissertações sobre o assunto "Cognitive science"

1

Schmitt, Wolfram. "The Cognitive View in Cognitive Science". Diss., lmu, 2006. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:19-76547.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

Greenlee, Christopher Alan. "Situated Cognition, Dynamicism, and Explanation in Cognitive Science". Thesis, Virginia Tech, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/46501.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
The majority of cognitive scientists today view the mind as a computer, instantiating some function mapping the inputs it gets from the environment to the gross behaviors of the organism. As a result, the emphasis in most ongoing research programmes is on finding that function, or some part of that function. Moreover, the types of functions considered are limited somewhat by the preconception that the mind must be instantiating a function that can be expressed as a computer program. I argue that research done in the last two decades suggests that we should approach cognition with as much consideration to the environment as to the inner workings of the mind. Our cognition is often shaped by the constraints the environment places on us, not just by the "inputs" we receive from it. I argue also that there is a new approach to cognitive science, viewing the mind not as a computer but as a dynamical system, which captures the shift in perspective while eliminating the requirement that cognitive functions be expressable as computer programs. Unfortunately, some advocates of this dynamical perspective have argued that we should replace all of traditional psychology and neuroscience with their new approach. In response to these advocates, I argue that we cannot develop an adequate dynamical picture of the mind without engaging in precisely those sorts of research and hypothesizing that traditional neuroscience and psychology engage in. In short, I argue that we require certain types of explanations in order to get our dynamical (or computational) theories off the ground, and we cannot get those from other dynamical (or computational) theories.
Master of Arts
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Shaik, Kadir Munirah. "Enlightening Science: Addressing the Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Aspects of Science Learning". Thesis, Australian Catholic University, 2018. https://acuresearchbank.acu.edu.au/download/62b15f077f8030a2d790b4b72bc33a91600baaf09ed4f6985e88b65dd6c99d62/6070465/SHAIK_KADIR_2018_Enlightening_science_addressing_the_cognitive_and.pdf.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Physical science (or physics) is known to be one of the least popular school curriculum domains, mainly because of its complexity. When students encounter seemingly insurmountable difficulties when learning something, they lose the motivation to continue. It has been suggested that both the cognitive (e.g., students’ conceptual understanding and achievement) and non-cognitive (e.g., psychological aspects such as academic self-concept and motivation) factors of learning are essential for helping students achieve their optimal best in a curriculum domain. However, there has not been much research, if any, which uses a dual approach to investigate both aspects of science learning. Most research focused on either the cognitive or non-cognitive aspect. Research on cognitive aspects of learning suggests that element interactivity is a useful construct with which to examine students’ cognitive processes and the complexity of learning materials. However, there has been no illustration on how an analysis of interacting elements in science learning tasks may improve learning. Studies on the effects of reducing element interactivity on students’ achievement and motivation are also scarce. Research on non-cognitive aspects of learning suggests that motivation is necessary to sustain students’ engagement in learning. However, if the complexity of learning tasks is so high that students experience repeated failures, their motivation is not sustained. Therefore, both cognitive and non-cognitive factors play a crucial role in students’ learning and both must be present to ensure an optimal learning environment. The overarching aim of this thesis is to investigate the cognitive (i.e., students’ achievement and cognitive processes in terms of element interactivity) and non-cognitive aspects (i.e., self-concept and other motivational factors) of students’ learning of science. The thesis includes five studies. The first study showed that the five main findings from past self-concept research were applicable to the Grade 7 students from Singapore selected for the study. Students’ sense of competence in a curriculum domain enhanced their future achievement in that domain only, except for physics and math, which showed interrelatedness (i.e., the enhancement was transferable from one domain to the other). The findings showed a strong interplay between academic self-concept and achievement and highlighted the important role that academic self-concept plays in determining students’ learning outcomes. Therefore, strategies to enhance students’ self-concept should be implemented in schools. The results of the second study showed strong positive correlations between students’ achievement and their motivation within a school year. Students’ Grade 6 (final primary school year) achievement did not strongly contribute to their motivation in Grade 7, indicating the importance of providing an optimal learning environment in Grade 7 for a positive start to their secondary school education. The third study showed how the interactions between the elements (i.e., element interactivity) in problem solving tasks reflect their level of complexity and how the number of operational lines that students used to solve problems could indicate their level of expertise in problem solving in that domain. This study highlighted the role of element interactivity as a planning tool for learning tasks and how teachers may use it to gain insights into students’ cognitive processes. The fourth study involved an intervention, which reduced element interactivity during science instruction, and the results revealed that students’ achievement improved, and their science self-concept was maintained. The results and implications of the first four studies were used to design a dual-approach instruction to facilitate both cognitive and non-cognitive aspects of students’ learning in the fifth and final study. The results of the final intervention study indicated that the dual-approach instruction was beneficial. The experimental group of students outperformed the comparison group in both cognitive and non-cognitive factors. Results from multiple regression analyses revealed that those who experienced the intervention not only had higher achievement than those in the comparison group in the complex problem tasks, but also had higher motivation (i.e., self-regulation, task goal, inquiry, and educational and career aspirations) and higher academic self-concept (i.e., sense of competence). This thesis demonstrates that there are strong associations and a significant interplay between students’ achievement and motivation levels (i.e., cognitive and non-cognitive aspects of learning). The analysis of learning tasks and instruction in terms of element interactivity enables the scaffolding of complex learning tasks to suit students’ cognitive levels, leading to higher achievement. Higher achievement contributes to higher motivation levels, including students’ academic self-concept. When learning environments attend to basic psychological needs (i.e., a sense of competence, autonomy, and relatedness), students’ motivation is enhanced and when motivated students experience learning that is within their ability and cognitive load capacities, their self-beliefs and motivation in the learning domain are sustained. Attention to both cognitive and non-cognitive factors in learning situations maximizes students’ learning potential and should therefore be strongly considered by educators and curriculum planners.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

Downes, Stephen Matthew. "Prospects for a cognitive science of science". Diss., This resource online, 1990. http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/theses/available/etd-08252008-162811/.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Ó, Maoldomhnaigh Micheál. "Cognitive stage, cognitive style, attitude and physical science option". Thesis, King's College London (University of London), 2004. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.406231.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

Akagi, Mikio Shaun Mikuriya. "Cognition in practice| Conceptual development and disagreement in cognitive science". Thesis, University of Pittsburgh, 2016. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10183682.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:

Cognitive science has been beset for thirty years by foundational disputes about the nature and extension of cognition—e.g. whether cognition is necessarily representational, whether cognitive processes extend outside the brain or body, and whether plants or microbes have them. Whereas previous philosophical work aimed to settle these disputes, I aim to understand what conception of cognition scientists could share given that they disagree so fundamentally. To this end, I develop a number of variations on traditional conceptual explication, and defend a novel explication of cognition called the sensitive management hypothesis.

Since expert judgments about the extension of “cognition” vary so much, I argue that there is value in explication that accurately models the variance in judgments rather than taking sides or treating that variance as noise. I say of explications that accomplish this that they are ecumenically extensionally adequate. Thus, rather than adjudicating whether, say, plants can have cognitive processes like humans, an ecumenically adequate explication should classify these cases differently: human cognitive processes as paradigmatically cognitive, and plant processes as controversially cognitive.

I achieve ecumenical adequacy by articulating conceptual explications with parameters, or terms that can be assigned a number of distinct interpretations based on the background commitments of participants in a discourse. For example, an explication might require that cognition cause “behavior,” and imply that plant processes are cognitive or not depending on whether anything plants do can be considered “behavior.” Parameterization provides a unified treatment of embattled concepts by isolating topics of disagreement in a small number of parameters.

I incorporate these innovations into an account on which cognition is the “sensitive management of organismal behavior.” The sensitive management hypothesis is ecumenically extensionally adequate, accurately classifying a broad variety of cases as paradigmatically or controversially cognitive phenomena. I also describe an extremely permissive version of the sensitive management hypothesis, arguing that it has the potential to explain several features of cognitive scientific discourse, including various facts about the way cognitive scientists ascribe representations to cognitive systems.

Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Svensson, Henrik. "Notions of Embodiment in Cognitive Science". Thesis, University of Skövde, Department of Computer Science, 2001. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:his:diva-588.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:

Cognitive science has traditionally viewed the mind as essentially disembodied, that is, the nature of mind and cognition is neither affected by the ¡Èsystem¡É it is implemented in nor affected by the environment that the system is situated in. But since the mid-1980s a new approach emerged in artificial intelligence that emphasized the importance of embodiment and situatedness and since then terms like embodied cognition, embodied intelligence have become more and more apparent in discussions of cognition. As embodied cognition has increased in interest so have the notions of embodiment and situatedness and they are not always compatible. This report has found that there are, at least, four notions of embodiment in the discussions of embodied cognition: software embodiment, physical embodiment, biological embodiment and human(oid) embodiment.

Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Chada, Daniel de Magalhães. "From cognitive science to management science: two computational contributions". reponame:Repositório Institucional do FGV, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10438/17053.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Submitted by Kelly Ayala (kelly.ayala@fgv.br) on 2016-09-12T12:57:06Z No. of bitstreams: 1 Chada 2011 FINAL ENTREGUE.pdf: 579283 bytes, checksum: f463590c20f51b84ba0f9357ab1a6e08 (MD5)
Approved for entry into archive by Kelly Ayala (kelly.ayala@fgv.br) on 2016-09-12T12:58:17Z (GMT) No. of bitstreams: 1 Chada 2011 FINAL ENTREGUE.pdf: 579283 bytes, checksum: f463590c20f51b84ba0f9357ab1a6e08 (MD5)
Approved for entry into archive by Kelly Ayala (kelly.ayala@fgv.br) on 2016-09-12T13:00:07Z (GMT) No. of bitstreams: 1 Chada 2011 FINAL ENTREGUE.pdf: 579283 bytes, checksum: f463590c20f51b84ba0f9357ab1a6e08 (MD5)
Made available in DSpace on 2016-09-12T13:03:31Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 Chada 2011 FINAL ENTREGUE.pdf: 579283 bytes, checksum: f463590c20f51b84ba0f9357ab1a6e08 (MD5) Previous issue date: 2011
This work is composed of two contributions. One borrows from the work of Charles Kemp and Joshua Tenenbaum, concerning the discovery of structural form: their model is used to study the Business Week Rankings of U.S. Business Schools, and to investigate how other structural forms (structured visualizations) of the same information used to generate the rankings can bring insights into the space of business schools in the U.S., and into rankings in general. The other essay is purely theoretical in nature. It is a study to develop a model of human memory that does not exceed our (human) psychological short-term memory limitations. This study is based on Pentti Kanerva’s Sparse Distributed Memory, in which human memories are registered into a vast (but virtual) memory space, and this registration occurs in massively parallel and distributed fashion, in ideal neurons.
Este trabalho é composto de duas contribuições. Uma se usa do trabalhode Charles Kemp e Joshua Tenenbaum sobre a descoberta da forma estrutural: o seu modelo é usado para estudar os rankings da revista Business Week sobre escolas de administração, e para investigar como outras formas estruturais (visualizações estruturadas) da mesma informação usada para gerar os rankings pode trazer discernimento no espaço de escolas de negócios nos Estados Unidos e em rankings em geral. O outro ensaio é de natureza puramente teórica. Ele é um estudo no desenvolvimento de um modelo de memória que não excede os nossos (humanos) limites de memória de curto-prazo. Este estudo se baseia na Sparse Distributed Memory (Memória Esparsa e Distribuida) de Pentti Kanerva, na qual memórias humanas são registradas em um vasto (mas virtual) espaço, e este registro ocorre de forma maciçamente paralela e distribuida, em neurons ideais.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Vervaeke, John Alexander. "The naturalistic imperative in cognitive science". Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1997. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/ftp03/NQ28308.pdf.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

Ladbury, Martin Samuel Durham. "The idea of a cognitive science". Thesis, University of Southampton, 2000. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.342914.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.

Livros sobre o assunto "Cognitive science"

1

N, Osherson Daniel, e Gleitman Lila R, eds. An invitation to cognitive science. 2a ed. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 1995.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

R, Gleitman Lila, e NetLibrary Inc, eds. An invitation to cognitive science: Thinking. 2a ed. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 1995.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

I, Posner Michael, ed. Foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 1989.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

A, Stillings Neil, ed. Cognitive science: An introduction. 2a ed. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 1995.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Barsalou, Lawrence W. Cognitive psychology: An overview for cognitive scientists. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1992.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

1954-, Lambalgen Michiel van, ed. Human reasoning and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2008.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Emmanuel, Dupoux, e Mehler Jacques, eds. Language, brain, and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press, 2001.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Lamberts, Koen. Cognitive Science. 1 Oliver's Yard, 55 City Road, London EC1Y 1SP United Kingdom: SAGE Publications Ltd, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781446261040.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Noel, Sheehy, e Chapman Antony J, eds. Cognitive science. Aldershot: Edward Elgar, 1995.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

1955-, Sheehy Noel, e Chapman Antony J, eds. Cognitive science. New York: New York University Press, 1995.

Encontre o texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.

Capítulos de livros sobre o assunto "Cognitive science"

1

Wiggins, Osborne P., e Manfred Spitzer. "Cognitive Science". In Contributions to Phenomenology, 101–4. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-5344-9_22.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

Ream, Derek, e Isaac Tourgeman. "Cognitive Science". In Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science, 1–7. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_477-1.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Mazzola, Guerino, René Guitart, Jocelyn Ho, Alex Lubet, Maria Mannone, Matt Rahaim e Florian Thalmann. "Cognitive Science". In The Topos of Music III: Gestures, 867–87. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-64481-3_4.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

Grush, Rick. "Cognitive Science". In The Blackwell Guide to the Philosophy of Science, 272–89. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers Ltd, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470756614.ch13.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Coulson, Seana, e Teenie Matlock. "Cognitive science". In Handbook of Pragmatics, 1–30. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/hop.9.cog5.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

Coulson, Seana. "Cognitive science". In Handbook of Pragmatics, 123–40. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1995. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/hop.m.cog5.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Coulson, Seana, e Teenie Matlock. "Cognitive science". In Cognition and Pragmatics, 86–109. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/hoph.3.06cou.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Coulson, Seana, e Teenie Matlock. "Cognitive science". In Handbook of Pragmatics, 195–216. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/hop.m2.cog5.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Peterson, Gregory. "Cognitive Science". In Encyclopedia of Sciences and Religions, 408. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-8265-8_200174.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

Pluck, Graham, e Kris Ariyabuddhiphongs. "Clinical Cognitive Sciences". In Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 130–48. Cham: Springer Nature Switzerland, 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-66021-4_9.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
AbstractClinical sciences involved with the mind and brain, including neurology, psychiatry, endocrinology and clinical psychology all frequently deal with cognitive symptoms, side effects, and risk factors. Consequently, there has long been some interaction between those clinical fields and traditional cognitive sciences, focused on computationalist and embodied approaches to understanding natural and machine cognition. Examples include the advances made in understanding the normal cognitive architecture made by studying its breakdown in disease, as well as the enhanced methods of defining and measuring cognitive disorders stemming from understanding the healthy state. Nevertheless, the fields currently fail to fully exploit the potential for mutual advancement. Here we explore the interactions between traditional clinical and cognitive sciences and highlighted strengths of the relationship, and areas that could benefit from greater multidisciplinary emphasis. We argue that original fields of cognitive science (philosophy, linguistics, computer science, anthropology, psychology and neuroscience) remain the core of the multidisciplinary cognitive sciences, but that they can all be applied fruitfully to clinical issues. We explore this in one sample disorder—voice hearing in schizophrenia, showing the potential for clinically applied cognitive sciences. It is our contention that greater achievement is possible, in both academic and applied fields dealing with cognition, if we can foster a mutually symbiotic relationship between the clinical and cognitive sciences.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.

Trabalhos de conferências sobre o assunto "Cognitive science"

1

Murphy, Dominic. "Cognitive Science Without Cognitive Psychology". In 9th Conference of the Australasian Society for Cognitive Science. Sydney: Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.5096/ascs200938.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

Adams, Ray. "Cognitive science meets computing science: The future of cognitive systems and cognitive engineering". In Proceedings of the ITI 2009 31st International Conference on Information Technology Interfaces (ITI). IEEE, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iti.2009.5196041.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Flanagan, Brian J. "Supersymmetry For Cognitive Science". In 1988 Robotics Conferences, editado por David P. Casasent. SPIE, 1989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/12.960287.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

Mukherjee, Sohini, Ashish Kumar Gupta, Sarif Aziz, Tariq Aziz, Pratik Jaiswal e Souvik Chatterjee. "Cognitive science: A review". In 2017 4th International Conference on Opto-Electronics and Applied Optics (Optronix). IEEE, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/optronix.2017.8349979.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Wang, Yingxu. "From information revolution to intelligence revolution: Big data science vs. intelligence science". In 2014 IEEE 13th International Conference on Cognitive Informatics & Cognitive Computing (ICCI*CC). IEEE, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icci-cc.2014.6921432.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

Cēdere, Dagnija, Inese Jurgena, Ineta Helmane, Inta Tiltiņa e Gunita Praulīte. "COGNITIVE INTEREST: PROBLEMS AND SOLUTIONS IN THE ACQUISITION OF SCIENCE AND MATHEMATICS IN SCHOOLS OF LATVIA". In 1st International Baltic Symposium on Science and Technology Education. Scientia Socialis Ltd., 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.33225/balticste/2015.33.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
The low level of pupils’ knowledge and skills in science and mathematics is a serious problem in the economic development of the country. Cognitive interest is a crucial learning motive; no successful learning process is possible without inciting interest. Grade 9 pupils were surveyed to find out the respondents’ cognitive interest in the field of exact sciences. On average the interest in subjects of exact sciences is poorly pronounced. Respondents have good understanding about the causal relations while the cognitive activity, the skill to overcome difficulties in learning is low. Cognitive interest in learning is promoted by diverse methodological approaches that are oriented towards pupils’ self-actualization and purposefulness. Key words: cognitive interest, learning process, science and mathematics.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Suwara, Marek, e Jan Werszowiec Plazowski. "Is cognitive science a science at all?" In 2012 IEEE 3rd International Conference on Cognitive Infocommunications (CogInfoCom). IEEE, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/coginfocom.2012.6421948.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Fiorini, Rodolfo A. "Human-Centered Symbiotic System Science". In 2019 IEEE 18th International Conference on Cognitive Informatics & Cognitive Computing (ICCI*CC). IEEE, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iccicc46617.2019.9146034.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Bradshaw, Jeffrey M., e J. Chris Forsythe. "Cognitive science and socio-cognitive theoryfor the HRI practitioner". In the seventh annual ACM/IEEE international conference. New York, New York, USA: ACM Press, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2157689.2157847.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

Crowston, Kevin, Thomas W. Malone e Felix Lin. "Cognitive science and organizational design". In the 1986 ACM conference. New York, New York, USA: ACM Press, 1986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/637069.637076.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.

Relatórios de organizações sobre o assunto "Cognitive science"

1

Forbus, Kenneth D., Kate Lockwood, Emmett Tomai, Morteza Dehghani e Jakub Czyz. Machine Reading as a Cognitive Science Research Instrument. Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, janeiro de 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada470412.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
2

McGee, Steven, Amanda Durik e Jess Zimmerman. The Impact of Text Genre on Science Learning in an Authentic Science Learning Environment. The Learning Partnership, abril de 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.51420/conf.2015.2.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
A gap exists between research on learning and research on interest. Cognitive researchers rarely consider motivational processes, and interest researchers rarely consider cognitive process. However, it is essential to consider both since achievement and interest are in fact intertwined. In this paper we (1) discuss a theoretical model that intertwines cognitive and interest development, (2) describe how that model informed the development of educational materials, and (3) report on the results of the cognitive components of a randomized research study examining the impact of text genre on learning and interest. In our prior analyses, we examined the effects of text characteristics (i.e., narrative or expository genre) on situational interest. We found that students with higher levels of prior individual interest preferred the narrative versions of text whereas students with lower levels of prior individual interest preferred the expository versions of text. In this paper, we examine the impact of text characteristics on student learning. The results of this research showed that contrary to prior research, there was no significant difference in comprehension based on text characteristics. These results provide evidence that is possible to differentiate instruction based students' prior interest without sacrificing learning outcomes.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
3

Sanchez, Christopher A. Support for the Annual Meeting (30th) of the Cognitive Science Society. Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, outubro de 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada488149.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
4

Moore, Jr, e L. R. Cognitive Model Exploration and Optimization: A New Challenge for Computational Science. Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, janeiro de 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada539438.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
5

Lu, Zhong-Lin. Workshop on Cognitive Science from Cellular Mechanisms to Computational Theories (CS-2009). Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, maio de 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada533451.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
6

López Bóo, Florencia, e Nicolás Ajzenman. 10 Lessons About Behavioral Economics for Policy Making in the Social Sector. Inter-American Development Bank, setembro de 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.18235/0008045.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Behavioral Economics is the science of evaluating psychological, cognitive, emotional, cultural and social factors and their impact on economic decisions. Enhancing our knowledge on Behavioral Sciences and their impact on public policies is a priority. The present document explores this intersection and offers 10 lessons for policy making in the social sector.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
7

Gray, Wayne. Request for AFOSR Support of the 24th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci2002). Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, janeiro de 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada420734.

Texto completo da fonte
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
8

Pinchuk, O. P., V. A. Tkachenko e O. Yu Burov. AV and VR as Gamification of Cognitive Tasks. CEUR Workshop Proceedings, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.33407/lib.naes.718697.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
The paper presents a comparative analysis of the functionality of mobile applications of the augmented reality Da Vinci Machines AR, Electricity AR, Bridges AR, Geometry, the collection of VR models VictoryVR Science Curriculum and the digital collection Mozaik. The possibility of using these tools for educational purposes is explored, in particular, to construct cognitive tasks for students during the study of subjects in the natural and mathematical cycle. The indicated shortcomings are stated, didactic requirements for such educational activities are formulated. Among others, attention is focused on the following indicators: hardware, usability, variability of model parameters, interactivity, interdisciplinary use, and the ability to activate certain cognitive actions of students, degree/form of gamification. The educational potential of using interactive models and video is analyzed for both group and individual work with students. Examples of methodical developments are given.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
9

Rollo, Greta, e Kellie Picker. Unpacking the science of reading research. Australian Council for Educational Research, junho de 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.37517/978-1-74286-742-7.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
The science of reading (SoR) is a term used for a body of evidence encompassing multi-disciplinary research from education, cognitive psychology, linguistics, and neuroscience. This evidence points to six key constructs that contribute to proficient reading: oral language, phonological awareness including phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary and reading comprehension. Research around these constructs provides researchers and teachers with an evidence base of the knowledge, skills and strategies involved in competent reading and describes how reading develops in both typical and atypical readers. This paper synthesises evidence reviews conducted by ACER researchers that unpack the science of reading. The aim of this synthesis is to demonstrate the impact that research in reading development is having on current ACER research and products. Most importantly, it supports understanding of the importance of embracing the complexity and nuance of reading research and the need for improved efforts to clearly communicate evolving research evidence. ACER draws on the evolving evidence of the science of reading to inform its approach to developing assessments and resources for teachers, and also refers to this evidence to describe where children are in their reading journey. This means a students' progress through each construct as described in this paper can be tracked and used to inform teaching and learning.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
10

LI, Na, Xia AI, Xinrong Guo, Juan Liu, Rongchao Zhang e Ruihui Wang. Effect of acupuncture treatment on cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury in adults: A systematic review protocol. INPLASY - International Platform of Registered Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Protocols, novembro de 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.37766/inplasy2021.11.0113.

Texto completo da fonte
Resumo:
Review question / Objective: Are acupuncture more effective than control interventions (i.e. treatment as sham acupuncture or placebo) in the treatment of motor and cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury in adults? Information sources: search database:The following electronic databases will be searched for relevant literature: the Cochrane Library, MEDLINE, EMBASE, Web of Science, Springer, the Chinese Science Citation Database (CSCD), China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI), the Chinese Biomedical Literature Database (CBM),Wanfang, and. the Chinese Scientific Journal Database (VIP). Time limit: the searches will be conducted from the inception of each database to November 30, 2021. Protocol of Systematic review and Meta analysis of acupuncture in the treatment of cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury and the included literatures were all RCTS with English and Chinese on language.
Estilos ABNT, Harvard, Vancouver, APA, etc.
Oferecemos descontos em todos os planos premium para autores cujas obras estão incluídas em seleções literárias temáticas. Contate-nos para obter um código promocional único!

Vá para a bibliografia