Kliknij ten link, aby zobaczyć inne rodzaje publikacji na ten temat: Primary Visual Cortex (area V1).

Artykuły w czasopismach na temat „Primary Visual Cortex (area V1)”

Utwórz poprawne odniesienie w stylach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard i wielu innych

Wybierz rodzaj źródła:

Sprawdź 50 najlepszych artykułów w czasopismach naukowych na temat „Primary Visual Cortex (area V1)”.

Przycisk „Dodaj do bibliografii” jest dostępny obok każdej pracy w bibliografii. Użyj go – a my automatycznie utworzymy odniesienie bibliograficzne do wybranej pracy w stylu cytowania, którego potrzebujesz: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver itp.

Możesz również pobrać pełny tekst publikacji naukowej w formacie „.pdf” i przeczytać adnotację do pracy online, jeśli odpowiednie parametry są dostępne w metadanych.

Przeglądaj artykuły w czasopismach z różnych dziedzin i twórz odpowiednie bibliografie.

1

Beltramo, Riccardo, i Massimo Scanziani. "A collicular visual cortex: Neocortical space for an ancient midbrain visual structure". Science 363, nr 6422 (3.01.2019): 64–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.aau7052.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Visual responses in the cerebral cortex are believed to rely on the geniculate input to the primary visual cortex (V1). Indeed, V1 lesions substantially reduce visual responses throughout the cortex. Visual information enters the cortex also through the superior colliculus (SC), but the function of this input on visual responses in the cortex is less clear. SC lesions affect cortical visual responses less than V1 lesions, and no visual cortical area appears to entirely rely on SC inputs. We show that visual responses in a mouse lateral visual cortical area called the postrhinal cortex are independent of V1 and are abolished upon silencing of the SC. This area outperforms V1 in discriminating moving objects. We thus identify a collicular primary visual cortex that is independent of the geniculo-cortical pathway and is capable of motion discrimination.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
2

Watanabe, Takeo, Yuka Sasaki, Satoru Miyauchi, Benno Putz, Norio Fujimaki, Matthew Nielsen, Ryosuke Takino i Satoshi Miyakawa. "Attention-Regulated Activity in Human Primary Visual Cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 79, nr 4 (1.04.1998): 2218–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.79.4.2218.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Watanabe, Takeo, Yuka Sasaki, Satoru Miyauchi, Benno Putz, Norio Fujimaki, Matthew Nielsen, Ryosuke Takino, and Satoshi Miyakawa. Attention-regulated activity in human primary visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 79: 2218–2221, 1998. Effects of attention to a local contour of a moving object on the activation of human primary visual cortex (area V1) were examined. Local cerebral oxygenation changes (an index of neuronal activity) in human area V1 were measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in conditions including the following two: 1) when attention was selectively directed toward one side of a moving wedge (the attention condition) and 2) when the wedges were viewed passively (the passive condition). Activation in area V1 was found to be higher in the attention condition than in the passive condition. To our knowledge, this is the first finding that attention to motion activates as early as area V1. We suggest that attentional activation of area V1 is task dependent.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
3

Froudarakis, Emmanouil, Paul G. Fahey, Jacob Reimer, Stelios M. Smirnakis, Edward J. Tehovnik i Andreas S. Tolias. "The Visual Cortex in Context". Annual Review of Vision Science 5, nr 1 (15.09.2019): 317–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034407.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
In this article, we review the anatomical inputs and outputs to the mouse primary visual cortex, area V1. Our survey of data from the Allen Institute Mouse Connectivity project indicates that mouse V1 is highly interconnected with both cortical and subcortical brain areas. This pattern of innervation allows for computations that depend on the state of the animal and on behavioral goals, which contrasts with simple feedforward, hierarchical models of visual processing. Thus, to have an accurate description of the function of V1 during mouse behavior, its involvement with the rest of the brain circuitry has to be considered. Finally, it remains an open question whether the primary visual cortex of higher mammals displays the same degree of sensorimotor integration in the early visual system.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
4

White, Brian J., Janis Y. Kan, Ron Levy, Laurent Itti i Douglas P. Munoz. "Superior colliculus encodes visual saliency before the primary visual cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, nr 35 (14.08.2017): 9451–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1701003114.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Models of visual attention postulate the existence of a bottom-up saliency map that is formed early in the visual processing stream. Although studies have reported evidence of a saliency map in various cortical brain areas, determining the contribution of phylogenetically older pathways is crucial to understanding its origin. Here, we compared saliency coding from neurons in two early gateways into the visual system: the primary visual cortex (V1) and the evolutionarily older superior colliculus (SC). We found that, while the response latency to visual stimulus onset was earlier for V1 neurons than superior colliculus superficial visual-layer neurons (SCs), the saliency representation emerged earlier in SCs than in V1. Because the dominant input to the SCs arises from V1, these relative timings are consistent with the hypothesis that SCs neurons pool the inputs from multiple V1 neurons to form a feature-agnostic saliency map, which may then be relayed to other brain areas.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
5

Seydell-Greenwald, Anna, Xiaoying Wang, Elissa L. Newport, Yanchao Bi i Ella Striem-Amit. "Spoken language processing activates the primary visual cortex". PLOS ONE 18, nr 8 (11.08.2023): e0289671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289671.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Primary visual cortex (V1) is generally thought of as a low-level sensory area that primarily processes basic visual features. Although there is evidence for multisensory effects on its activity, these are typically found for the processing of simple sounds and their properties, for example spatially or temporally-congruent simple sounds. However, in congenitally blind individuals, V1 is involved in language processing, with no evidence of major changes in anatomical connectivity that could explain this seemingly drastic functional change. This is at odds with current accounts of neural plasticity, which emphasize the role of connectivity and conserved function in determining a neural tissue’s role even after atypical early experiences. To reconcile what appears to be unprecedented functional reorganization with known accounts of plasticity limitations, we tested whether V1’s multisensory roles include responses to spoken language in sighted individuals. Using fMRI, we found that V1 in normally sighted individuals was indeed activated by comprehensible spoken sentences as compared to an incomprehensible reversed speech control condition, and more strongly so in the left compared to the right hemisphere. Activation in V1 for language was also significant and comparable for abstract and concrete words, suggesting it was not driven by visual imagery. Last, this activation did not stem from increased attention to the auditory onset of words, nor was it correlated with attentional arousal ratings, making general attention accounts an unlikely explanation. Together these findings suggest that V1 responds to spoken language even in sighted individuals, reflecting the binding of multisensory high-level signals, potentially to predict visual input. This capability might be the basis for the strong V1 language activation observed in people born blind, re-affirming the notion that plasticity is guided by pre-existing connectivity and abilities in the typically developed brain.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
6

Pereira, Catia M., Marco Aurelio M. Freire, José R. Santos, Joanilson S. Guimarães, Gabriella Dias-Florencio, Sharlene Santos, Antonio Pereira i Sidarta Ribeiro. "Non-visual exploration of novel objects increases the levels of plasticity factors in the rat primary visual cortex". PeerJ 6 (23.10.2018): e5678. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.5678.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Background Historically, the primary sensory areas of the cerebral cortex have been exclusively associated with the processing of a single sensory modality. Yet the presence of tactile responses in the primary visual (V1) cortex has challenged this view, leading to the notion that primary sensory areas engage in cross-modal processing, and that the associated circuitry is modifiable by such activity. To explore this notion, here we assessed whether the exploration of novel objects in the dark induces the activation of plasticity markers in the V1 cortex of rats. Methods Adult rats were allowed to freely explore for 20 min a completely dark box with four novel objects of different shapes and textures. Animals were euthanized either 1 (n = 5) or 3 h (n = 5) after exploration. A control group (n = 5) was placed for 20 min in the same environment, but without the objects. Frontal sections of the brains were submitted to immunohistochemistry to measure protein levels of egr-1 and c-fos, and phosphorylated calcium-dependent kinase (pCaKMII) in V1 cortex. Results The amount of neurons labeled with monoclonal antibodies against c-fos, egr-1 or pCaKMII increased significantly in V1 cortex after one hour of exploration in the dark. Three hours after exploration, the number of labeled neurons decreased to basal levels. Conclusions Our results suggest that non-visual exploration induces the activation of immediate-early genes in V1 cortex, which is suggestive of cross-modal processing in this area. Besides, the increase in the number of neurons labeled with pCaKMII may signal a condition promoting synaptic plasticity.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
7

Shi, Li, Qi Ming Ye i Xiao Ke Niu. "Orientation Coding by Population of Neurons in Rats' Primary Visual Cortex". Applied Mechanics and Materials 427-429 (wrzesień 2013): 2089–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amm.427-429.2089.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Research on Primate visual cortex (V1 area) neurons orientation coding mechanism is the base of revealing the whole visual cortex information processing mechanism. Firstly, this paper adopted different orientation grating to stimulate visually on rats. Meanwhile, gather response signals of population neurons from V1 area using multi-electrode arrays. Then, screen effective response channels according to the orientation selection of different neurons in different channels. Besides, extract Spike average fire rate and LFPγ band power feature in every effective channel signals within specific stimulus response time to construct population response joint features. Finally, taking Lasso regression model as coding model, use joint features to differentiate grating orientation, in order to research on V1 areas population neurons orientation coding. The consequences indicate that the results of population response joint features coding for six different orientation are superior to the results of any single feature of population response coding, and remarkably better than the results of single channel response feature coding.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
8

Andelin, Adrian K., Jaime F. Olavarria, Ione Fine, Erin N. Taber, Daniel Schwartz, Christopher D. Kroenke i Alexander A. Stevens. "The Effect of Onset Age of Visual Deprivation on Visual Cortex Surface Area Across-Species". Cerebral Cortex 29, nr 10 (18.12.2018): 4321–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy315.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Abstract Blindness early in life induces permanent alterations in brain anatomy, including reduced surface area of primary visual cortex (V1). Bilateral enucleation early in development causes greater reductions in primary visual cortex surface area than at later times. However, the time at which cortical surface area expansion is no longer sensitive to enucleation is not clearly established, despite being an important milestone for cortical development. Using histological and MRI techniques, we investigated how reductions in the surface area of V1 depends on the timing of blindness onset in rats, ferrets and humans. To compare data across species, we translated ages of all species to a common neuro-developmental event-time (ET) scale. Consistently, blindness during early cortical expansion induced large (~40%) reductions in V1 surface area, in rats and ferrets, while blindness occurring later had diminishing effects. Longitudinal measurements on ferrets confirmed that early enucleation disrupted cortical expansion, rather than inducing enhanced pruning. We modeled the ET associated with the conclusion of the effect of blindness on surface area at maturity (ETc), relative to the normal conclusion of visual cortex surface area expansion, (ETdev). A final analysis combining our data with extant published data confirmed that ETc occurred well before ETdev.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
9

Dagnino, Bruno, Marie-Alice Gariel-Mathis i Pieter R. Roelfsema. "Microstimulation of area V4 has little effect on spatial attention and on perception of phosphenes evoked in area V1". Journal of Neurophysiology 113, nr 3 (1.02.2015): 730–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00645.2014.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Previous transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies suggested that feedback from higher to lower areas of the visual cortex is important for the access of visual information to awareness. However, the influence of cortico-cortical feedback on awareness and the nature of the feedback effects are not yet completely understood. In the present study, we used electrical microstimulation in the visual cortex of monkeys to test the hypothesis that cortico-cortical feedback plays a role in visual awareness. We investigated the interactions between the primary visual cortex (V1) and area V4 by applying microstimulation in both cortical areas at various delays. We report that the monkeys detected the phosphenes produced by V1 microstimulation but subthreshold V4 microstimulation did not influence V1 phosphene detection thresholds. A second experiment examined the influence of V4 microstimulation on the monkeys' ability to detect the dimming of one of three peripheral visual stimuli. Again, microstimulation of a group of V4 neurons failed to modulate the monkeys' perception of a stimulus in their receptive field. We conclude that conditions exist where microstimulation of area V4 has only a limited influence on visual perception.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
10

Jiang, Fei, Jian-Wen Fang, Yin-Quan Ye, Yan-Jin Tian, Xian-Jun Zeng i Yu-Lin Zhong. "Altered effective connectivity of primary visual cortex in primary angle closure glaucoma using Granger causality analysis". Acta Radiologica 61, nr 4 (7.08.2019): 508–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0284185119867644.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Background Previous neuroimaging studies demonstrated that primary angle closure glaucoma patients were associated with abnormal intrinsic brain activity in primary visual cortex (V1). Purpose The purpose of this study was to investigate the effective connectivity patterns of V1 in patients with primary angle closure glaucoma. Material and Methods Thirty-seven patients with primary angle closure glaucoma (20 men, 17 women) and 36 healthy controls (20 men, 16 women) closely matched for age, sex, and education, underwent resting-state MRI scans. A voxel-wise Granger causality analysis method was performed to explore different effective connectivity pattern of V1 between the two groups. Results Compared with healthy controls, patients with primary angle closure glaucoma showed decreased effective connectivity from the left V1 to left cuneus and increased effective connectivity from the left V1 to left precentral gyrus and right supplementary motor area. Meanwhile, patients with primary angle closure glaucoma showed decreased effective connectivity from left precentral gyrus to left V1 and right frontal middle gyrus to left V1. In addition, patients with primary angle closure glaucoma showed a decreased effective connectivity from the right V1 to left cuneus/calcarine and increased effective connectivity from the right V1 to left inferior frontal gyrus and right caudate. Meanwhile, patients with primary angle closure glaucoma showed decreased effective connectivity from right middle frontal gyrus/precentral gyrus to right V1 and left precentral gyrus to right V1. Conclusion Our results highlighted that patients with primary angle closure glaucoma had abnormal effective connectivity between V1 and higher visual area, motor cortices, somatosensory cortices, and frontal lobe, which indicated that they might present with abnormal top-down modulations, visual imagery, vision-motor function, and vision-related higher cognition function.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
11

Johnson, Elizabeth N., Michael J. Hawken i Robert Shapley. "Cone Inputs in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 91, nr 6 (czerwiec 2004): 2501–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01043.2003.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
To understand the role of primary visual cortex (V1) in color vision, we measured directly the input from the 3 cone types in macaque V1 neurons. Cells were classified as luminance-preferring, color-luminance, or color-preferring from the ratio of the peak amplitudes of spatial frequency responses to red/green equiluminant and to black/white (luminance) grating patterns, respectively. In this study we used L-, M-, and S-cone–isolating gratings to measure spatial frequency response functions for each cone type separately. From peak responses to cone-isolating stimuli we estimated relative cone weights and whether cone inputs were the same or opposite sign. For most V1 cells the relative S-cone weight was <0.1. All color-preferring cells were cone opponent and their L/M cone weight ratio was clustered around a value of –1, which is roughly equal and opposite L and M cone signals. Almost all cells (88%) classified as luminance cells were cone nonopponent, with a broad distribution of cone weights. Most cells (73%) classified as color-luminance cells were cone opponent. This result supports our conclusion that V1 color-luminance cells are double-opponent. Such neurons are more sensitive to color boundaries than to areas of color and thereby could play an important role in color perception. The color-luminance population had a broad distribution of L/M cone weight ratios, implying a broad distribution of preferred colors for the double-opponent cells.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
12

Kaas, Jon H., i Mary K. L. Baldwin. "The Evolution of the Pulvinar Complex in Primates and Its Role in the Dorsal and Ventral Streams of Cortical Processing". Vision 4, nr 1 (30.12.2019): 3. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/vision4010003.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Current evidence supports the view that the visual pulvinar of primates consists of at least five nuclei, with two large nuclei, lateral pulvinar ventrolateral (PLvl) and central lateral nucleus of the inferior pulvinar (PIcl), contributing mainly to the ventral stream of cortical processing for perception, and three smaller nuclei, posterior nucleus of the inferior pulvinar (PIp), medial nucleus of the inferior pulvinar (PIm), and central medial nucleus of the inferior pulvinar (PIcm), projecting to dorsal stream visual areas for visually directed actions. In primates, both cortical streams are highly dependent on visual information distributed from primary visual cortex (V1). This area is so vital to vision that patients with V1 lesions are considered “cortically blind”. When the V1 inputs to dorsal stream area middle temporal visual area (MT) are absent, other dorsal stream areas receive visual information relayed from the superior colliculus via PIp and PIcm, thereby preserving some dorsal stream functions, a phenomenon called “blind sight”. Non-primate mammals do not have a dorsal stream area MT with V1 inputs, but superior colliculus inputs to temporal cortex can be more significant and more visual functions are preserved when V1 input is disrupted. The current review will discuss how the different visual streams, especially the dorsal stream, have changed during primate evolution and we propose which features are retained from the common ancestor of primates and their close relatives.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
13

Resca, Lorenzo G., i Nicholas A. Mecholsky. "Geometry and Geodesy on the Primary Visual Cortex as a Surface of Revolution". Mathematical and Computational Applications 25, nr 4 (29.09.2020): 64. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/mca25040064.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Biological mapping of the visual field from the eye retina to the primary visual cortex, also known as occipital area V1, is central to vision and eye movement phenomena and research. That mapping is critically dependent on the existence of cortical magnification factors. Once unfolded, V1 has a convex three-dimensional shape, which can be mathematically modeled as a surface of revolution embedded in three-dimensional Euclidean space. Thus, we solve the problem of differential geometry and geodesy for the mapping of the visual field to V1, involving both isotropic and non-isotropic cortical magnification factors of a most general form. We provide illustrations of our technique and results that apply to V1 surfaces with curve profiles relevant to vision research in general and to visual phenomena such as ‘crowding’ effects and eye movement guidance in particular. From a mathematical perspective, we also find intriguing and unexpected differential geometry properties of V1 surfaces, discovering that geodesic orbits have alternative prograde and retrograde characteristics, depending on the interplay between local curvature and global topology.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
14

van Ommen, Marouska M., Teus van Laar, Remco Renken, Frans W. Cornelissen i Richard Bruggeman. "Visual Hallucinations in Psychosis: The Curious Absence of the Primary Visual Cortex". Schizophrenia Bulletin 49, Supplement_1 (24.02.2023): S68—S81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac140.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Abstract Background and Hypothesis Approximately one-third of patients with a psychotic disorder experience visual hallucinations (VH). While new, more targeted treatment options are warranted, the pathophysiology of VH remains largely unknown. Previous studies hypothesized that VH result from impaired functioning of the vision-related networks and impaired interaction between those networks, including a possible functional disconnection between the primary visual cortex (V1) and higher-order visual processing regions. Testing these hypotheses requires sufficient data on brain activation during actual VH, but such data are extremely scarce. Study Design We therefore recruited seven participants with a psychotic disorder who were scanned in a 3 T fMRI scanner while indicating the occurrence of VH by pressing a button. Following the scan session, we interviewed participants about the VH experienced during scanning. We then used the fMRI scans to identify regions with increased or decreased activity during VH periods versus baseline (no VH). Study Results In six participants, V1 was not activated during VH, and in one participant V1 showed decreased activation. All participants reported complex VH such as human-like beings, objects and/or animals, during which higher-order visual areas and regions belonging to the vision-related networks on attention and memory were activated. Discussion These results indicate that VH are associated with diffuse involvement of the vision-related networks, with the exception of V1. We therefore propose a model for the pathophysiology of psychotic VH in which a dissociation of higher-order visual processing areas from V1 biases conscious perception away from reality and towards internally generated percepts.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
15

Liang, Hualou, Xiajing Gong, Minggui Chen, Yin Yan, Wu Li i Charles D. Gilbert. "Interactions between feedback and lateral connections in the primary visual cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, nr 32 (24.07.2017): 8637–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1706183114.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Perceptual grouping of line segments into object contours has been thought to be mediated, in part, by long-range horizontal connectivity intrinsic to the primary visual cortex (V1), with a contribution by top-down feedback projections. To dissect the contributions of intraareal and interareal connections during contour integration, we applied conditional Granger causality analysis to assess directional influences among neural signals simultaneously recorded from visual cortical areas V1 and V4 of monkeys performing a contour detection task. Our results showed that discounting the influences from V4 markedly reduced V1 lateral interactions, indicating dependence on feedback signals of the effective connectivity within V1. On the other hand, the feedback influences were reciprocally dependent on V1 lateral interactions because the modulation strengths from V4 to V1 were greatly reduced after discounting the influences from other V1 neurons. Our findings suggest that feedback and lateral connections closely interact to mediate image grouping and segmentation.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
16

Petro, L. S., A. T. Paton i L. Muckli. "Contextual modulation of primary visual cortex by auditory signals". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 372, nr 1714 (19.02.2017): 20160104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0104.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Early visual cortex receives non-feedforward input from lateral and top-down connections (Muckli & Petro 2013 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23 , 195–201. ( doi:10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.020 )), including long-range projections from auditory areas. Early visual cortex can code for high-level auditory information, with neural patterns representing natural sound stimulation (Vetter et al. 2014 Curr. Biol. 24 , 1256–1262. ( doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.04.020 )). We discuss a number of questions arising from these findings. What is the adaptive function of bimodal representations in visual cortex? What type of information projects from auditory to visual cortex? What are the anatomical constraints of auditory information in V1, for example, periphery versus fovea, superficial versus deep cortical layers? Is there a putative neural mechanism we can infer from human neuroimaging data and recent theoretical accounts of cortex? We also present data showing we can read out high-level auditory information from the activation patterns of early visual cortex even when visual cortex receives simple visual stimulation, suggesting independent channels for visual and auditory signals in V1. We speculate which cellular mechanisms allow V1 to be contextually modulated by auditory input to facilitate perception, cognition and behaviour. Beyond cortical feedback that facilitates perception, we argue that there is also feedback serving counterfactual processing during imagery, dreaming and mind wandering, which is not relevant for immediate perception but for behaviour and cognition over a longer time frame. This article is part of the themed issue ‘Auditory and visual scene analysis’.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
17

de-Wit, Lee H., Robert W. Kentridge i A. David Milner. "Shape Processing Area LO and Illusory Contours". Perception 38, nr 8 (1.01.2009): 1260–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p6388.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Recent functional MRI has demonstrated that illusory contours can activate the primary visual cortex. Our investigation sought to demonstrate whether this correlation reflects computations performed in the primary visual cortex or feedback effects from shape processing area LO. We explored this in a patient who has a bilateral lesion to LO, but a functionally spared V1. Our data indicate that illusory contours are unable to influence behaviour without visual area LO. Whilst we would not claim that our data provide evidence for the ‘cognitive’ nature of illusory contours, they certainly suggest that illusory contours are dependent upon the computations involved in extracting shape representations in LO. Our data highlight the importance of neuropsychological research in interpreting the role of feedforward and feedback effects in the generation of visual illusions.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
18

Merabet, Lotfi B., Jascha D. Swisher, Stephanie A. McMains, Mark A. Halko, Amir Amedi, Alvaro Pascual-Leone i David C. Somers. "Combined Activation and Deactivation of Visual Cortex During Tactile Sensory Processing". Journal of Neurophysiology 97, nr 2 (luty 2007): 1633–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00806.2006.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
The involvement of occipital cortex in sensory processing is not restricted solely to the visual modality. Tactile processing has been shown to modulate higher-order visual and multisensory integration areas in sighted as well as visually deprived subjects; however, the extent of involvement of early visual cortical areas remains unclear. To investigate this issue, we employed functional magnetic resonance imaging in normally sighted, briefly blindfolded subjects with well-defined visuotopic borders as they tactually explored and rated raised-dot patterns. Tactile task performance resulted in significant activation in primary visual cortex (V1) and deactivation of extrastriate cortical regions V2, V3, V3A, and hV4 with greater deactivation in dorsal subregions and higher visual areas. These results suggest that tactile processing affects occipital cortex via two distinct pathways: a suppressive top-down pathway descending through the visual cortical hierarchy and an excitatory pathway arising from outside the visual cortical hierarchy that drives area V1 directly.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
19

CAO, AN, i PETER H. SCHILLER. "Neural responses to relative speed in the primary visual cortex of rhesus monkey". Visual Neuroscience 20, nr 1 (styczeń 2003): 77–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952523803201085.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Relative motion information, especially relative speed between different input patterns, is required for solving many complex tasks of the visual system, such as depth perception by motion parallax and motion-induced figure/ground segmentation. However, little is known about the neural substrate for processing relative speed information. To explore the neural mechanisms for relative speed, we recorded single-unit responses to relative motion in the primary visual cortex (area V1) of rhesus monkeys while presenting sets of random-dot arrays moving at different speeds. We found that most V1 neurons were sensitive to the existence of a discontinuity in speed, that is, they showed higher responses when relative motion was presented compared to homogenous field motion. Seventy percent of the neurons in our sample responded predominantly to relative rather than to absolute speed. Relative speed tuning curves were similar at different center–surround velocity combinations. These relative motion-sensitive neurons in macaque area V1 probably contribute to figure/ground segmentation and motion discontinuity detection.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
20

Reyes, Laura D., Tessa Harland, Roger L. Reep, Chet C. Sherwood i Bob Jacobs. "Golgi Analysis of Neuron Morphology in the Presumptive Somatosensory Cortex and Visual Cortex of the Florida Manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris)". Brain, Behavior and Evolution 87, nr 2 (2016): 105–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000445495.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
The current study investigates neuron morphology in presumptive primary somatosensory (S1) and primary visual (V1) cortices of the Florida manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris) as revealed by Golgi impregnation. Sirenians, including manatees, have an aquatic lifestyle, a large body size, and a relatively large lissencephalic brain. The present study examines neuron morphology in 3 cortical areas: in S1, dorsolateral cortex area 1 (DL1) and cluster cortex area 2 (CL2) and in V1, dorsolateral cortex area 4 (DL4). Neurons exhibited a variety of morphological types, with pyramidal neurons being the most common. The large variety of neuron types present in the manatee cortex was comparable to that seen in other eutherian mammals, except for rodents and primates, where pyramid-shaped neurons predominate. A comparison between pyramidal neurons in S1 and V1 indicated relatively greater dendritic branching in S1. Across all 3 areas, the dendritic arborization pattern of pyramidal neurons was also similar to that observed previously in the afrotherian rock hyrax, cetartiodactyls, opossums, and echidnas but did not resemble the widely bifurcated dendrites seen in the large-brained African elephant. Despite adaptations for an aquatic environment, manatees did not share specific neuron types such as tritufted and star-like neurons that have been found in cetaceans. Manatees exhibit an evolutionarily primitive pattern of cortical neuron morphology shared with most other mammals and do not appear to have neuronal specializations for an aquatic niche.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
21

Klein, Isabelle, Anne-Lise Paradis, Jean-Baptiste Poline, Stephen M. Kosslyn i Denis Le Bihan. "Transient Activity in the Human Calcarine Cortex During Visual-Mental Imagery: An Event-Related fMRI Study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, supplement 2 (listopad 2000): 15–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892900564037.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Although it is largely accepted that visual-mental imagery and perception draw on many of the same neural structures, the existence and nature of neural processing in the primary visual cortex (or area V1) during visual imagery remains controversial. We tested two general hypotheses: The first was that V1 is activated only when images with many details are formed and used, and the second was that V1 is activated whenever images are formed, even if they are not necessarily used to perform a task. We used event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (ER-fMRI) to detect and characterize the activity in the calcarine sulcus (which contains the primary visual cortex) during single instances of mental imagery. The results revealed reproducible transient activity in this area whenever participants generated or evaluated a mental image. This transient activity was strongly enhanced when participants evaluated characteristics of objects, whether or not details actually needed to be extracted from the image to perform the task. These results show that visual imagery processing commonly involves the earliest stages of the visual system.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
22

LEVITT, JONATHAN B., i JENNIFER S. LUND. "The spatial extent over which neurons in macaque striate cortex pool visual signals". Visual Neuroscience 19, nr 4 (lipiec 2002): 439–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952523802194065.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
We recorded activity of single units in macaque monkey primary visual cortex (V1) to define the retinotopic extent of the visual inputs that drive or modulate V1 neuron responses in parafoveal and peripheral (calcarine) cortex. We used high-contrast drifting grating stimuli to define the extent of the area over which responses summate and the extent of the receptive-field surround. We found responses of most V1 cells to summate over 1 deg, with a suppressive surround typically twice that in diameter, though for some cells (even in parafoveal V1) surrounds exceeded 13 deg in diameter. Surprisingly, we found no significant laminar differences in these dimensions or in the strength of surround suppression. We found that surround suppression in most cells arises from both the ends and sides of the receptive field. Our measures indicate that the strongest modulatory input arises from regions immediately adjacent to the excitatory summation area. These physiological measures suggest that the high-contrast summation field of V1 neurons can be accounted for by the sum of lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) inputs offered to the local cortical column, with monosynaptic lateral connections within area V1 adding the larger dimensions of the low-contrast summation field and the near surround. Neither of these inputs suffice to explain the largest surrounds, which most likely derive from feedback from extrastriate visual areas.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
23

Nelles, Gereon, Guido Widmann, Joachim Esser, Anette Meistrowitz, Johannes Weber, Michael Forsting i H. Christoph Diener. "Cortical reorganization of the visual system in post-stroke hemianopia studied with fMRI". Stroke 32, suppl_1 (styczeń 2001): 334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/str.32.suppl_1.334.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
102 Introduction: Restitution of unilateral visual field defects following occipital cortex lesions occurs rarely. Partial recovery, however, can be observed in patients with incomplete lesion of the visual cortex. Our objective was to study the neuroplastic changes in the visual system that underlie such recovery. Methods and Results: Six patients with a left PCA-territory cortical stroke and 6 healthy control subjects were studied during rest and during visual stimulation using a 1.5 T fMRI with a 40 mT gradient. Visual stimuli were projected with a laptop computer onto a 154 x 115 cm screen, placed 90 cm in front of the gantry. Subjects were asked to fixate a red point in the center of the screen during both conditions. During stimulation, a black-and-white checkerboard pattern reversal was presented in each hemifield. For each side, 120 volumes of 48 contiguous axial fMRI images were obtained during rest and during hemifield stimulation in alternating order (60 volumes for each condition). Significant differences of rCBF between stimulation and rest were assessed as group analyses using statistical parametric mapping (SPM 99; p<0.01, corrected for multiple comparison). In controls, strong increases of rCBF (Z=7.6) occurred in the contralateral primary visual cortex V1 (area 17) and in V3a (area 18) and V5 (area 19). No differences were found between the right and left side in controls. During stimulation of the unaffected (left) visual field in hemianopic patients, activation occurred in contralateral V1, but the strongest increases of rCBF (Z>10) were seen in contralateral V3a (area 18) and V5 (area 19). During stimulation of the hemianopic (right) visual field, no activation was found in the primary visual cortex of either hemisphere. The most significant activation (Z=9.2) was seen in the ipsilateral V3a and V5 areas, and contralateral (left) V3a. Conclusions: Partial recovery from hemianopia is associated with strong ipsilateral activation of the visual system. Processing of visual stimuli in the hemianopic side spares the primary visual cortex and may involve recruitment of neurons in ipsilateral (contralesional) areas V3a and V5.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
24

Khayat, P. S., A. Pooresmaeili i P. R. Roelfsema. "Time Course of Attentional Modulation in the Frontal Eye Field During Curve Tracing". Journal of Neurophysiology 101, nr 4 (kwiecień 2009): 1813–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.91050.2008.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Neurons in the frontal eye fields (FEFs) register incoming visual information and select visual stimuli that are relevant for behavior. Here we investigated the timing of the visual response and the timing of selection by recording from single FEF neurons in a curve-tracing task that requires shifts of attention followed by an oculomotor response. We found that the behavioral selection signal in area FEF had a latency of 147 ms and that it was delayed substantially relative to the visual response, which occurred 50 ms after stimulus presentation. We compared the FEF responses to activity previously recorded in the primary visual cortex (area V1) during the same task. Visual responses in area V1 preceded the FEF responses, but the latencies of selection signals in areas V1 and FEF were similar. The similarity of timing of selection signals in structures at opposite ends of the visual cortical processing hierarchy supports the view that stimulus selection occurs in an interaction between widely separated cortical regions.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
25

Kaliukhovich, Dzmitry A., i Hans Op de Beeck. "Hierarchical stimulus processing in rodent primary and lateral visual cortex as assessed through neuronal selectivity and repetition suppression". Journal of Neurophysiology 120, nr 3 (1.09.2018): 926–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00673.2017.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Similar to primates, visual cortex in rodents appears to be organized in two distinct hierarchical streams. However, there is still little known about how visual information is processed along those streams in rodents. In this study, we examined how repetition suppression and position and clutter tolerance of the neuronal representations evolve along the putative ventral visual stream in rats. To address this question, we recorded multiunit spiking activity in primary visual cortex (V1) and the more downstream visual laterointermediate (LI) area of head-restrained Long-Evans rats. We employed a paradigm reminiscent of the continuous carry-over design used in human neuroimaging. In both areas, stimulus repetition attenuated the early phase of the neuronal response to the repeated stimulus, with this response suppression being greater in area LI. Furthermore, stimulus preferences were more similar across positions (position tolerance) in area LI than in V1, even though the absolute responses in both areas were very sensitive to changes in position. In contrast, the neuronal representations in both areas were equally good at tolerating the presence of limited visual clutter, as modeled by the presentation of a single flank stimulus. When probing tolerance of the neuronal representations with stimulus-specific adaptation, we detected no position tolerance in either examined brain area, whereas, on the contrary, we revealed clutter tolerance in both areas. Overall, our data demonstrate similarities and discrepancies in processing of visual information along the ventral visual stream of rodents and primates. Moreover, our results stress caution in using neuronal adaptation to probe tolerance of the neuronal representations. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Rodents are emerging as a popular animal model that complement primates for studying higher level visual functions. Similar to findings in primates, we demonstrate a greater repetition suppression and position tolerance of the neuronal representations in the downstream laterointermediate area of Long-Evans rats compared with primary visual cortex. However, we report no difference in the degree of clutter tolerance between the areas. These findings provide additional evidence for hierarchical processing of visual stimuli in rodents.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
26

Inui, Koji, i Ryusuke Kakigi. "Temporal Analysis of the Flow From V1 to the Extrastriate Cortex in Humans". Journal of Neurophysiology 96, nr 2 (sierpień 2006): 775–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00103.2006.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
We previously examined the cortical processing in response to somatosensory, auditory and noxious stimuli, using magnetoencephalography in humans. Here, we performed a similar analysis of the processing in the human visual cortex for comparative purposes. After flash stimuli applied to the right eye, activations were found in eight cortical areas: the left medial occipital area around the calcarine fissure (primary visual cortex, V1), the left dorsomedial area around the parietooccipital sulcus (DM), the ventral (MOv) and dorsal (MOd) parts of the middle occipital area of bilateral hemispheres, the left temporo-occipito-parietal cortex corresponding to human MT/V5 (hMT), and the ventral surface of the medial occipital area (VO) of the bilateral hemispheres. The mean onset latencies of each cortical activity were (in ms): 27.5 (V1), 31.8 (DM), 32.8 (left MOv), 32.2 (right MOv), 33.4 (left MOd), 32.3 (right MOv), 37.8 (hMT), 46.9 (left VO), and 46.4 (right VO). Therefore the cortico-cortical connection time of visual processing at the early stage was 4–6 ms, which is very similar to the time delay between sequential activations in somatosensory and auditory processing. In addition, the activities in V1, MOd, DM, and hMT showed a similar biphasic waveform with a reversal of polarity after 10 ms, which is a common activation profile of the cortical activity for somatosensory, auditory, and pain-evoked responses. These results suggest similar mechanisms of the serial cortico-cortical processing of sensory information among all sensory areas of the cortex.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
27

Vreysen, Samme, Bin Zhang, Yuzo M. Chino, Lutgarde Arckens i Gert Van den Bergh. "Dynamics of spatial frequency tuning in mouse visual cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 107, nr 11 (1.06.2012): 2937–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00022.2012.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Neuronal spatial frequency tuning in primary visual cortex (V1) substantially changes over time. In both primates and cats, a shift of the neuron's preferred spatial frequency has been observed from low frequencies early in the response to higher frequencies later in the response. In most cases, this shift is accompanied by a decreased tuning bandwidth. Recently, the mouse has gained attention as a suitable animal model to study the basic mechanisms of visual information processing, demonstrating similarities in basic neuronal response properties between rodents and highly visual mammals. Here we report the results of extracellular single-unit recordings in the anesthetized mouse where we analyzed the dynamics of spatial frequency tuning in V1 and the lateromedial area LM within the lateral extrastriate area V2L. We used a reverse-correlation technique to demonstrate that, as in monkeys and cats, the preferred spatial frequency of mouse V1 neurons shifted from low to higher frequencies later in the response. However, this was not correlated with a clear selectivity increase or enhanced suppression of responses to low spatial frequencies. These results suggest that the neuronal connections responsible for the temporal shift in spatial frequency tuning may considerably differ between mice and monkeys.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
28

Patterson, Carlyn A., Jacob Duijnhouwer, Stephanie C. Wissig, Bart Krekelberg i Adam Kohn. "Similar adaptation effects in primary visual cortex and area MT of the macaque monkey under matched stimulus conditions". Journal of Neurophysiology 111, nr 6 (15.03.2014): 1203–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00030.2013.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Recent stimulus history, or adaptation, can alter neuronal response properties. Adaptation effects have been characterized in a number of visually responsive structures, from the retina to higher visual cortex. However, it remains unclear whether adaptation effects across stages of the visual system take a similar form in response to a particular sensory event. This is because studies typically probe a single structure or cortical area, using a stimulus ensemble chosen to provide potent drive to the cells of interest. Here we adopt an alternative approach and compare adaptation effects in primary visual cortex (V1) and area MT using identical stimulus ensembles. Previous work has suggested these areas adjust to recent stimulus drive in distinct ways. We show that this is not the case: adaptation effects in V1 and MT can involve weak or strong loss of responsivity and shifts in neuronal preference toward or away from the adapter, depending on stimulus size and adaptation duration. For a particular stimulus size and adaptation duration, however, effects are similar in nature and magnitude in V1 and MT. We also show that adaptation effects in MT of awake animals depend strongly on stimulus size. Our results suggest that the strategies for adjusting to recent stimulus history depend more strongly on adaptation duration and stimulus size than on the cortical area. Moreover, they indicate that different levels of the visual system adapt similarly to recent sensory experience.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
29

Zhang, Qing-fang, Hao Li, Ming Chen, Aike Guo, Yunqing Wen i Mu-ming Poo. "Functional organization of intrinsic and feedback presynaptic inputs in the primary visual cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115, nr 22 (14.05.2018): E5174—E5182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1719711115.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
In the primary visual cortex (V1) of many mammalian species, neurons are spatially organized according to their preferred orientation into a highly ordered map. However, whether and how the various presynaptic inputs to V1 neurons are organized relative to the neuronal orientation map remain unclear. To address this issue, we constructed genetically encoded calcium indicators targeting axon boutons in two colors and used them to map the organization of axon boutons of V1 intrinsic and V2–V1 feedback projections in tree shrews. Both connections are spatially organized into maps according to the preferred orientations of axon boutons. Dual-color calcium imaging showed that V1 intrinsic inputs are precisely aligned to the orientation map of V1 cell bodies, while the V2–V1 feedback projections are aligned to the V1 map with less accuracy. Nonselective integration of intrinsic presynaptic inputs around the dendritic tree is sufficient to reproduce cell body orientation preference. These results indicate that a precisely aligned map of intrinsic inputs could reinforce the neuronal map in V1, a principle that may be prevalent for brain areas with function maps.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
30

Reich, Daniel S., Ferenc Mechler i Jonathan D. Victor. "Formal and Attribute-Specific Information in Primary Visual Cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 85, nr 1 (1.01.2001): 305–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.305.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
We estimate the rates at which neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) of anesthetized macaque monkeys transmit stimulus-related information in response to three types of visual stimulus. The stimuli—randomly modulated checkerboard patterns, stationary sinusoidal gratings, and drifting sinusoidal gratings—have very different spatiotemporal structures. We obtain the overall rate of information transmission, which we call formal information, by a direct method. We find the highest information rates in the responses of simple cells to drifting gratings (median: 10.3 bits/s, 0.92 bits/spike); responses to randomly modulated stimuli and stationary gratings transmit information at significantly lower rates. In general, simple cells transmit information at higher rates, and over a larger range, than do complex cells. Thus in the responses of V1 neurons, stimuli that are rapidly modulated do not necessarily evoke higher information rates, as might be the case with motion-sensitive neurons in area MT. By an extension of the direct method, we parse the formal information into attribute-specific components, which provide estimates of the information transmitted about contrast and spatiotemporal pattern. We find that contrast-specific information rates vary across neurons—about 0.3 to 2.1 bits/s or 0.05 to 0.22 bits/spike—but depend little on stimulus type. Spatiotemporal pattern-specific information rates, however, depend strongly on the type of stimulus and neuron (simple or complex). The remaining information rate, typically between 10 and 32% of the formal information rate for each neuron, cannot be unambiguously assigned to either contrast or spatiotemporal pattern. This indicates that some information concerning these two stimulus attributes is confounded in the responses of single neurons in V1. A model that considers a simple cell to consist of a linear spatiotemporal filter followed by a static rectifier predicts higher information rates than are found in real neurons and completely fails to replicate the performance of real cells in generating the confounded information.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
31

Ringach, Dario L. "Spatial Structure and Symmetry of Simple-Cell Receptive Fields in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 88, nr 1 (1.07.2002): 455–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.2002.88.1.455.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
I present measurements of the spatial structure of simple-cell receptive fields in macaque primary visual cortex (area V1). Similar to previous findings in cat area 17, the spatial profile of simple-cell receptive fields in the macaque is well described by two-dimensional Gabor functions. A population analysis reveals that the distribution of spatial profiles in primary visual cortex lies approximately on a one-parameter family of filter shapes. Surprisingly, the receptive fields cluster into even- and odd-symmetry classes with a tendency for neurons that are well tuned in orientation and spatial frequency to have odd-symmetric receptive fields. The filter shapes predicted by two recent theories of simple-cell receptive field function, independent component analysis and sparse coding, are compared with the data. Both theories predict receptive fields with a larger number of subfields than observed in the experimental data. In addition, these theories do not generate receptive fields that are broadly tuned in orientation and low-pass in spatial frequency, which are commonly seen in monkey V1. The implications of these results for our understanding of image coding and representation in primary visual cortex are discussed.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
32

Rumberger, A., C. J. Tyler i J. S. Lund. "Intra- and inter-areal connections between the primary visual cortex V1 and the area immediately surrounding V1 in the rat". Neuroscience 102, nr 1 (styczeń 2001): 35–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00475-9.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
33

Ruff, Douglas A., i Marlene R. Cohen. "A normalization model suggests that attention changes the weighting of inputs between visual areas". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, nr 20 (1.05.2017): E4085—E4094. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1619857114.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Models of divisive normalization can explain the trial-averaged responses of neurons in sensory, association, and motor areas under a wide range of conditions, including how visual attention changes the gains of neurons in visual cortex. Attention, like other modulatory processes, is also associated with changes in the extent to which pairs of neurons share trial-to-trial variability. We showed recently that in addition to decreasing correlations between similarly tuned neurons within the same visual area, attention increases correlations between neurons in primary visual cortex (V1) and the middle temporal area (MT) and that an extension of a classic normalization model can account for this correlation increase. One of the benefits of having a descriptive model that can account for many physiological observations is that it can be used to probe the mechanisms underlying processes such as attention. Here, we use electrical microstimulation in V1 paired with recording in MT to provide causal evidence that the relationship between V1 and MT activity is nonlinear and is well described by divisive normalization. We then use the normalization model and recording and microstimulation experiments to show that the attention dependence of V1–MT correlations is better explained by a mechanism in which attention changes the weights of connections between V1 and MT than by a mechanism that modulates responses in either area. Our study shows that normalization can explain interactions between neurons in different areas and provides a framework for using multiarea recording and stimulation to probe the neural mechanisms underlying neuronal computations.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
34

ARMSTRONG, R. A. "Pathological Changes in the Primary Visual Cortex (Area V1) in Sporadic Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease". Optometry and Vision Science 80, nr 4 (kwiecień 2003): 298–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-200304000-00007.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
35

Haak, Koen V., Koen V. Haak, Antony B. Morland, Koen V. Haak, Antony B. Morland i Stephen A. Engel. "Plasticity, and Its Limits, in Adult Human Primary Visual Cortex". Multisensory Research 28, nr 3-4 (2015): 297–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002496.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
There is an ongoing debate about whether adult human primary visual cortex (V1) is capable of large-scale cortical reorganization in response to bilateral retinal lesions. Animal models suggest that the visual neural circuitry maintains some plasticity through adulthood, and there are also a few human imaging studies in support this notion. However, the interpretation of these data has been brought into question, because there are factors besides cortical reorganization, such as the presence of sampling bias and/or the unmasking of task-dependent feedback signals from higher level visual areas, that could also explain the results. How reasonable would it be to accept that adult human V1 does not reorganize itself in the face of disease? Here, we discuss new evidence for the hypothesis that adult human V1 is not as capable of reorganization as in animals and juveniles, because in adult humans, cortical reorganization would come with costs that outweigh its benefits. These costs are likely functional and visible in recent experiments on adaptation — a rapid, short-term form of neural plasticity — where they prevent reorganization from being sustained over the long term.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
36

Schmidt, Kerstin E., Wolf Singer i Ralf A. W. Galuske. "Processing Deficits in Primary Visual Cortex of Amblyopic Cats". Journal of Neurophysiology 91, nr 4 (kwiecień 2004): 1661–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00878.2003.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Early esotropic squint frequently results in permanent visual deficits in one eye, referred to as strabismic amblyopia. The neurophysiological substrate corresponding to these deficits is still a matter of investigation. Electrophysiological evidence is available for disturbed neuronal interactions in both V1 and higher cortical areas. In this study, we investigated the modulation of responses in cat V1 to gratings at different orientations and spatial frequencies (SFs; 0.1–2.0 cycles/°) with optical imaging of intrinsic signals. Maps evoked by both eyes were well modulated at most spatial frequencies. The layout of the maps resembled that of normal cats, and iso-orientation domains tended to cross adjacent ocular dominance borders preferentially at right angles. Visually evoked potentials (VEPs) were recorded at SFs ranging from 0.1 to 3.5 cycles/° and revealed a consistently weaker eye for the majority of squinting cats. At each SF, interocular differences in VEP amplitudes corresponded well with differences in orientation response and selectivity in the maps. At 0.7–1.3 cycles/°, population orientation selectivity was significantly lower for the weaker eye in cats with VEP differences compared with those with no VEP amplitude differences. In addition, the cutoff SF, above which gratings no longer induced orientation maps, was lower for the weaker eye (≥1.0 cycles/°). These data reveal a close correlation between the loss of visual acuity in amblyopia as assessed by VEPs and the modulation of neuronal activation as seen by optical imaging of intrinsic signals. Furthermore, the results indicate that amblyopia is associated with altered intracortical processing already in V1.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
37

Vermaercke, Ben, Florian J. Gerich, Ellen Ytebrouck, Lutgarde Arckens, Hans P. Op de Beeck i Gert Van den Bergh. "Functional specialization in rat occipital and temporal visual cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 112, nr 8 (15.10.2014): 1963–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00737.2013.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Recent studies have revealed a surprising degree of functional specialization in rodent visual cortex. Anatomically, suggestions have been made about the existence of hierarchical pathways with similarities to the ventral and dorsal pathways in primates. Here we aimed to characterize some important functional properties in part of the supposed “ventral” pathway in rats. We investigated the functional properties along a progression of five visual areas in awake rats, from primary visual cortex (V1) over lateromedial (LM), latero-intermediate (LI), and laterolateral (LL) areas up to the newly found lateral occipito-temporal cortex (TO). Response latency increased >20 ms from areas V1/LM/LI to areas LL and TO. Orientation and direction selectivity for the used grating patterns increased gradually from V1 to TO. Overall responsiveness and selectivity to shape stimuli decreased from V1 to TO and was increasingly dependent upon shape motion. Neural similarity for shapes could be accounted for by a simple computational model in V1, but not in the other areas. Across areas, we find a gradual change in which stimulus pairs are most discriminable. Finally, tolerance to position changes increased toward TO. These findings provide unique information about possible commonalities and differences between rodents and primates in hierarchical cortical processing.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
38

ZIETSCH, BRENDAN, i GUY N. ELSTON. "FRACTAL ANALYSIS OF PYRAMIDAL CELLS IN THE VISUAL CORTEX OF THE GALAGO (OTOLEMUR GARNETTI): REGIONAL VARIATION IN DENDRITIC BRANCHING PATTERNS BETWEEN VISUAL AREAS". Fractals 13, nr 02 (czerwiec 2005): 83–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s0218348x05002829.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Previously it has been shown that the branching pattern of pyramidal cells varies markedly between different cortical areas in simian primates. These differences are thought to influence the functional complexity of the cells. In particular, there is a progressive increase in the fractal dimension of pyramidal cells with anterior progression through cortical areas in the occipitotemporal (OT) visual stream, including the primary visual area (V1), the second visual area (V2), the dorsolateral area (DL, corresponding to the fourth visual area) and inferotemporal cortex (IT). However, there are as yet no data on the fractal dimension of these neurons in prosimian primates. Here we focused on the nocturnal prosimian galago (Otolemur garnetti). The fractal dimension (D), and aspect ratio (a measure of branching symmetry), was determined for 111 layer III pyramidal cells in V1, V2, DL and IT. We found, as in simian primates, that the fractal dimension of neurons increased with anterior progression from V1 through V2, DL, and IT. Two important conclusions can be drawn from these results: (1) the trend for increasing branching complexity with anterior progression through OT areas was likely to be present in a common primate ancestor, and (2) specialization in neuron structure more likely facilitates object recognition than spectral processing.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
39

Smith, Matthew A., Xiaoxuan Jia, Amin Zandvakili i Adam Kohn. "Laminar dependence of neuronal correlations in visual cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 109, nr 4 (15.02.2013): 940–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00846.2012.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Neuronal responses are correlated on a range of timescales. Correlations can affect population coding and may play an important role in cortical function. Correlations are known to depend on stimulus drive, behavioral context, and experience, but the mechanisms that determine their properties are poorly understood. Here we make use of the laminar organization of cortex, with its variations in sources of input, local circuit architecture, and neuronal properties, to test whether networks engaged in similar functions but with distinct properties generate different patterns of correlation. We find that slow timescale correlations are prominent in the superficial and deep layers of primary visual cortex (V1) of macaque monkeys, but near zero in the middle layers. Brief timescale correlation (synchrony), on the other hand, was slightly stronger in the middle layers of V1, although evident at most cortical depths. Laminar variations were also apparent in the power of the local field potential, with a complementary pattern for low frequency (<10 Hz) and gamma (30–50 Hz) power. Recordings in area V2 revealed a laminar dependence similar to V1 for synchrony, but slow timescale correlations were not different between the input layers and nearby locations. Our results reveal that cortical circuits in different laminae can generate remarkably different patterns of correlations, despite being tightly interconnected.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
40

Scholte, H. Steven, Jacob Jolij, Johannes J. Fahrenfort i Victor A. F. Lamme. "Feedforward and Recurrent Processing in Scene Segmentation: Electroencephalography and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20, nr 11 (listopad 2008): 2097–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20142.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
In texture segregation, an example of scene segmentation, we can discern two different processes: texture boundary detection and subsequent surface segregation [Lamme, V. A. F., Rodriguez-Rodriguez, V., & Spekreijse, H. Separate processing dynamics for texture elements, boundaries and surfaces in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 406–413, 1999]. Neural correlates of texture boundary detection have been found in monkey V1 [Sillito, A. M., Grieve, K. L., Jones, H. E., Cudeiro, J., & Davis, J. Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities. Nature, 378, 492–496, 1995; Grosof, D. H., Shapley, R. M., & Hawken, M. J. Macaque-V1 neurons can signal illusory contours. Nature, 365, 550–552, 1993], but whether surface segregation occurs in monkey V1 [Rossi, A. F., Desimone, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex of macaques. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 1698–1709, 2001; Lamme, V. A. F. The neurophysiology of figure ground segregation in primary visual-cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1605–1615, 1995], and whether boundary detection or surface segregation signals can also be measured in human V1, is more controversial [Kastner, S., De Weerd, P., & Ungerleider, L. G. Texture segregation in the human visual cortex: A functional MRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, 2453–2457, 2000]. Here we present electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging data that have been recorded with a paradigm that makes it possible to differentiate between boundary detection and scene segmentation in humans. In this way, we were able to show with EEG that neural correlates of texture boundary detection are first present in the early visual cortex around 92 msec and then spread toward the parietal and temporal lobes. Correlates of surface segregation first appear in temporal areas (around 112 msec) and from there appear to spread to parietal, and back to occipital areas. After 208 msec, correlates of surface segregation and boundary detection also appear in more frontal areas. Blood oxygenation level-dependent magnetic resonance imaging results show correlates of boundary detection and surface segregation in all early visual areas including V1. We conclude that texture boundaries are detected in a feedforward fashion and are represented at increasing latencies in higher visual areas. Surface segregation, on the other hand, is represented in “reverse hierarchical” fashion and seems to arise from feedback signals toward early visual areas such as V1.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
41

Mundinano, Inaki-Carril, William C. Kwan i James A. Bourne. "Retinotopic specializations of cortical and thalamic inputs to area MT". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116, nr 46 (28.10.2019): 23326–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1909799116.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Retinotopic specializations in the ventral visual stream, especially foveal adaptations, provide primates with high-acuity vision in the central visual field. However, visual field specializations have not been studied in the dorsal visual stream, dedicated to processing visual motion and visually guided behaviors. To investigate this, we injected retrograde neuronal tracers occupying the whole visuotopic representation of the middle temporal (MT) visual area in marmoset monkeys and studied the distribution and morphology of the afferent primary visual cortex (V1) projections. Contrary to previous reports, we found a heterogeneous population of V1-MT projecting neurons distributed in layers 3C and 6. In layer 3C, spiny stellate neurons were distributed mainly in foveal representations, while pyramidal morphologies were characteristic of peripheral eccentricities. This primate adaptation of the V1 to MT pathway is arranged in a way that we had not previously understood, with abundant stellate projection neurons in the high-resolution foveal portions, suggesting rapid relay of motion information to visual area MT. We also describe that the medial portion of the inferior pulvinar (PIm), which is the main thalamic input to area MT, shows a retinotopic organization, likely reflecting the importance of this pathway during development and the establishment of area MT topography.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
42

Kumano, Hironori, i Takanori Uka. "The Spatial Profile of Macaque MT Neurons Is Consistent With Gaussian Sampling of Logarithmically Coordinated Visual Representation". Journal of Neurophysiology 104, nr 1 (lipiec 2010): 61–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00040.2010.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Neurons in extrastriate visual areas have large receptive fields (RFs) compared with those in primary visual cortex (V1), suggesting extensive spatial integration. To examine the spatial integration of neurons in area MT, we modeled the RFs of MT neurons based on a symmetrical (Gaussian) integration of V1 outputs and tested the model using single-unit recording in two fixating macaque monkeys. Because visual representation in V1 is logarithmically compressed along eccentricity, the resulting RF model is log-Gaussian along the radial axis in polar coordinates. To test the log-Gaussian model, the RF of each neuron was mapped on a 5 × 5 grid using a small patch of random dots drifting at the preferred velocity of the neuron. The majority of MT neurons had RFs with a steeper slope near the fovea and a shallower slope away from the fovea. Among various two-dimensional Gaussian models fitted to the RFs, the log-Gaussian model provided the best description. The fitted parameters revealed that the range of sampling by MT neurons has no systematic relationship with eccentricities, consistent with a recent study for V4 neurons. Our results suggest that MT neurons integrate inputs from constant-sized patches of V1 cortex.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
43

Hallum, Luke E., Michael S. Landy i David J. Heeger. "Human primary visual cortex (V1) is selective for second-order spatial frequency". Journal of Neurophysiology 105, nr 5 (maj 2011): 2121–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01007.2010.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
A variety of cues can differentiate objects from their surrounds. These include “first-order” cues such as luminance modulations and “second-order” cues involving modulations of orientation and contrast. Human sensitivity to first-order modulations is well described by a computational model involving spatially localized filters that are selective for orientation and spatial frequency (SF). It is widely held that first-order modulations are represented by the firing rates of simple and complex cells (“first-order” neurons) in primary visual cortex (V1) that, likewise, have spatially localized receptive fields that are selective for orientation- and SF. Human sensitivity to second-order modulations is well described by a filter-rectify-filter (FRF) model, with first- and second-order filters selective for orientation and SF. However, little is known about how neuronal activity in visual cortex represents second-order modulations. We tested the FRF model by using an functional (f)MRI-adaptation protocol to characterize the selectivity of activity in visual cortex to second-order, orientation-defined gratings of two different SFs. fMRI responses throughout early visual cortex exhibited selective adaptation to these stimuli. The low-SF grating was a more effective adapter than the high-SF grating, incompatible with the FRF model. To explain the results, we extended the FRF model by incorporating normalization, yielding a filter-rectify-normalize-filter model, in which normalization enhances selectivity for second-order SF but only for low spatial frequencies. We conclude that neurons in human visual cortex are selective for second-order SF, that normalization (surround suppression) contributes to this selectivity, and that the selectivity in higher visual areas is simply fed forward from V1.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
44

Mock, Vanessa L., Kimberly L. Luke, Jacqueline R. Hembrook-Short i Farran Briggs. "Dynamic communication of attention signals between the LGN and V1". Journal of Neurophysiology 120, nr 4 (1.10.2018): 1625–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00224.2018.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Correlations and inferred causal interactions among local field potentials (LFPs) simultaneously recorded in distinct visual brain areas can provide insight into how visual and cognitive signals are communicated between neuronal populations. Based on the known anatomical connectivity of hierarchically organized visual cortical areas and electrophysiological measurements of LFP interactions, a framework for interareal frequency-specific communication has emerged. Our goals were to test the predictions of this framework in the context of the early visual pathways and to understand how attention modulates communication between the visual thalamus and primary visual cortex. We recorded LFPs simultaneously in retinotopically aligned regions of the visual thalamus and primary visual cortex in alert and behaving macaque monkeys trained on a contrast-change detection task requiring covert shifts in visual spatial attention. Coherence and Granger-causal interactions among early visual circuits varied dynamically over different trial periods. Attention significantly enhanced alpha-, beta-, and gamma-frequency interactions, often in a manner consistent with the known anatomy of early visual circuits. However, attentional modulation of communication among early visual circuits was not consistent with a simple static framework in which distinct frequency bands convey directed inputs. Instead, neuronal network interactions in early visual circuits were flexible and dynamic, perhaps reflecting task-related shifts in attention. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Attention alters the way we perceive the visual world. For example, attention can modulate how visual information is communicated between the thalamus and cortex. We recorded local field potentials simultaneously in the visual thalamus and cortex to quantify the impact of attention on visual information communication. We found that attentional modulation of visual information communication was not static, but dynamic over the time course of trials.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
45

Sellers, Kristin K., Davis V. Bennett, Axel Hutt, James H. Williams i Flavio Fröhlich. "Awake vs. anesthetized: layer-specific sensory processing in visual cortex and functional connectivity between cortical areas". Journal of Neurophysiology 113, nr 10 (czerwiec 2015): 3798–815. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00923.2014.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
During general anesthesia, global brain activity and behavioral state are profoundly altered. Yet it remains mostly unknown how anesthetics alter sensory processing across cortical layers and modulate functional cortico-cortical connectivity. To address this gap in knowledge of the micro- and mesoscale effects of anesthetics on sensory processing in the cortical microcircuit, we recorded multiunit activity and local field potential in awake and anesthetized ferrets ( Mustela putoris furo) during sensory stimulation. To understand how anesthetics alter sensory processing in a primary sensory area and the representation of sensory input in higher-order association areas, we studied the local sensory responses and long-range functional connectivity of primary visual cortex (V1) and prefrontal cortex (PFC). Isoflurane combined with xylazine provided general anesthesia for all anesthetized recordings. We found that anesthetics altered the duration of sensory-evoked responses, disrupted the response dynamics across cortical layers, suppressed both multimodal interactions in V1 and sensory responses in PFC, and reduced functional cortico-cortical connectivity between V1 and PFC. Together, the present findings demonstrate altered sensory responses and impaired functional network connectivity during anesthesia at the level of multiunit activity and local field potential across cortical layers.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
46

Kirchberger, Lisa, Sreedeep Mukherjee, Ulf H. Schnabel, Enny H. van Beest, Areg Barsegyan, Christiaan N. Levelt, J. Alexander Heimel i in. "The essential role of recurrent processing for figure-ground perception in mice". Science Advances 7, nr 27 (czerwiec 2021): eabe1833. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abe1833.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
The segregation of figures from the background is an important step in visual perception. In primary visual cortex, figures evoke stronger activity than backgrounds during a delayed phase of the neuronal responses, but it is unknown how this figure-ground modulation (FGM) arises and whether it is necessary for perception. Here, we show, using optogenetic silencing in mice, that the delayed V1 response phase is necessary for figure-ground segregation. Neurons in higher visual areas also exhibit FGM and optogenetic silencing of higher areas reduced FGM in V1. In V1, figures elicited higher activity of vasoactive intestinal peptide–expressing (VIP) interneurons than the background, whereas figures suppressed somatostatin-positive interneurons, resulting in an increased activation of pyramidal cells. Optogenetic silencing of VIP neurons reduced FGM in V1, indicating that disinhibitory circuits contribute to FGM. Our results provide insight into how lower and higher areas of the visual cortex interact to shape visual perception.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
47

Maniglia, M., Y. Trotter i F. Aedo-Jury. "TMS reveals inhibitory extrastriate cortico-cortical feedback modulation of V1 activity in humans". Brain Structure and Function 224, nr 9 (17.10.2019): 3399–408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01964-z.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Abstract The interaction between the primary visual cortex (V1) and extrastriate visual areas provides the first building blocks in our perception of the world. V2, in particular, seems to play a crucial role in shaping contextual modulation information through feedback projections to V1. However, whether this feedback is inhibitory or excitatory is still unclear. In order to test the nature of V2 feedback to V1, we used neuronavigation-guided offline inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) on V2 before testing participants on collinear facilitation, a contrast detection task with lateral masking. This contextual modulation task is thought to rely on horizontal connections in V1 and possibly extrastriate feedback. Results showed that when inhibitory TMS was delivered over V2, contrast thresholds decreased for targets presented in the contralateral hemifield, consistent with the retinotopic mapping of this area, while having no effect for targets presented in the ipsilateral hemifield or after control (CZ) stimulation. These results suggest that feedback from V2 to V1 during contextual modulation is mostly inhibitory, corroborating recent observations in monkey electrophysiology and extending this mechanism to human visual system. Moreover, we provide for the first time direct evidence of the involvement of extrastriate visual areas in collinear facilitation.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
48

Ishii, Naohiro, Toshinori Deguchi, Masashi Kawaguchi i Hiroshi Sasaki. "Vector Operations in Neural Network Computations". International Journal of Software Innovation 1, nr 2 (kwiecień 2013): 40–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijsi.2013040104.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Nonlinearity is an important factor in the biological visual neural networks. Among prominent features of the visual networks, movement detections are carried out in the visual cortex. The visual cortex for the movement detection, consist of two layered networks, called the primary visual cortex (V1), followed by the middle temporal area (MT), in which nonlinear functions will play important roles in the visual systems. These networks will be decomposed to asymmetric sub-networks with nonlinearities. In this paper, the fundamental characteristics in asymmetric and symmetric neural networks with nonlinearities are developed for the detection of the changing stimulus or the movement detection in these neural networks. By the optimization of the asymmetric networks, movement detection Equations are derived. Then, it was clarified that the even – odd nonlinearity combined asymmetric networks, has the ability of generating directional vector in the stimulus change detection or movement detection, while symmetric networks need the time memory to have the same ability. Further, the vector operations in the neural network are developed. These facts are applied to two layered networks, V1 and MT.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
49

Celeghin, Alessia, Matteo Diano, Beatrice de Gelder, Lawrence Weiskrantz, Carlo A. Marzi i Marco Tamietto. "Intact hemisphere and corpus callosum compensate for visuomotor functions after early visual cortex damage". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, nr 48 (13.11.2017): E10475—E10483. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1714801114.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Unilateral damage to the primary visual cortex (V1) leads to clinical blindness in the opposite visual hemifield, yet nonconscious ability to transform unseen visual input into motor output can be retained, a condition known as “blindsight.” Here we combined psychophysics, functional magnetic resonance imaging, and tractography to investigate the functional and structural properties that enable the developing brain to partly overcome the effects of early V1 lesion in one blindsight patient. Visual stimuli appeared in either the intact or blind hemifield and simple responses were given with either the left or right hand, thereby creating conditions where visual input and motor output involve the same or opposite hemisphere. When the V1-damaged hemisphere was challenged by incoming visual stimuli, or controlled manual responses to these unseen stimuli, the corpus callosum (CC) dynamically recruited areas in the visual dorsal stream and premotor cortex of the intact hemisphere to compensate for altered visuomotor functions. These compensatory changes in functional brain activity were paralleled by increased connections in posterior regions of the CC, where fibers connecting homologous areas of the parietal cortex course.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
50

Krishna, Aravind, Seiji Tanabe i Adam Kohn. "Decision Signals in the Local Field Potentials of Early and Mid-Level Macaque Visual Cortex". Cerebral Cortex 31, nr 1 (27.08.2020): 169–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa218.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Abstract The neural basis of perceptual decision making has typically been studied using measurements of single neuron activity, though decisions are likely based on the activity of large neuronal ensembles. Local field potentials (LFPs) may, in some cases, serve as a useful proxy for population activity and thus be useful for understanding the neural basis of perceptual decision making. However, little is known about whether LFPs in sensory areas include decision-related signals. We therefore analyzed LFPs recorded using two 48­electrode arrays implanted in primary visual cortex (V1) and area V4 of macaque monkeys trained to perform a fine orientation discrimination task. We found significant choice information in low (0–30 Hz) and higher (70–500 Hz) frequency components of the LFP, but little information in gamma frequencies (30–70 Hz). Choice information was more robust in V4 than V1 and stronger in LFPs than in simultaneously measured spiking activity. LFP-based choice information included a global component, common across electrodes within an area. Our findings reveal the presence of robust choice-related signals in the LFPs recorded in V1 and V4 and suggest that LFPs may be a useful complement to spike-based analyses of decision making.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
Oferujemy zniżki na wszystkie plany premium dla autorów, których prace zostały uwzględnione w tematycznych zestawieniach literatury. Skontaktuj się z nami, aby uzyskać unikalny kod promocyjny!

Do bibliografii