Kliknij ten link, aby zobaczyć inne rodzaje publikacji na ten temat: Emotions and cognition.

Rozprawy doktorskie na temat „Emotions and cognition”

Utwórz poprawne odniesienie w stylach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard i wielu innych

Wybierz rodzaj źródła:

Sprawdź 50 najlepszych rozpraw doktorskich naukowych na temat „Emotions and cognition”.

Przycisk „Dodaj do bibliografii” jest dostępny obok każdej pracy w bibliografii. Użyj go – a my automatycznie utworzymy odniesienie bibliograficzne do wybranej pracy w stylu cytowania, którego potrzebujesz: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver itp.

Możesz również pobrać pełny tekst publikacji naukowej w formacie „.pdf” i przeczytać adnotację do pracy online, jeśli odpowiednie parametry są dostępne w metadanych.

Przeglądaj rozprawy doktorskie z różnych dziedzin i twórz odpowiednie bibliografie.

1

Boakes, Jolee Alison. "The role of specific emotions in affective priming effects". University of Western Australia. Graduate School of Education, 2010. http://theses.library.uwa.edu.au/adt-WU2010.0116.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
[Truncated abstract] The finding that less time is needed to evaluate the valence of a target when it is preceded by a prime of the same valence, as opposed to one of the opposite valence, has become known as the affective priming effect. The research reported here investigated a new model of the mechanisms underlying affective priming effects, which focuses on the role of specific emotions within global valence categories. Specifically, this model stipulates that when presented with a stimulus that elicits particular emotions, the area of the brain corresponding to that emotion is automatically stimulated. This in turn will produce a diffuse activation of meaning nodes that are associated with that emotion. This emotion-based priming model departs from previous spreading activation accounts of affective priming, as it suggests that the facilitation effects observed in such studies may be due primarily to spreading activation via emotion-congruent, rather than valence-congruent, nodes. The overarching goal of the empirical research programme reported here was to test predictions based on this model. In three studies, facilitation effects ascribable to specific emotion-based congruence (e.g., fear-fear) were compared with those ascribable to global valence-based congruence (e.g., negative-negative) alone (i.e., in the absence of emotion-based congruence). Participants made valence judgements on targets which represented one of five basic human emotions: one positive (happy) and four negative (disgust, fear, sad, and anger). ... The fundamental design elements were the same across all three studies: the only difference was in the stimulus format of the prime-target pairs: emotion-laden scenes were used in Study One; facial expressions in Study Two; and emotion-laden words in Study Three. Results showed that, in comparison to the neutral and incongruent baselines, there were significant emotion-based priming effects across all stimulus formats. This result was also consistent across all of the negative emotions employed. Significant valence-based priming effects were, however, also obtained in each of the three studies, although these effects were more inconsistent than those obtained for emotion-based priming. That is, reaction times were significantly shorter on valence-congruent than on neutral and incongruent baseline trials, but only for a portion of the tests performed across the three studies. In Studies One and Two, reaction times were consistently shorter for emotion-congruent prime-target pairs than for valence-congruent prime-target pairs. This trend was absent in Study Three, in which word stimuli were used. These results indicate that while reaction times were facilitated, albeit inconsistently, for valence-congruent prime-target pairs, they were facilitated significantly further for emotion-congruent pairs when picture-based stimuli were used. The emotion-based and valence-based priming effects obtained across the three studies are discussed in terms of three competing theories: (i) spreading activation via global valence nodes, (ii) spreading activation via emotion centres, and (iii) expectancy-based processes. A dual-process hypothesis of affective priming is then proposed. The plausibility of the hypothesis is then explored through a synthesis and re-examination of results reported in previous affective priming research. Directions for future research to elaborate and extend on this work are discussed.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
2

Cheung, Ching-ying Crystal. "Cognition of emotion recognition". Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2002. http://hub.hku.hk/bib/B29740277.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
3

Lowe, Christine A. "The effects of suppressing anger on cognition and behaviour". Thesis, Available from the University of Aberdeen Library and Historic Collections Digital Resources. Restricted: no access until April 1, 2014, 2008. http://digitool.abdn.ac.uk:80/webclient/DeliveryManager?application=DIGITOOL-3&owner=resourcediscovery&custom_att_2=simple_viewer&pid=25964.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
4

Dodd, Jessica Amen Alexandra Fineman Stephanie. "Mechanisms of self-regulation associations between cognitive control and emotion regulation /". Diss., Connect to the thesis, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10066/1427.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
5

Amen, Alexandra. "Monitoring the mind the relationship between individual differences in cognitive control and emotion regulation /". Diss., Connect to the thesis, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10066/1428.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
6

McWeeny, Jennifer. "Knowing emotions : emotional intentionality and epistemological sense /". view abstract or download file of text, 2005. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/uoregon/fullcit?p3201692.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Oregon, 2005.
Typescript. Includes vita and abstract. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 256-273). Also available for download via the World Wide Web; free to University of Oregon users.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
7

Baumeister, Jenny Charlotte. "Physically emotional: the role of embodied emotions from encoding to memory". Doctoral thesis, SISSA, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11767/3910.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Theories of embodied cognition hold that the perception of an emotional stimulus can trigger a simulation of the correspondent state in the motor, somatosensory, and affective systems. Amongst other bodily reactions, it is thought that such embodied simulations are also reflected in facial expressions in accordance to the emotional connotation of the presented stimulus – a phenomenon also referred to as facial motor resonance. Chapter 1 reviews the theories of embodied cognition, in general, and facial motor resonance, in particular. The aim of the present thesis was to further define the function of embodied simulations, reconciling previous inconsistent results concerning the level at which embodied simulations affect the processing of emotional information, and to explore uncharted aspects of embodiment theories such as their role in memory for emotional information. In Chapter 2, I investigated the hypothesis that embodied simulations play a key role in processing only emotional information (happy and sad sentences and faces), which is low in emotional intensity or difficult to encode. This hypothesis was tested in a behavioral experiment involving a group of participants undergoing subcutaneous cosmetic injections of Botulinum Toxin-A (Botox) compared with a matched control group. The results confirmed the hypothesis: participants in the Botox group, but not those in the control group, rated emotional sentences and faces as less emotional after the Botox treatment. Furthermore, they were slower at identifying sad faces as sad after the treatment. The critical nuance of these findings was that only stimuli with moderate emotional intensity were affected. Upon considering the findings of Chapter 2, the question arose as to whether facial motor resonance, in addition to playing a role in the initial processing and recognition of emotional content, also determines its retrieval. This topic was investigated in the study reported in Chapter 3, in which eighty participants underwent a memory task for emotional and neutral words. The task consisted of an encoding and a retrieval phase. Facial muscles were blocked by a hardening facial mask in one of four conditions: during encoding, during retrieval, during both encoding and retrieval, or never (control). The results showed that memory for emotional words decreased significantly if embodiment was blocked at either point in time during the experiment (during encoding, during retrieval, or during both), in contrast to the control condition. These results suggest that facial motor resonance is involved in the encoding and retrieval of emotional words. In Chapter 4, this line of research was extended and applied to the processing of emotional content in a second language (L2). In a classical memory task involving an encoding and a retrieval phase, thirty-two Spanish/English late bilinguals were presented with emotional (happy and angry) and neutral words. Electromyographic (EMG) activity and skin conductance (SC) were recorded during the encoding phase. The results suggest that the emotionality of an L2 appears to be not only reduced as compared with a first language (L1), but also to be less embodied. This was suggested both by the absence of the Enhanced Emotional Memory (EEM) effect in L2 as well as by partially decreased and delayed EMG and SC activity in response to emotional words in L2 as compared with L1. If facial motor resonance is involved in the recollection of emotional information, what is its role in forgetting emotional information? This question was pursued in the study reported in Chapter 5, employing the directed forgetting paradigm (DF), which involves the presentation of a stimulus (e.g. a word), followed by a cue to “remember” (R-cue) or to “forget” (F-cue). Twenty-one participants were instructed to remember or to intentionally forget neutral, negative, and positive words. EMG from the zygomaticus and corrugator muscle was simultaneously recorded with event related potentials (ERPs). The behavioral results showed that both neutral and emotional words were forgotten at equal rates. However, the type of word and cue instruction interactively modulated facial motor resonance, as measured by EMG. Upon R-cues, the muscle activation patterns for both negative and positive word types were significantly enhanced, in contrast to the facial motor resonance evoked by F-cues. It was speculated that the increase in facial motor resonance reflects active rehearsal, whereas the decrease is associated to active suppression mechanisms. This assumption was supported by the ERP data, indicating that the successful forgetting of affective words required more active suppression, as was indexed by enhanced frontal positivities. In contrast, intentional encoding of emotional words followed by R-cues seemed to be facilitated by an enhanced P3 and late positive potential (LPP) components emerging from centro-parietal areas. These components have been hypothesized to reflect rehearsal and memory consolidation processes. Overall, the present results suggest that embodied simulations help with the processing of indefinite emotional information and assist with the formation of enduring representations of emotional stimuli. The implications of these findings for theories of embodied cognition, in general, and for emotion processing, in particular, are discussed in Chapter 6.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
8

Stepanenko, Walter Scott. "Passionate Cognition: A Perceptual Theory of Emotion and the Role of the Emotions inCognition". University of Toledo / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=toledo1396533522.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
9

Warner, Robert L. "A computational model of human emotion". Thesis, This resource online, 1991. http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/theses/available/etd-12302008-063852/.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
10

Hines, Karen Anne. "Imagining the future and predicting emotions: The influence of imagery perspective on anticipated emotions". The Ohio State University, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1388402040.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
11

Moser, Jason Scot. "Intentional regulation of negative emotions is reflected in event-related brain potentials". Access to citation, abstract and download form provided by ProQuest Information and Learning Company; downloadable PDF file 0.59 Mb., 38 p, 2006. http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:1435829.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
12

Beck, Erika D. "The perception, experience and regulation of emotion : an fMRI approach /". Diss., Connect to a 24 p. preview or request complete full text in PDF format. Access restricted to UC IP addresses, 2001. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/ucsd/fullcit?p3026379.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
13

Ng, Hau-hei. "The effect of mood on facial emotion recognition". Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2004. http://hdl.handle.net/10722/210312.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
14

Gleave, Rachel Anne. "An Electrophysiological Investigation of Emotional Attention and Memory Biases in Depression: The Role of Working Memory Inhibitory Control Deficits". Thesis, Griffith University, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/10072/367702.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
The high recurrence rate in depression suggests specific cognitive factors increase an individual’s risk for developing repeated episodes of the disorder. A factor implicated in the literature is biased cognitive processing of negative information. This includes sustained attention, elaboration and autobiographical memory for negative versus positive events. Empirical evidence and contemporary models suggest impaired ability to utilise inhibitory control over the entry and removal of extraneous negative information in working memory mediates these emotional processing biases (see Beck, 2008; Joormann, Yoon, & Zetsche, 2007). In depression, inhibitory control deficits are linked to poor emotional regulation (Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014) and increased tendency to rumination (Joormann & Gotlib, 2008), which serve to perpetuate and exacerbate depressed affect (Beck, 2008; Joormann, Yoon, & Zetsche, 2007). Previous research has primarily focused on examining the relationship between depression and biased cognitive processes and cognitive control deficits in different studies. However, the predicted interrelations between these processes in depressed and remitted-depressed samples have received limited investigation. To advance insight into the functional relations among these emotional information processing biases and cognitive deficits, the aim of the dissertation was to investigate these processes in a single investigation.
Thesis (PhD Doctorate)
Doctor of Philosophy (PhD)
School of Applied Psychology
Griffith Health
Full Text
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
15

Williams, R. M. "Individual differences in the effects of mood on cognition". Thesis, University of Oxford, 1988. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.235083.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
16

Yan, Hoi-fai Arthur, i 殷凱輝. "Explorations into the role of emotion in moral judgement". Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2009. http://hub.hku.hk/bib/B41758171.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
17

Hare, Todd Anthony. "Developmental and individual differences in cognitive-affective processes /". Access full-text from WCMC, 2007. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1490070881&sid=14&Fmt=2&clientId=8424&RQT=309&VName=PQD.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
18

Yan, Hoi-fai Arthur. "Explorations into the role of emotion in moral judgement". Click to view the E-thesis via HKUTO, 2009. http://sunzi.lib.hku.hk/hkuto/record/B41758171.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
19

Roeder, Megan B. "Emotional influences on cross-modal attentional capture". Click here for download, 2009. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1827435611&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=3260&RQT=309&VName=PQD.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
20

Ramos, Melanie A. Kerns John Gerald. "Engagement of cognitive control and down-regulation of negative affect". Diss., Columbia, Mo. : University of Missouri--Columbia, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/10355/6543.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
The entire thesis text is included in the research.pdf file; the official abstract appears in the short.pdf file; a non-technical public abstract appears in the public.pdf file. Title from PDF of title page (University of Missouri--Columbia, viewed on December 18, 2009). Thesis advisor: Dr. John Kerns. Includes bibliographical references.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
21

Winblad, Stefan. "Myotonic dystrophy type 1 : cognition, personality and emotion /". Göteborg : Göteborg University, Dept. of Psychology, 2006. http://bvbr.bib-bvb.de:8991/F?func=service&doc_library=BVB01&doc_number=015464022&line_number=0001&func_code=DB_RECORDS&service_type=MEDIA.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
22

Paterson, Helena M. "The perception and cognition of emotion from motion". Thesis, Connect to e-thesis, 2002. http://theses.gla.ac.uk/1072/.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Thesis (Ph.D.) -- University of Glasgow, 2002.
Ph.D. thesis submitted to the Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, 2002. Includes bibliographical references. Print version also available.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
23

Mgrublian, Kathryn H. "Express Yourself: The Effects of Body Position on Non-verbal Communication of Emotions". Scholarship @ Claremont, 2011. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/cmc_theses/161.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Recent research has documented that we tend to use the face to express some emotions, but use the body to express other emotions. To understand the contributions of the body to non-verbal emotional communication, we compared the performance of able-bodied participants who were allowed to express emotions naturally (standing) to able-bodied participants who were confined to a wheelchair. Theories of embodied emotion would predict that restraining the use of the body should change emotion production and communication confidence, especially for body-related emotions. Participants expressed six different emotions in three conditions: 1) naturally, 2) face only, and 3) body only. After each trial, they indicated their confidence that they effectively communicated the emotion. Results indicated that for emotion production, both groups used primarily the face to express happiness and disgust. We predicted that participants in the wheelchair group would use the face more to express body-related emotions, but our findings show that the extinction of body occurs with specific emotions. Like the standing group, wheelchair participants used their bodies to express submissive emotions of embarrassment and fear. In contrast, they showed a distinct lack of body use for emotional displays expressing higher status or dominant emotions--pride and anger. Nonetheless, confidence in communication did not differ across groups despite production differences. These findings suggest that current body states affect how emotions are expressed. In terms of embodied emotion theory, body restrictions may make a person feel less pride or anger. From an evolutionary standpoint, it might be that displaying pride or anger when one is less physically able reduces one’s chance for survival.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
24

Cornew, Lauren A. "Emotion processing in the auditory modality the time course and development of emotional prosody recognition /". Diss., Connect to a 24 p. preview or request complete full text in PDF format. Access restricted to UC campuses, 2008. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/ucsd/fullcit?p3330854.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of California, San Diego, 2008.
Title from first page of PDF file (viewed December 11, 2008). Available via ProQuest Digital Dissertations. Vita. Includes bibliographical references.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
25

Workman, Clifford. "The role of moral cognition and emotions in remitted major depressive disorder". Thesis, University of Manchester, 2016. https://www.research.manchester.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/the-role-of-moral-cognition-and-emotions-in-remitted-major-depressive-disorder(e1c3b588-b506-47c2-a579-4ccedf1f113b).html.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Background: The aim of this thesis was to investigate the relationship of moral cognition and emotions to the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder (MDD). Patients with MDD may experience excessive guilt or self-blaming biases despite recovery from the depressed state. Since guilt is a moral emotion thought to motivate altruistic behaviours, it has been hypothesized that elevated self-blame in MDD may result in pathological increases to altruism in some patients. The relationship of self-blame to altruistic choices in individuals with remitted MDD (rMDD), however, has not been established. Guilt has been shown to activate the subgenual cingulate and adjacent septal region (SCSR) which is of known importance to the pathophysiology of MDD. Since MDD is thought to arise from network-level dysfunctions, and moral cognition and emotions are hypothesized to emerge from network-level binding, investigating resting-state SCSR functional connectivity in rMDD patients and healthy control (HC) participants could reveal networks of potential relevance both to MDD and to moral cognition and emotions. Chapter 2: We investigated whether melancholic rMDD patients could be distinguished from non-melancholic and HC groups on the basis of resting-state functional connectivity to an SCSR seed region. Lower SCSR-amygdala connectivity distinguished the melancholic rMDD group from non-melancholic and HC groups. Chapter 3: We investigated whether patients who remained resilient to recurring depressive episodes were distinguishable from recurring episode MDD and HC groups on the basis of resting-state connectivity to an SCSR seed region. Lower interhemispheric SCSR connectivity distinguished the resilient MDD patients from the recurring episode MDD and HC groups. Chapter 4: We measured explicit and implicit preferences for social options with and without altruistic motivations relative to selfish options in the rMDD and HC groups during emotion priming to modulate feelings of guilt. The rMDD patients explicitly preferred prosocial options (i.e., social options and altruism directed towards friends or colleagues) less than HC participants. Regardless of group, guilt priming increased explicit and implicit preferences for altruism towards strangers. Chapter 5: We investigated whether explicit and/or implicit preferences for prosocial options during guilt priming were correlated with resting-state connectivity to an SCSR seed region, and whether this relationship could distinguish the rMDD and HC groups. Across all participants, implicit prosocial choice preference negatively correlated with connectivity between the SCSR and right temporoparietal junction (TPJ). The relationship of SCSR-TPJ connectivity to implicit preferences for social options and for altruism towards friends and colleagues was weaker in the rMDD group compared to the HC group, particularly for implicit altruism. Conclusions: We identified resting-state SCSR networks associated with vulnerability to melancholia and with resilience to recurring depressive episodes. Patients with rMDD explicitly preferred options entailing social withdrawal, a symptom associated with MDD vulnerability. Irrespective of group, guilt motivated altruism towards strangers but not friends and colleagues. Implicit prosociality was negatively associated with connectivity in a social agency network, and the comparatively weak relationships between connectivity and implicit choice preferences in rMDD patients may reflect a vulnerability factor for MDD.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
26

Broughan, Christine. "The effects of the olfactory properties of essential oils on selected aspects of cognition". Thesis, Coventry University, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.365208.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
27

Gu, Junhua. "Emotional design of smart pantry for mid-age women". Thesis, Available online, Georgia Institute of Technology, 2006, 2006. http://etd.gatech.edu/theses/available/etd-07052006-120502/.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
28

盧笑蓮 i Siu-lin Cola Lo. "The role of rumination in depression: differentiation of adaptive and maladaptive effects on mood andcognitions". Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2008. http://hub.hku.hk/bib/B41712560.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
29

Allen, Stephen Richard. "Concern processing in autonomous agents". Thesis, University of Birmingham, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.369169.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
30

Lo, Siu-lin Cola. "The role of rumination in depression differentiation of adaptive and maladaptive effects on mood and cognitions /". Click to view the E-thesis via HKUTO, 2008. http://sunzi.lib.hku.hk/hkuto/record/B41712560.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
31

Yeung, Siu-sze. "A study of the underlying process by which evaluative information influences self-evaluations /". Hong Kong : University of Hong Kong, 1999. http://sunzi.lib.hku.hk/hkuto/record.jsp?B21806299.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
32

Sunew, Emily Yamada. "Emotional intelligence in school-aged children : relations to early maternal depression and cognitive functioning /". Thesis, Connect to this title online; UW restricted, 2004. http://hdl.handle.net/1773/9051.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
33

Semrau, Penelope. "An analysis of cognitive theories in artificial intelligence and psychology in relation to the qualitative process of emotion /". The Ohio State University, 1987. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1487329662146988.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
34

Klein, Caronia. "It's not catharsis, it's cognition : a new approach to emotion in composition /". View online, 2010. http://repository.eiu.edu/theses/docs/32211131524472.pdf.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
35

Forsberg, Ekdal Linda. "Begreppslig validering : av ett ledarskapstest". Thesis, Luleå tekniska universitet, Institutionen för ekonomi, teknik och samhälle, 2018. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:ltu:diva-70845.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Ledin (2002) har skapat ett arbetspsykologiskt ledarskapstest där han använder sig av olika scenarion för att beskriva olika påståenden. Syftet med mitt arbete är att validera Ledins faktornamn genom att jämföra de faktornamn Ledin använt, med de faktornamn jag skapat genom en begreppsanalys. Finns det några likheter respektive skillnader mellan faktornamnen? Detta undersöktes med en kvalitativ enkät som besvarades av tio studenter. Försökspersonen fick ange ett gemensamt begrepp för ett antal punkter som berörde en ledares reaktioner i en störd mötessituation. Elva uppsättningar punkter med reaktioner skulle förses med varsitt begrepp. De föreslagna begreppen analyserades med hjälp av Svenska akademins ordbok för att finna den ursprungliga betydelsen. När begreppen sammanställts var ett faktornamn funnet. Tre faktornamn motsvarar kognitioner, tre faktornamn motsvarar emotioner och fem faktornamn motsvarar beteenden. Ledins och mina faktornamn har liknande betydelser, skillnader gäller hur smala betydelserna är och de påverkas även av att en betydelse beror av situation och miljö. Därmed är Ledins (2002) faktornamn validerade.
Ledin (2002) has created a work psychological leadership test where he uses different scenarios to explain different statement. The purpose of my work is to validate Ledin’s factor names by comparing the factor names used by Ledin with those created by me through a concept analysis. Are there any similarity or difference between the factor names? This was examined with a qualitative questionnaire that was filled in by ten students. The subject was asked to suggest a common concept for a number of statements concerning leader’s in a disturbed meeting situation. Concepts were assigned to eleven sets with reactions. The concepts were analysed with Svenska akademins ordbok in order to identify the original definitions. When the concepts had been compiled a factor name was specified. Three factor names correspond to cognitions, three correspond to emotions and five factor names correspond to behavior. Ledin’s and my factor names have similar meanings, the differences have to do with how narrow the meanings are and they are also affected by the impact on a meaning from the situation and context. Thus, Ledin’s (2002) factor names are validated.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
36

Wallin-Ruschman, Jennifer. "A Girl Power Study: Looking and Listening to the Role of Emotions and Relationality in Developing Critical Consciousness". PDXScholar, 2014. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/1837.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
The concept of critical consciousness centers on the capacity for involvement in social change efforts. Its development has been the aim of many recent social movements (e.g., the consciousness raising groups of the women's movement). In this work, critical consciousness is defined as the highest level of socio-political-cultural (SPC) consciousness development. SPC consciousness is characterized by the linking of the personal and the political so that structures and discourses of oppression are not only understood but also lead to critical action and transforming relations of domination. Additionally, critical consciousness includes the ability to tolerate ambivalence and conflict as well as the capacity to form group identifications that support critical reflection. While critical consciousness can develop in a variety of settings, it has a historical affinity with liberation education projects, particularly education projects that combine Critical Pedagogy and community engaged learning. Empirical inquiry on critical consciousness development is extremely limited. This dissertation addresses that gap, focusing specifically on the role of emotion and relationality in critical consciousness development. Further, the study offers a feminist critique of the literature, addressing as well the contribution of Community Psychology to conceptualizing critical consciousness. This dissertation analyzes data gathered through the Girl Power Senior Capstone, a course routinely taught at an urban Pacific Northwest public university. The six-hour course lasts for one quarter-term and integrates classroom time with community engagement. A central aim of the course is the development of critical consciousness. Specifically, the research was designed to address the following questions: 1) How are emotionally and relationally significant Girl Power experiences related to SPC consciousness development? 2) What tensions arise between the dominant culture and/or significant others' values and the values of the Girl Power capstone and how do these tensions move individuals toward or away from critical consciousness? The theoretical framework and interview schedule were guided by participant observation of the Girl Power course conducted over an academic term. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with all consenting and available capstone participants (N=10) in the course where participant-observations were carried out. The interviews were transcribed and analyzed based on a modified version of Carol Gilligan's Listening Guide. Two primary themes emerged from the data analysis-- the processes of awakening and sources of dissonance. The first theme relates to the processes of transformation that participants undergo during and following the course. Participants discuss this process as coming to see the world in a new way though their emotional experiences and relations developed in the course. The second theme, sources of dissonance, addresses sources of conflict that emerge as participants undergo this process of awakening. Areas of tension that were particularly salient centered on relationships and experiences in the course. Participants identified experiences in the course that they perceived as contributing in key respects to SPC consciousness. Yet some aspects of change in the course seemed to reflect limiting capacities, including magical thinking, a limited range of critical action strategies, and lack of critical community post Girl Power. The findings from the dissertation can be used to inform the creation and implementation of future projects of critical consciousness development and social justice work more broadly.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
37

Cassano, Michael. "A Break from the Norm: Parental Emotion Regulation, Expectancy Violations, and Gender in the Parental Socialization of Sadness Regulation in Childhood". Fogler Library, University of Maine, 2008. http://www.library.umaine.edu/theses/pdf/CassanoM2008.pdf.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
38

Abrahamyan, Arman. "Attentional capture of emotional static and dynamic hand gestures and faces : the effect of valence in a novel stroop-based paradigm". Thesis, View thesis, 2008. http://handle.uws.edu.au:8081/1959.7/45703.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Task-irrelevant static emotional faces, pictures, and words are known to involuntarily receive or capture attention. Hand gestures, similar to faces, are biological stimuli that are often used to express emotion. This thesis examines attentional capture using static emotional hand gestures and dynamic emotional hand gestures and faces. The component processes model of emotion predicts that either positive or negative stimuli that are appraised as relevant will capture attention for further and deeper processing. Attentional capture and the resulting competition for resources to process emotional stimuli can cause interference with an ongoing implicit cognitive task. In a series of experiments, participants identified the gender of hand or face stimuli while neutral, positive, and negative valence of stimuli was manipulated. It was hypothesised that interference resulting from attentional capture would lead to greater gender identification accuracy for static neutral hand gestures than for static positive or negative hand gestures. Neutral and emotional static faces were used as comparison stimuli. The hypothesis was tested in six experiments using a novel Stroop-based experimental paradigm, specifically devised for future implementation in Magnetoencephalography (MEG) environment. Stimulus items were presented to the peripheral visual field in one of four quadrants for 100 ms (Experiments 1 and 4), 200 ms (Experiment 2), and 300 ms (Experiment 3) each, in trials of eight items with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) that varied from 1800 to 2300 ms. As hypothesised, accuracy of identifying neutral hand gestures was significantly greater than accuracy of identifying gender of positive or negative hand gestures. The hypothesis was also supported for the case of negative faces but not for positive faces. The lack of an effect in response to positive faces is possibly due to the valence ambiguity of neutral faces capturing attention and interfering with gender identification. Experiment 5 examined attentional capture using emotional static hand gestures and faces presented to the central visual field. Only positive hand gestures and positive faces captured attention in this experiment. Experiment 6 tested two hypotheses using dynamic stimuli presented in the peripheral visual field for 300 ms. The hypothesis that greater gender identification accuracy would be found for neutral hand gestures/faces than for positive or negative hand gestures/faces was supported for positive hand gestures and negative faces, but not for negative hand gestures and positive faces. A second hypothesis was that greater attentional capture and resulting interference from dynamic stimuli would lead to poorer gender identification accuracy for emotional dynamic stimuli than for emotional static stimuli. The results of Experiment 6 did not support this hypothesis. These six experiments demonstrate that both static and dynamic positive and negative hand gestures and dynamic negative faces capture attention and interfere with an ongoing cognitive task when presented to the peripheral visual field. The experiments also demonstrate that static positive hand gestures and static positive faces capture attention when presented to the central visual field. Moreover, the novel Stroop-based experimental design appears to be a useful method for studying attentional capture using emotional pictorial stimuli and has already been implemented in a companion MEG experiment. A model explaining attentional capture and interference using emotional stimuli is proposed, based on the component processes model of emotion. In the model, the degree of attention recruited during the primary appraisal is proportional to the degree of interference with an implicit cognitive task.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
39

Senesac, Erin. "Cognitive depletion in emotion regulation: age differences depend on regulation strategy". Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/34837.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Recent work has suggested that emotion regulation of inner emotional experience requires fewer cognitive resources for older adults than for young adults (Scheibe&Blanchard-Fields, 2009). The present study investigated whether cognitive costs are reduced for various types of emotion regulation strategies or only for certain types. The suppression of emotional expression, for example, is a particularly costly strategy for young adults, but little information exists regarding its cognitive costs for older adults. Furthermore, suppression of emotional expression is not a strategy that older adults are likely to use or that they become more effective at using. By contrast, the regulation of inner emotional experience has been shown to be more effective in older adults and presents less of a cognitive cost. The present study examined the cognitive costs of regulation of inner emotional experience (to conceptually replicate previous findings) and the cognitive costs of suppression of the outer expression of emotion. The results suggest that regulating and suppressing emotions do not require the same degree of resources for older and young adults. Whereas older adults may require more resources to suppress expression of emotions than to regulate emotions, young adults appear to require more resources to regulate emotions than to suppress the expression of emotions.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
40

Abrahamyan, Arman. "Attentional capture of emotional static and dynamic hand gestures and faces the effect of valence in a novel stroop-based paradigm /". View thesis, 2008. http://handle.uws.edu.au:8081/1959.7/45703.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--University of Western Sydney, 2008.
A thesis presented to the University of Western Sydney, College of Arts, MARCS Auditory Laboratories, in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Includes bibliographies.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
41

Durso, Geoffrey Royce Oates. "Social Judgments of Others’ Emotions Versus Their Traits as a Function of Expectations". The Ohio State University, 2018. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1531417615156007.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
42

Carlsson, Katrina. "Prior knowledge and present events in the brain /". Stockholm, 2005. http://diss.kib.ki.se/2005/91-7140-429-5/.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
43

Lundqvist, Daniel. "The face of wrath : how facial attention captures visual attention /". Stockholm, 2003. http://diss.kib.ki.se/2003/91-7349-556-5.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
44

Rich, Shannon Scott. "The role of restraint and disinhibition in the processing of threatening stimuli : a cognitive information processing approach /". Thesis, Connect to Dissertations & Theses @ Tufts University, 2000.

Znajdź pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Tufts University, 2000.
Adviser: Holly Taylor. Submitted to the Dept. of Psychology. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 109-136). Access restricted to members of the Tufts University community. Also available via the World Wide Web;
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
45

Bolen, Kimberly Rose. "The effect of mood on decision-making : a role for personal experience /". Electronic version (PDF), 2007. http://dl.uncw.edu/etd/2007-3/bolenk/kimberlybolen.pdf.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
46

Leung, Siu Yuk. "The association of emotional intensity and high ability /". Thesis, McGill University, 1997. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=35365.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
This study was undertaken in an attempt to assess the Affect Intensity Measure (AIM) as a simpler alternative to the Overexcitability Questionnaire (OEQ) as a measure of emotional intensity in high ability young people. Participants were 30 young adolescents from grade 6 to grade 11 from the McGill Summer "Explorations" Program for the gifted, 75 undergraduate students and 46 doctoral students from various departments of McGill University. The Affect Intensity Measure was administrated to all three groups. There were no affect-intensity differences among the three groups of participants. Affect Intensity Measures particularly failed to distinguish gifted and nongifted groups. This result was inconsistent with previous studies using Overexcitability Questionnaire. There were gender differences but no age differences in affect intensity. The gender differences result was also inconsistent with the findings in several earlier studies using Overexcitability Questionnaire in which no gender differences in overexcitability were found among the gifted. Reasons why the AIM was not found to be an adequate substitute for the OEQ are explored, with the present samples, and possibly in general.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
47

Yip, Tin-hang James. "Emotion recognition in patients with Parkinson's disease : contribution of the substantia nigra /". Hong Kong : University of Hong Kong, 2002. http://sunzi.lib.hku.hk/hkuto/record.jsp?B24873007.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
48

Abdelfattah, Nadya. "“THE DEEPEST BLUSH”: BODILY STATES OF EMOTIONS IN JANE AUSTEN’S NOVELS". Cleveland State University / OhioLINK, 2018. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=csu1533837779817506.

Pełny tekst źródła
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
49

Raykos, Bronwyn C. "Attentional and interpretive biases : independent dimensions of individual difference or expressions of a common selective processing mechanism?" University of Western Australia. School of Psychology, 2007. http://theses.library.uwa.edu.au/adt-WU2007.0018.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
[Truncated abstract] Attentional and interpretive biases are important dimensions of individual difference that have been implicated in the etiology and maintenance of a range of clinical problems. Yet there has been no systematic investigation into the relationship between these dimensions of individual difference. The current research program tested predictions derived from two competing theoretical accounts of the relationship between attentional and interpretive biases. The Common Mechanism Account proposes that cognitive biases represent concurrent manifestations of a single underlying selective processing mechanism. The Independent Mechanism account proposes that independent mechanisms underlie each bias. . . An apparent contradiction is that the manipulation of one bias served to also modify the other bias, despite the observation that the magnitude of the resulting change in both biases was uncorrelated. Neither the Common Mechanism nor the Independent Pathways accounts can adequately explain this pattern of results. A new account is proposed, in which attentional and interpretive biases are viewed as representing mechanisms that are related but that are not the same. Theoretical and applied implications of these findings are discussed, including the possibility that the two biases each may best predict emotional reactions to quite different stressful events and that training programs designed to attenuate allocation of attentional resources to threat may serve to reduce both attentional and interpretive selectivity in emotionally vulnerable individuals.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
50

Ryan, Melissa-Sue, i n/a. "Ageing and emotion : categorisation, recognition, and social understanding". University of Otago. Department of Psychology, 2009. http://adt.otago.ac.nz./public/adt-NZDU20090309.150008.

Pełny tekst źródła
Streszczenie:
The present thesis investigated age differences in emotion recognition skills of 146 older adults (age range 60-92 years) and 146 young adults (age range 18-25 years) in four experiments. Experiment 1 assessed participants� ability to categorise facial expressions of sadness, fear, happiness, and surprise. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants were asked to identify six emotions (happiness, sadness, surprise, fear, anger, disgust) from still and dynamic faces, alone and in combination with vocal expressions. Finally, Experiment 4 compared performance on these standard emotion recognition paradigms to that of more ecologically-valid measures; the Faux Pas and Verbosity and Social Cues Tasks. Across the four studies, there was evidence of an age-related decline in emotion recognition skills. Older adults were overall less sensitive to perceptual differences between faces in Experiment 1 and showed a loss of categorical perception effect for fearful faces. Older adults were less accurate than young adults at recognising expressions of sadness, anger, and fear, across types of expression (voices and faces). There were some differences across modalities, with older adults showing difficulties with fear recognition for faces, but not voices, and difficulty in matching happy voices to happy faces but not for happy voices and faces presented in isolation. Experiment 2 also showed that the majority of older adult participants had some decline in emotion recognition skills. Age differences in performance were also apparent on the more ecologically-valid measures. Older adults were more likely than young adults to rate the protagonist as behaving inappropriately in the Faux Pas Task, even with the control videos, suggesting difficulty in discriminating faux pas. Older adults were also judged to be more verbose and to offer more off-topic information during the Verbosity Task than young adults and were less likely to recognise expressions of boredom in the Social Cues Task. These findings are discussed in terms of three theoretical accounts. A positivity bias (indicating increased recognition and experience of positive emotions and reduction for negative emotions) was not consistent with the older adults� difficulties with matching happy faces to voices and relatively preserved performance with disgusted expressions. Age-related decline in cognitive processes did not account for the specific pattern of age differences observed. The most plausible explanation for the age differences in the present thesis is that age-related neurological changes in the brain areas that process emotions, specifically the temporal and frontal areas, are likely to contribute to the older adults� declines in performance on emotion categorisation, emotion recognition, and social cognition tasks. The implications for everyday social interactions for older adults are also discussed.
Style APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO itp.
Oferujemy zniżki na wszystkie plany premium dla autorów, których prace zostały uwzględnione w tematycznych zestawieniach literatury. Skontaktuj się z nami, aby uzyskać unikalny kod promocyjny!

Do bibliografii