Tesi sul tema "Fear conditioning"
Cita una fonte nei formati APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard e in molti altri stili
Vedi i top-50 saggi (tesi di laurea o di dottorato) per l'attività di ricerca sul tema "Fear conditioning".
Accanto a ogni fonte nell'elenco di riferimenti c'è un pulsante "Aggiungi alla bibliografia". Premilo e genereremo automaticamente la citazione bibliografica dell'opera scelta nello stile citazionale di cui hai bisogno: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver ecc.
Puoi anche scaricare il testo completo della pubblicazione scientifica nel formato .pdf e leggere online l'abstract (il sommario) dell'opera se è presente nei metadati.
Vedi le tesi di molte aree scientifiche e compila una bibliografia corretta.
Urcelay, Gonzalo Pablo. "Potentiation and overshadowing in Pavlovian fear conditioning". Diss., Online access via UMI:, 2008.
Cerca il testo completoHakim, Marziah. "Neurogenesis and neuroplasticity following olfactory fear conditioning". Thesis, Queensland University of Technology, 2019. https://eprints.qut.edu.au/157474/1/Marziah_Hakim_Thesis.pdf.
Testo completoHolahan, Matthew R. "Amygdala involvement in aversive conditioning". Thesis, McGill University, 2003. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=19529.
Testo completoAntoniadis, Elena Anna. "Discriminative fear conditioning to context expressed by multiple measures of fear in the rat". Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1997. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/ftp01/MQ29181.pdf.
Testo completoTinoco, González Daniella. "Fear conditioning to socially relevant stimuli in social anxiety". Doctoral thesis, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/120553.
Testo completoAnxiety disorders represent a challenge for psychiatry and clinical psychology. Near 30 % of the population suffers, or has suffered, one or more anxiety disorder along his life, being this disorder the most frequent group of them inside the DSM-IV. The theoretical approximations based on aversive learning models have occupied traditionally a very important place among the etiological models of these disorders. Despite the fact that fear conditioning is an adaptative process of great importance for survival, it can turn into clinical relevant when the reactivity to the Conditioned Stimulus (CS) persists in absence of contingency between the CS and the Unconditioned Stimulus (US). By means of classical fear conditioning processes, an anxiety disorder could appear during or after a traumatic event or in a period of significant stress. Nevertheless, not all the persons exposed to this type of events end up developing a disorder. Some studies have demonstrated that patients with anxiety disorders are characterized by a high conditionability and resistance to extinction in anxiety patients compared to healthy controls suggesting that patients are characterized by en enhanced conditioning and that this is one of the reasons for which, in situations of exhibition to aversive incidents, only some individuals go on to develop pathological fears, whereas others show an adaptative response of fear. Many of these studies have use the startle reflex as an index of emotional activation. It consists of a defensive sudden response that many animal species present in presence of an intense and unexpected stimulus. In humans, it can be measured very simply by registering the electromyographic response in the orbicularis oculi muscle. The increase of the startle reflex when an individual is experiencing fear or anxiety is named fear potentiated startle. During the last years, it has been converted into a very useful tool for traslational investigation of anxiety disorders. Up to recent dates, most of the published studies with humans using classical conditioning paradigms have used evolutionarily “unprepared” stimuli to be conditioned, and many have not demonstrated an enhanced conditioning in anxiety patients. This alerts us of the importance to use paradigms that take into account unconditioned stimuli relevant to the disorder object of study. The overall goal of the present dissertation was to investigate fear conditioning processes in social anxiety using the fear potentiated startle in patients with social anxiety compared to patients with panic disorder with agoraphobia and healthy controls. To address this goal, we used a novel paradigm developed by Lissek et al. (2008) in which neutral facial expressions from three female actors served as the CS and were paired with one of three types audiovisual stimuli: insults and critical facial expressions (USneg); comments and neutral facial expressions (USneu); and compliments and positive facial expressions (USpos). Our results did not demonstrate an enhanced conditioning among patients with social anxiety compared to patients with panic disorder with agoraphobia and healthy controls. It is plausible that other associative (e.g. fear extinction) and non-associative processes (e.g cognitive and attentional processes) play a greater role in explaining social anxiety rather than enhanced fear conditioning.
Hardwick, Sascha. "Startle modification during human fear conditioning : attention or emotion? /". [St. Lucia, Qld.], 2005. http://www.library.uq.edu.au/pdfserve.php?image=thesisabs/absthe19287.pdf.
Testo completoAngelhuber, Martin. "The neural circuitry of fear conditioning : a theoretical account". Thesis, Strasbourg, 2016. http://www.theses.fr/2016STRAJ082/document.
Testo completoFear conditioning is a successful paradigm for studying neural substrates of emotional learning. In this thesis, two computational models of the underlying neural circuitry are presented. First, the effects of changes in neuronal membrane conductance on input processing are analyzed in a biologically realistic model. We show that changes in tonic inhibitory conductance increase the responsiveness of the network to inputs. Then, the model is analyzed from a functional perspective and predictions that follow from this proposition are discussed. Next, a systems level model is presented based on a recent high-level approach to conditioning. It is proposed that the interaction between fear and extinction neurons in the basal amygdala is a neural substrate of the switching between latent states, allowing the animal to infer causal structure. Important behavioral and physiological results are reproduced and predictions and questions that follow from the main hypothesis are considered
Garfield, Joshua Benjamin Bernard Psychology Faculty of Science UNSW. "FG7142 attenuates expression of overexpectation in Pavlovian fear conditioning". Publisher:University of New South Wales. Psychology, 2008. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/43241.
Testo completoMerckelbach, Harald Lodewijk Gerardus Joseph. "Preparedness and classical conditioning of fear a critical inquiry /". [Maastricht : Maastricht : Rijksuniversiteit Limburg] ; University Library, Maastricht University [Host], 1989. http://arno.unimaas.nl/show.cgi?fid=5450.
Testo completoMcguire, Joseph F. "Fear Conditioning and Extinction in Childhood Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder". Scholar Commons, 2015. http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/5741.
Testo completoAntunes, Raquel A. G. "Neural mechanisms of stimulus generalization in auditory fear conditioning". Doctoral thesis, Universidade Nova de Lisboa. Instituto de Tecnologia Química e Biológica, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10362/6175.
Testo completoFear is a physiological trait with a strong weight on survival and adaptation. Great progress has been made to understand the mechanisms of fear learning, mainly using auditory fear conditioning (AFC). In this behavioral paradigm, an initial neutral tone (conditioned stimulus, CS) acquires aversive predictive properties after successive pairings with a footshock (unconditioned stimulus, US) and comes to elicit responses characteristically elicited by threatening stimuli. In this behavioral paradigm, the amygdala has been identified has a key neural substrate for associative fear learning, and the site where unconditioned stimuli (US) and conditioned (CS) auditory stimuli come to be associated. Auditory information may reach the amygdala either directly from the auditory thalamus or indirectly via thalamo-cortico-amygdala projections. The “high route/low route” hypothesis has thus been proposed, which claims that the cortical pathway (“high route”) is crucial for discrimination between fearful and neutral sounds, while the direct thalamic pathway (“low route”) provides a rapid but less accurate relay of auditory information to the amygdala. This hypothesis relies on the assumption that more complex processing requires cortical activity and that thalamic relay is faster then cortical transmission to the amygdala. The present work essentially aims at putting to test this largely accepted hypothesis.Auditory fear conditioning was used as the behavioral paradigm to unravel the possible functional explanation for the coexistence of two parallel auditory pathways converging into the amygdala, and the high route/low route hypothesis was the working model for the identification of neuronal substrates of auditory discrimination.(...)
This work was supported by Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (Grant SFRH/BD/27500/2006).
Ryan, Katherine M. "Methodological differences in Pavlovian fear learning, extinction and return of fear". Thesis, Griffith University, 2021. http://hdl.handle.net/10072/410147.
Testo completoThesis (PhD Doctorate)
Doctor of Philosophy (PhD)
School of Applied Psychology
Griffith Health
Full Text
Conceição, Luiz Henrique Santana. "Sleep modifications after contextual fear conditioning and extinction in rats". reponame:Repositório Institucional da UFABC, 2016.
Cerca il testo completoDissertação (mestrado) - Universidade Federal do ABC, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Neurociência e Cognição, 2016.
Memórias de extinção são um produto das variações nas condições de condicionamento e na quantidade de tempo e sessões de extinção. Aumentos de sono paradoxal após a exposição a sessões de extinção foram descritas anteriormente, contudo trabalhos anteriores não testaram mais de um dia de extinção e tão pouco testaram se a modificação do sono após a extinção dependeria do intervalo de tempo entre condicionamento e extinção. Nós exploramos modificações da arquitetura do sono em diferentes condições de aprendizagem a extinção do medo condicionado. Em primeiro lugar, usamos uma tarefa de condicionamento de medo ao contexto (CMC) a fim de explorar o efeito de um evento aversivo (o choque elétrico) e um possível efeito do intervalo de tempo entre a sessão condicionamento e a sessão de extinção no sono e comportamento. O primeiro grupo, chamado Extinção Múltipla recebeu um treino de CMC com uma apresentação de choque único seguido por cinco sessões de extinção. O segundo grupo, chamado extinção única, foi treinado no CMC e expostos à extinção sete dias após este treino. O terceiro grupo - chamado choque imediato - recebeu uma sessão de treinamento com um único choque aplicado imediatamente depois de entrar na caixa de condicionamento e seguiu o mesmo protocolo de extinção que o grupo de extinção múltipla. A resposta de congelamento foi o parâmetro comportamental analisado. Informações sobre sono-vigília foram registradas através da coleta de dados de ECOG e EMG e classificado entre três fases: vigília, sono de ondas lentas e sono paradoxal. Os resultados mostraram aumento do sono de ondas lentas após CMC e aumento do sono paradoxal depois de CMC e extinção entre os grupos T-múltipla e T-única. Nossas descobertas apoiam achados anteriores sobre a relação entre sono paradoxal e aprendizagem da extinção e sugerem que modificações de sono de ondas lentas para extinção antecipada sejam dependentes do tempo.
Extinction memory is a product of variations in fear conditioning and fear extinction procedure and the amount of time and sessions of extinction. Increases in paradoxical sleep (PS) after exposure to extinction sessions was previously described; however, previous works did not test more than one day of extinction and did not test whether sleep modifications after extinction are dependent upon the time interval between conditioning and extinction. We explored sleep architecture modifications on different conditions of conditioned fear extinction learning. We first adapted a contextual fear-conditioning task in order to explore the effect of an aversive event (the electric shock) and a possible effect of time interval between conditioning and extinction session on sleep and behaviour. The first group, named Multiple Extinction (T-10 Multiple) received a contextual fear conditioning (CFC) training with a single shock presentation followed by five sessions of extinction. The second group, named single extinction (T-Single), was trained in the same CFC procedure and exposed to one single extinction session, seven days after training. The third group ¿ named immediate shock ¿ received a training 1session with a single shock applied immediately after entering the conditioning box following the same protocol of extinction as the T-Multiple group. The freezing response was the behavioural parameter analysed. Sleep-wake information was recorded by collecting electrocorticogram (ECOG) and electromyogram (EMG) data and scored as one between three phases: awake, slow wave sleep (SWS) and PS. Results showed that SWS increased after CFC, and it also showed that PS increased after CFC and extinction for either T-Multiple and T- Single group. Our findings support previous findings on PS relation with extinction learning and suggest some time-dependent SWS modification for early extinction re-exposure. The discovery of the participation of PS in contextual fear extinction and SWS role on nuances of extinction procedure expands the understanding of behaviour and sleep relations and, at the same time, offer a behavioural model to study sleep dependent stressful memory related to PTSD or HPA axis without the unconditioned behavioural and physiological effects of ES.
Cullen, Patrick Kennedy. "NEUROBIOLOGICAL MECHANISMS OF FEAR GENERALIZATION". Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1374536919.
Testo completoKunkel, Rebecca Ann. "Can Positive Reinforcement Overcome Fear? An Investigation of Competing Contingencies". Thesis, University of North Texas, 2011. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc84232/.
Testo completoLewis, Michael. "Fear Conditioning as an Intermediate Phenotype: An RDoC Inspired Methodological Analysis". Thesis, Virginia Tech, 2018. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/83837.
Testo completoMaster of Science
To date, most clinical psychology research has been based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), which is a catalog of mental health disorders that was originally designed to facilitate communication among clinicians. Many experts contend that this approach has hampered progress in the field of biological clinical psychology research. Thus, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) created a new template for biological clinical psychology research called the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). Since RDoC calls for a complete overhaul in the conceptualization of clinical dysfunction, this approach requires statistical and experimental innovation. One traditional experimental approach that may be helpful in understanding the RDoC topic of acute threat (“fear”) is called Pavlovian Fear Learning (PFL). However, traditional PFL studies have utilized statistical methods that are based on comparing group averages and require researchers to determine groups of interest based on theory before the study begins. This is problematic because RDoC calls for research that begins with evidence rather than theory. Growth Mixture Modeling (GMM) is a statistical methodology that may allow researchers to analyze fear learning data without having to begin with theoretically determined categories such as DSM disorders. However, little research has tested how well this approach would work. This study is just the second to apply a GMM approach to a human PFL study. The findings from this investigation may inform efforts to develop a statistical technique that is well suited for RDoCian research and may also spur innovation in psychotherapeutic and psychopharmacological treatment.
Kochli, Daniel. "The amygdala is critical for trace, delay, and contextual fear conditioning". Miami University / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=miami1406560508.
Testo completoIsaacs, Sofie. "The Roles of the Amygdala and the Hippocampus in Fear Conditioning". Thesis, Högskolan i Skövde, Institutionen för biovetenskap, 2015. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:his:diva-11284.
Testo completoIberico, Carlos, Debora Vansteenwegen, Bram Vervliet e Dirk Hermans. "The effect of (un)predictability on contextual fear: A replication of the basic findings". Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú, 2012. http://repositorio.pucp.edu.pe/index/handle/123456789/102314.
Testo completoEl objetivo de este estudio fue investigar el rol de la (im)predictabilidad en el paradigma de condicionamiento de miedo, al analizar las diferencias tanto en el condicionamiento específico (cue) como en el contextual. Por consiguiente, se manipuló la presentación del estímulo incondicionado (EI) utilizando dos condiciones: una presentación apareada del EI con el estímulo condicionado —EC en adelante— (predecible) y una presentación no apareada del EI (impredecible). Se manipuló el contexto utilizando la luz central del cuarto experimental: condiciones de oscuridad y claridad. Las variables independientes fueron la respuesta de conductancia de la piel y la medición de la respuesta de sobresalto. Los participantes fueron 65 alumnos de primer año de psicología. Los resultados muestran más condicionamiento contextual en el bloque no apareado (impredecible) comparado con el apareado, y condicionamiento específico en el bloque apareado (predecible).
Björkstrand, Johannes. "The Amygdala, Fear and Reconsolidation : Neural and Behavioral Effects of Retrieval-Extinction in Fear Conditioning and Spider Phobia". Doctoral thesis, Uppsala universitet, Institutionen för psykologi, 2017. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-317866.
Testo completoRay, Madelyn. "TheRole of the Nucleus Accumbens Core in Scaling Fear to Degree of Threat:". Thesis, Boston College, 2021. http://hdl.handle.net/2345/bc-ir:109068.
Testo completoIdentifying the neural circuits underlying adaptive fear is fundamental to understanding and developing more effective treatments for anxiety disorders. Adaptive behavior requires fear to scale to the level of threat and dysfunction in this capacity is a hallmark of fear-related anxiety disorders. Identifying the neural circuits underlying adaptive fear is fundamental to understanding anxiety disorders and propelling more effective treatments for patients. Fear is adaptive when the level of the response rapidly scales to degree of threat. Using a discrimination procedure consisting of danger, uncertainty, and safety cues, our laboratory has found rapid fear scaling (within 2 s of cue presentation). However, the neural underpinnings of this behavior are unknown. The overarching goal of this dissertation is to examine a role for the nucleus accumbens core (NAcc) in scaling fear to degree of threat. In three experiments I used neurotoxic lesions, optogenetic inhibition, and in vivo electrophysiology combined with an intricate fear learning procedure to elucidate a role for the NAcc in both general and rapid scaling of fear. Permanent NAcc dysfunction, via neurotoxic lesion, generally disrupted the ability to scale fear to degree of threat and specifically impaired one component of scaling: rapid discrimination of uncertain threat and safety. Reversible NAcc dysfunction, via optogenetic inhibition, specifically impaired rapid discrimination of uncertain threat and safety. Further, I demonstrated that NAcc activity is threat responsive and exhibits heterogeneity in the timing and specific nature of threat firing. The results reveal that the NAcc is essential to scale fear to degree of threat and responds to threat cues across both rapid and general timescales. Taken together, the results reveal a novel role for the NAcc in scaling fear and identify it as a plausible source of dysfunction in stress and anxiety disorders. Identifying the brain regions underlying adaptive fear is fundamental to understanding and developing more effective treatments for anxiety disorders
Thesis (PhD) — Boston College, 2021
Submitted to: Boston College. Graduate School of Arts and Sciences
Discipline: Psychology
Ågren, Thomas. "Erasing Fear : Effect of Disrupting Fear Memory Reconsolidation on Central and Peripheral Nervous System Activity". Doctoral thesis, Uppsala universitet, Institutionen för psykologi, 2012. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-180202.
Testo completoLeung, Hiu Tin Psychology Faculty of Science UNSW. "Spontaneous recovery in Pavlovian fear extinction and latent inhibition". Publisher:University of New South Wales. Psychology, 2009. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/43701.
Testo completoTop, Jr David Nicholas. "A fMRI of Fear Conditioning and Auditory Looming in Autism Spectrum Disorder". BYU ScholarsArchive, 2020. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/8599.
Testo completoUlmen, Adam Richard. "The NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 renders pavlovian fear conditioning state-dependent". Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1430140456.
Testo completoBellafiore, Amber. "Fear Conditioning: Using the Scream of a Woman's Voice: A Pilot Study". Thesis, The University of Arizona, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/144176.
Testo completoDiggs, Herman Augustus. "EFFECTS OF ACUTE THC ADMINISTRATION ON EXTINCTION OF CONDITIONED FEAR RESPONSES IN HUMANS: A FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF HIGH DENSITY EEG". OpenSIUC, 2014. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/dissertations/947.
Testo completoWalker, Rachel Ann. "Neural Mechanisms of Aversive Prediction Errors:". Thesis, Boston College, 2020. http://hdl.handle.net/2345/bc-ir:109027.
Testo completoUncertainty is a pervasive facet of life, and responding appropriately and proportionally to uncertain threats is critical for adaptive behavior. Aversive prediction errors are signals that allow for appropriate fear responses, especially in the face of uncertainty, and provide a critical updating mechanism to adapt to change. Positive prediction errors (+PE) are generated when an actual outcome of an event is worse than the predicted outcome and increase fear upon future encounters with the related predictive cue. Negative prediction errors (-PE) are generated when the predicted outcome is worse than the actual outcome and decrease fear upon future encounters with the related predictive cue. While some regions have been offered as the neural source of positive and negative prediction errors, no causal evidence has been able to identify their sources of generation. The objective of this dissertation was to causally identify the neural basis of aversive prediction error signaling. Using precise neural manipulations paired with a robust behavioral fear discrimination task, I present causal evidence for vlPAG generation of +PEs and for a ventrolateral periaqueductal grey (vlPAG) to medial central amygdala (CeM) pathway to carry out +PE fear updating. Further, I demonstrate that while dorsal raphe serotonergic neurons are not the source of -PE generation, they appear to receive and utilize this signal. Understanding the neural network responsible for aversive prediction error signaling will not only inform understanding of the neurological basis of fear but also may provide insights into disorders, such as PTSD and anxiety disorders, that are characterized by excessive/inappropriate fear responses
Thesis (PhD) — Boston College, 2020
Submitted to: Boston College. Graduate School of Arts and Sciences
Discipline: Psychology
Levillain, Mary Elizabeth. "Nicotine Facilitates Trace Fear Conditioning in Normal and FASD Rats Tested as Adolescents". W&M ScholarWorks, 2008. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539626575.
Testo completoChaaya, Nicholas. "Investigation of cellular and molecular mechanisms involved in contextual fear memory encoding". Thesis, Queensland University of Technology, 2019. https://eprints.qut.edu.au/134258/1/Nicholas_Chaaya_Thesis.pdf.
Testo completoRantavuori, K. (Kari). "Aspects and determinants of children’s dental fear". Doctoral thesis, University of Oulu, 2008. http://urn.fi/urn:isbn:9789514289439.
Testo completoTiivistelmä Tutkimuksen tarkoituksena oli selvittää lasten hammashoitopelon piirteitä ja niihin liittyviä seikkoja eri-ikäisillä lapsilla. Tutkimuksessa käytettiin havaintoaineistoa, joka koostui 378:sta 3–13-vuotiaasta italialaisesta lapsesta Veneton maakunnan alueelta sekä 1474:stä 3-, 6-, 9-, 12- ja 15-vuotiaasta lapsesta Jyväskylästä ja Kuopiosta. Tutkimuksessa italialaisilla lapsilla vanhempi täytti kyselylomakkeen, joka sisälsi kysymyksiä lapsen iästä, hammashoitopelosta, ensimmäisestä hammashoitokäynnistä ja seuraavien hoitokäyntien lukumäärästä sekä vanhemman omasta hammashoitopelosta. Suomalaisilla lapsilla tutkimustiedot kerättiin kyselylomakkeella, joka sisälsi 11 kysymystä lapsen hammashoitopelosta ja kysymyksiä lapsen suun terveystottumuksista sekä perheeseen liittyvistä seikoista, kuten perheenjäsenten hammashoitopelosta. Hampaiden senhetkinen kliininen tila sekä kolmen tarkastusta edeltävän vuoden hoitokäynnit ja tuolloin tehdyt toimenpiteet otettiin mukaan tutkimukseen. Suomalaisista lapsista 21–36 % pelkäsi jonkin verran tai paljon jotain asiaa hammashoidossa. Suomalaisten lasten hammashoitopelko ei ollut alempi nuoremmilla lapsilla vaan vaihteli ikäryhmien välillä. Myös hammashoitopelon luonne vaihteli ikäluokittain. Suomalaisesta kyselystä löydettiin neljä pelon osa-aluetta: paikkaushoitoon liittyvä pelko, hammaslääkärissä käymiseen liittyvä pelko, yleinen hammashoitopelko sekä voimakkaimmaksi koettu hammashoitoon liittyvä yksittäinen pelko. Tutkimuksessa tutkittiin pelon osa-alueiden välisiä korrelaatioita ikäryhmittäin, erikseen yksittäisen pelon osa-alueen vaihtelua ikäryhmien välillä sekä hammashoitopelon liittyviä seikkoja ikäryhmittäin. Nuoremmilla lapsilla hammashoitopelko oli useammin abstraktia, yleensä hammashoidossa käymiseen liittyvää pelkoa. Vanhemmilla lapsilla hammashoitopelko oli usein hammashoitotoimenpiteisiin liittyvää pelkoa, esimerkiksi puudutuksen ja porauksen pelkoa. Kivun pelko oli yleistä kaikissa ikäryhmissä. Ensimmäiset hammashoitokokemukset olivat voimakkaita hammashoitopelon selittäjiä italialaisilla lapsilla. Suomalaisten lasten hammashoitopelko oli vahvasti yhteydessä muiden perheenjäsenten hammashoitopelkoon. 15-vuotiaat tytöt pelkäsivät hammashoitoa enemmän kuin pojat, mutta sukupuolten välisiä eroja ei havaittu nuoremmissa ikäryhmissä. Tutkimus osoittaa, että lasten hammashoitopelko ei ole ainoastaan seurausta suorasta ehdollistumisesta hammashoitokokemusten kautta vaan siihen vaikuttavat enemmänkin lapseen, perheeseen ja ympäristöön liittyvät seikat
Diggs, Herman Augustus. "EFFECTS OF ACUTE THC ADMINISTRATION ON EXTINCTION OF CONDITIONED FEAR RESPONSES IN HUMANS". OpenSIUC, 2010. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/theses/305.
Testo completoKircher, Tilo, Volker Arolt, Andreas Jansen, Martin Pyka, Isabelle Reinhardt, Thilo Kellermann, Carsten Konrad et al. "Effect of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy on Neural Correlates of Fear Conditioning in Panic Disorder". Saechsische Landesbibliothek- Staats- und Universitaetsbibliothek Dresden, 2013. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:14-qucosa-120091.
Testo completoRands, Gabriella. "The effect of executive function on the conditioning and counter-coditioning of children's fear". Thesis, University of East Anglia, 2009. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.521023.
Testo completoKim, Jee Hyun Psychology Faculty of Science UNSW. "Extinction of conditioned fear in the developing rat". Publisher:University of New South Wales. Psychology, 2008. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/41106.
Testo completoDalrymple, Savannah. "Development of Ethologically-Based Inhibitory Avoidance Models of Fear Memory". Scholar Commons, 2017. http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/6823.
Testo completoRaybuck, Jonathan Dennis. "SPECIFIC EFFECTS OF NICOTINE AND NICOTINIC ANTAGONISTS ON TRACE AND CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING IN C57BL/6 MICE: A ROLE FOR NICOTINIC ACETYLCHOLINERGIC SIGNALING IN THE DORSAL HIPPOCAMPUS, VENTRAL HIPPOCAMPUS, AND MEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTEX IN TRACE FEAR CONDITIONING". Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2009. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/59884.
Testo completoPh.D.
Nicotine has been shown to enhance multiple forms of learning and memory. However the substrates through which these effects occur are not well understood. To examine the specific substrates of nicotine's acute effects on trace fear conditioning, I infused nicotine into areas thought to support trace fear conditioning, the dorsal hippocampus, ventral hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Additionally, we investigated the contributions of nicotinic acetylcholinergic signaling to trace fear conditioning by infusing the nicotinic antagonists dihydro-beta-erythroidine (DHbE) and methyllycaconitine (MLA) into these areas. Nicotine had different effects on both trace and contextual fear conditioning depending on dose and brain region, as did the nicotinic antagonists. In the dorsal hippocampus nicotine infusion enhanced both trace and contextual conditioning, although these effects were dissociable by dose and training protocol. Additionally, the high-affinity nicotinic antagonist DHbE produced selective deficits in trace conditioning, suggesting that while enhancement of nicotinic signaling can affect both contextual and trace learning, nicotinic activity in the dorsal hippocampus is critically involved in trace but not contextual conditioning. In the ventral hippocampus nicotine infusion produced deficits in both trace and contextual fear conditioning, without affecting delay conditioning, while the antagonists had no effect. This finding suggests that altered nicotinic signaling in the ventral hippocampus can suppress hippocampus dependent learning. In the mPFC nicotine selectively enhanced trace conditioning though both antagonists also enhanced trace fear conditioning. Unlike in the mPFC or dorsal hippocampus, where nicotine and antagonist induced effects occurred during training, effects in the ventral hippocampus occurred at both training and testing, suggesting that the ventral hippocampus may be able to modulate acquisition as well as expression of hippocampus dependent learning. Additionally, antagonist infusion into the mPFC during testing produced deficits in expression, suggesting that this area can modulate fear expression. Thus, the substrates of nicotinic acetylcholinergic contributions to trace and contextual fear conditioning are diverse. I put forth a multi-component model of these contributions, where trace fear conditioning is supported by dorsal hippocampus dependent maintenance of the CS during the trace interval, long-term storage in the mPFC and ventral hippocampal mediated acquisition and expression.
Temple University--Theses
Senn, Verena. "Differential activation of anatomically defined neuronal subpopulations in the amygdala during fear conditioning and extinction /". [S.l.] : [s.n.], 2009. http://edoc.unibas.ch/diss/DissB_8782.
Testo completoHalonen, Joshua D. "Predator-Based Fear Conditioning: A Novel Approach to the Study of the Neurobiology of Memory". Scholar Commons, 2012. http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/4062.
Testo completoPierson, Jamie L. "DORSAL HIPPOCAMPUS INFUSIONS OF CNQX INTO THE DENTATE GYRUS DISRUPT EXPRESSION OF TRACE FEAR CONDITIONING". Miami University / OhioLINK, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=miami1355165370.
Testo completoLueken, U., B. Straube, I. Reinhardt, N. I. Maslowski, H. U. Wittchen, A. Ströhle, A. Wittmann et al. "Altered top-down and bottom-up processing of fear conditioning in panic disorder with agoraphobia". Cambridge University Press, 2014. https://tud.qucosa.de/id/qucosa%3A39007.
Testo completoSchmeltzer, Sarah N. "The Role of Forebrain Neuropeptide Y in the Regulation and Development of PTSD-like Behaviors". University of Cincinnati / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ucin1479820132199223.
Testo completoYuzhe, Li. "Computational Investigations on Uncertainty-Dependent Extinction of Fear Memory". 京都大学 (Kyoto University), 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/2433/225756.
Testo completoGanev, Jennifer. "The Role of the Amygdala and Other Forebrain Structures in the Immediate Fear Arousal Produced by Footshock Exposure". Thesis, University of Canterbury. Psychology, 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/10092/1503.
Testo completoKitamura, Haruka. "Boundary conditions of acquisition and post-retrieval extinction in fear conditioning: Individual differences and methodological factors". Thesis, Queensland University of Technology, 2021. https://eprints.qut.edu.au/207826/2/Haruka%20Kitamura%20Thesis.pdf.
Testo completoCoffman, Marika Cerie. "Common and Distinct Neural Mechanisms of Fear Acquisition and Reversal in comorbid Autism with Social Anxiety and Social Anxiety Disorder uncomplicated by Autism". Diss., Virginia Tech, 2019. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/102409.
Testo completoDoctor of Philosophy
Vurbic, Drina. "Mechanisms of Secondary Extinction". ScholarWorks @ UVM, 2010. http://scholarworks.uvm.edu/graddis/237.
Testo completoSantos, Teresa P. G. "Tone-evoked Fos labeling in the central auditory pathway : effects of stimulus intensity and auditory fear conditioning". Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/37905.
Testo completoIncludes bibliographical references.
Understanding intensity coding and auditory learning are basic concerns of research on the auditory central pathway. There is no unifying model of intensity coding but several mechanisms have been proposed to play a role. The first aim of this thesis was to determine the mechanisms of intensity coding in the central auditory pathway from the cochlear nucleus to the auditory cortex. The Fos labeling method was used to assess neuronal activation in the central auditory system. This technique allows one to study large regions of the brain in awake animals. Increasing sound pressure level led to: (1) spreading of labeling towards neurons with higher best frequencies; (2) spread of labeling orthogonal to the tonotopic axis; (3) and increased density of labeling within the tonotopic band. In addition to encoding the physical features of a stimulus, it is fundamental for survival that we learn about the meaning of sounds and put them in a behavioral context. The second aim of this thesis was to study how learning, in particular auditory fear conditioning, changes the pattern of neuronal activation of neurons, as measured with Fos labeling, in the central nervous system. Conditioning led to an increase in Fos labeling in central auditory nuclei.
(cont.) This increase in labeling was similar to the effects of increasing sound intensity. The present results support the idea that auditory fear memories are stored in the auditory pathway.
by Teresa P.G. Santos.
Ph.D.
Portugal, George Sussman. "Strain-dependent and age-dependent effects of acute, chronic, and withdrawal from chronic nicotine on fear conditioning". Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2010. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/111566.
Testo completoPh.D.
Research in both humans and animals demonstrates that nicotine addiction is a complex disorder that can be influenced by several factors. For instance, individual differences in genetics can impact sensitivity to nicotine and can modulate the severity of nicotine withdrawal. Although nicotine alters cognitive processes such as learning and memory, it remains unknown whether genetic variability modulates the effects of nicotine on these cognitive functions. Thus, the present study characterized the effects of acute, chronic, and withdrawal from chronic nicotine administration on fear conditioning in 8 strains of inbred mice. Furthermore, nicotine withdrawal-related changes in somatic signs and the elevated plus maze were examined because nicotine withdrawal consists of multiple symptoms that can include increased somatic signs and increased anxiety. Strain-dependent effects of acute nicotine and nicotine withdrawal on contextual fear conditioning were observed in several inbred strains. However, the effects of acute nicotine on contextual fear conditioning were not associated with the effects of nicotine withdrawal, suggesting that different genetic substrates may mediate these two effects. Nicotine withdrawal produced few changes in somatic signs and exploration in the elevated plus maze. Overall, these data demonstrate that genetics contribute to variability in the effects of acute nicotine and withdrawal from chronic nicotine treatment on contextual fear conditioning. The identification of genes that may alter the effects of nicotine on cognition may lead to more efficacious treatments for nicotine addiction. The age during which nicotine use begins is a second factor that may influence the severity of nicotine addiction. Pre-adolescence and adolescence are periods of development that have an increased risk for developing addiction to nicotine. Nicotine alters contextual learning, but it remains unknown whether these effects are age-dependent. Therefore, the present study examined the effects of acute, chronic, and withdrawal from chronic nicotine on fear conditioning in pre-adolescent, adolescent, and adult mice. In addition, we investigated whether exposure to chronic nicotine during pre-adolescence or adolescence has long-lasting effects on contextual learning that occurs during adulthood. Pre-adolescent mice were more sensitive to the effects of acute nicotine than adolescents and adults, showed enhanced contextual learning when treated with high doses of chronic nicotine, and were less sensitive than adolescents and adults to nicotine withdrawal-related deficits in contextual learning. In contrast, adolescent mice were less sensitive to the effects of acute nicotine on contextual learning than pre-adolescents and adults and were more sensitive to nicotine withdrawal-related deficits in contextual learning relative to pre-adolescents and adults. Chronic nicotine exposure during pre-adolescence or adolescence also produced long-lasting impairments in contextual learning that were observed during adulthood, whereas adult chronic nicotine exposure had no effect on fear conditioning. Together, these data suggest that pre-adolescent and adolescent nicotine exposure has both short-term and long-term effects on contextual learning that may play an important role in the development and maintenance of nicotine addiction.
Temple University--Theses
Blechert, Jens. "The psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder and panic disorder : fear conditioning, autonomous underpinnings and issues of measurement /". Basel : [s.n.], 2006. http://edoc.unibas.ch/diss/DissB_7853.
Testo completo