Thèses sur le sujet « Second Variation »
Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres
Consultez les 50 meilleures thèses pour votre recherche sur le sujet « Second Variation ».
À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.
Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.
Parcourez les thèses sur diverses disciplines et organisez correctement votre bibliographie.
Lin, Yuhan. « Stylistic Variation and Social Perception in Second Dialect Acquisition ». The Ohio State University, 2018. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1532059573668516.
Texte intégralSheikh, Christine. « Religious and Ethnic Variation Among Second-Generation Muslim Americans ». Diss., The University of Arizona, 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/194730.
Texte intégralJenkins, Jennifer. « Variation in phonological error in interlanguage talk ». Thesis, University College London (University of London), 1995. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/10019151/.
Texte intégralDzacka, Charles Nunya. « A Variation of the Carleman Embedding Method for Second Order Systems ». Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2009. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/1877.
Texte intégralBiberauer, Mary Theresa. « Verb second (V2) in Afrikaans : a minimalist investigation of word order variation ». Thesis, University of Cambridge, 2004. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.615733.
Texte intégralDyson, Bronwen Patricia, University of Western Sydney et of Arts Education and Social Sciences College. « Developmental style in second language processing : a study of inter-learner variation in the acquisition of English as a second language ». THESIS_CAESS_XXX_Dyson_B.xml, 2004. http://handle.uws.edu.au:8081/1959.7/817.
Texte intégralDoctor of Philosophy (PhD)
Dyson, Bronwen Patricia. « Developmental style in second language processing a study of inter-learner variation in the acquisition of English as a second language / ». View thesis, 2004. http://library.uws.edu.au/adt-NUWS/public/adt-NUWS20050729.140020/index.html.
Texte intégralA thesis presented to the University of Western Sydney in fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Includes bibliography.
Gonzalez, Johnson Aracelis Maydee. « Dialectal Allophonic Variation in L2 Pronunciation ». OpenSIUC, 2012. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/theses/783.
Texte intégralAusten, Martha. « The Role of Listener Experience in Perception of Conditioned Dialect Variation ». The Ohio State University, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu159532560325774.
Texte intégralFisher, Isaac W. « Transfer of stylistic phonetic variables indexing sexuality in second language contexts ». Kansas State University, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/2097/32676.
Texte intégralDepartment of Modern Languages
Earl K. Brown
This paper reports on a study that analyzes how a sequential bilingual speaker (L1 Mexican Spanish, L2 American English) uses stylistic phonetic variation in different speech types during an interview (short answer, spontaneous speech, dramatic anecdote, reading) to construct a dynamic gay persona. There are many stylistic variables that can interact when an individual is creating a persona in an interaction, and this becomes even more complex when analyzing L1 speech as well as L2 speech as there are two collections of stylistic phonetic variables (indexical fields) interacting from two different cultural ideologies available to the interlocutors. It is problematic to assign one distinct variable to an identity, such as gay, as it homogenizes a diverse social group of individuals and underestimates members' ability to manage perceptual salience of their identity as a gay individual based on context and social pressure(s). While the field of Lavender Linguistics (language use associated with the LGBTQ community) has shown that there are many resources that can be used to "sound gay," this case study focuses on how a speaker stylistically creates a gay persona throughout the interview through stylistic variation of two principle variables: 1) word-final /s/ duration, and 2) center of gravity of word-final /s/.
Omenyi, Louis Okechukwu. « On the second variation of the spectral zeta function of the Laplacian on homogeneous Riemanniann manifolds ». Thesis, Loughborough University, 2014. https://dspace.lboro.ac.uk/2134/16167.
Texte intégralGathany, Mark A. « Sources of variation in ecosystem carbon pools : a comparison of adjacent old- and second-growth forests / ». Ohio University / OhioLINK, 2004. http://www.ohiolink.edu/etd/view.cgi?ohiou1102537971.
Texte intégralFavart, Françoise. « La représentation de " l'oralité populaire"dans quelques romans du second XXème siècle (1966-2006) ». Paris 10, 2009. http://www.theses.fr/2009PA100090.
Texte intégralThis study aims to analyse the way popular speech is represented in several French novels of the second half of the 20th century (1966-2006). The theoretical part offers definitions of the polysemous concepts of ‘orality’ and ‘popular’; linguistic markers are also presented which, from the sociolectic standpoint, are frequently treated as connoting popular speech. The analysis of the phenomena of popular orality which the 1966-2006 corpus demonstrates, and likewise the synthesis of the data provided in the second and third parts, allow the author to highlight changes in the way popular speech has been represented in literature over this period, with reference to the first half of the century as well. Furthermore, this study shows that popular language, as reconstructed by the writers, is an artefact, based on a perspective of the spoken language shifting between its actual and its imagined use. Popular language as it is represented is therefore a decoy, based on the use of a code. In the last analysis, the author poses the question as to whether popular language really exists, or whether in fact it concerns an operational notion rather than a true linguistic phenomenon
Lindström, Eva. « L’acquisition du genre en français L2 – développement et variation ». Doctoral thesis, Stockholms universitet, Romanska och klassiska institutionen, 2013. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-93043.
Texte intégralWallgren, Jonas. « Attitudes Towards and Uses of the Japanese Adverbzenzen by Swedish Learners of Japanese ». Thesis, Högskolan Dalarna, Engelska, 2015. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:du-19264.
Texte intégralKunschak, Claudia. « Awareness of and attitudes toward variation in L2 : Origins, prevalence and implications for second/foreign language teaching ». Diss., The University of Arizona, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/289947.
Texte intégralLi, Rui. « Differences in the Motivations of Chinese Learners of English in Different (Foreign or Second Language) Contexts ». BYU ScholarsArchive, 2017. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/6299.
Texte intégralSeyhan, Ninva. « Pedagogisk dokumentation : En kvalitativ studie om pedagogers uppfattningar kring arbetet med pedagogisk dokumentation i relation till det systematiska kvalitetsarbetet i Reggio Emiliainspirerade förskolor ». Thesis, Södertörns högskola, Lärarutbildningen, 2018. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:sh:diva-35393.
Texte intégralCollins, Brett. « SANDHI-VARIATION AND THE COMPREHENSION OF SPOKEN ENGLISH FOR JAPANESE LEARNERS ». Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2018. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/500157.
Texte intégralPh.D.
In this study I addressed three problems related to how sandhi-variation, the adjustments made by speakers to the speech stream, filters comprehension for second language listener processing. The first was the need to better understand proficiency problems encountered by L2 listeners as they decode the speech stream with the phonological features of sandhi-variation, elision and assimilation, by investigating the item difficulty hierarchy of the phenomena. The second was the scarcity of research on aural processing abilities of second language learners in relation to their understanding sandhi-variation in aural texts. The third concerns the lack of research investigating links between learners’ backgrounds and their ability to handle listening texts, especially variations in the speech stream in target aural texts. The purpose of this study was threefold. My first purpose was to investigate the item difficulty hierarchy of sandhi-variation types that learners have in relation to L2 listening proficiency. My second purpose was to evaluate links between aural input containing elision and assimilation and second language aural processing, to provide insight into how learners deal with sandhi-variation as they process such input. My third purpose was to investigate through the use of interviews the aural input that participants have encountered prior to the interventions of this study, to help explain which types of aural input can facilitate intake. Twenty-five first- and second-year Japanese university students participated in the current study. The participants completed a series of instruments, which included (a) a Test of English as a Foreign Language Paper-Based Test (TOEFL PBT), (b) a Listening Vocabulary Levels Test (LVLT), (c) a Modern Language Aptitude Test–Elementary (MLAT-E), (d) a Pre-Listening in English questionnaire, (e) an Elicited Imitation Test (EIT), and (f) a Background and Length of Residency interview. The EIT was used as a sandhi-variation listening test with two component parts (i.e., elision and assimilation) and two sub-component parts (e.g., two different utterance rates), using elicited imitation. Finally, the participants were interviewed about their language backgrounds to gauge their understanding and feelings about English. An empirical item hierarchy for elision and assimilation was investigated, along with the determinants of the hierarchy. Overall, the tendency was for items with elision and assimilation to be more difficult. Results also indicated that the two input rate variables combined with elision and assimilation affected the non-native participants’ listening comprehension. Moreover, the strength of the relationship between two measures of the participants’ language ability, proficiency and aptitude, and their comprehension of items with and without the phonological features of elision and assimilation, were investigated. The results confirmed a positive relationship between language aptitude as measured by the MLAT-E and the comprehension of the phonological features of elision and assimilation. Finally, the results indicated that there were no significant, positive correlations between English language proficiency scores and both the Pre-Listening Questionnaire, which measured the participants’ feelings about second language listening, and the Background and Length of Residency Interview. More research needs to be conducted to determine how learners’ backgrounds are related to listening comprehension in order to better prescribe aural input in second language listening classrooms.
Temple University--Theses
McCourt, Claire A. « Learner use of French second-person pronouns in synchronous electronic communication ». Thesis, University of North Texas, 2008. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc9753/.
Texte intégralMurakami, Akira. « Individual variation and the role of L1 in the L2 development of English grammatical morphemes : insights from learner corpora ». Thesis, University of Cambridge, 2014. https://www.repository.cam.ac.uk/handle/1810/254430.
Texte intégralBonacini, Marco. « Minimality and stability results for a class of free-discontinuity and nonlocal isoperimetric problems ». Doctoral thesis, SISSA, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11767/4832.
Texte intégralEngel, Hugues. « Dislocation et référence aux entités en français L2 : Développement, interaction, variation ». Doctoral thesis, Stockholms universitet, Institutionen för franska, italienska och klassiska språk, 2010. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-38716.
Texte intégralPiffet, Loïc. « Décomposition d’image par modèles variationnels : débruitage et extraction de texture ». Thesis, Orléans, 2010. http://www.theses.fr/2010ORLE2053/document.
Texte intégralThis thesis is devoted in a first part to the elaboration of a second order variational modelfor image denoising, using the BV 2 space of bounded hessian functions. We here take a leaf out of the well known Rudin, Osher and Fatemi (ROF) model, where we replace the minimization of the total variation of the function with the minimization of the second order total variation of the function, that is to say the total variation of its partial derivatives. The goal is to get a competitive model with no staircasing effect that generates the ROF model anymore. The model we study seems to be efficient, but generates a blurry effect. In order to deal with it, we introduce a mixed model that permits to get solutions with no staircasing and without blurry effect on details. In a second part, we take an interset to the texture extraction problem. A model known as one of the most efficient is the T V -L1 model. It just consits in replacing the L2 norm of the fitting data term with the L1 norm.We propose here an original way to solve this problem by the use of augmented Lagrangian methods. For the same reason than for the denoising case, we also take an interest to the T V 2-L1 model, replacing again the total variation of the function by the second order total variation. A mixed model for texture extraction is finally briefly introduced. This manuscript ends with a huge chapter of numerical tests
Cottrell, Catherine E. « Genetic variation and complex disease : the examination of an X-linked disorder and a multifactorial disease ». The Ohio State University, 2007. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1196182829.
Texte intégralOlteanu, Constanta. « ”Vad skulle x kunna vara?” : andragradsekvation och andragradsfunktion som objekt för lärande ». Doctoral thesis, Umeå University, Mathematics, Technology and Science Education, 2007. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-1363.
Texte intégralAlgebraic equations and functions play an important role in various mathematical topics, including algebra, trigonometry, linear programming and calculus. Accordingly, various documents, such as the most recent Swedish curriculum (Lpf 94) for upper secondary school and the course syllabi in mathematics, specify what the students should learn in Mathematics Course B. They should be able to solve quadratic equations and apply this knowledge in solving problems, explain the properties of a function, as well as be able to set up, interpret and use some nonlinear functions as models for real processes. To implement these recommendations, it is crucial to understand the students’ way of experiencing quadratic equations and functions, and describe the meaning these have for the students in relation to the possibility they have to their experience of them.
The aim of this thesis is to analyse, understand and explain the relation between the handled and learned content, which consists of second-degree equations and quadratic functions, in classroom practice. This means that content is the research object and not the teacher’s conceptions or knowledge of, or about this content. This restriction implies that the handled and learned contents are central in this study and will be analysed from different perspectives.
The study includes two teachers and 45 students in two different classes. The data consist of video-recordings of lessons, individual sessions, interviews and the teachers’/researcher’s review of the individual sessions. The students’ tests also constituted an important part of the data collection.
When analysing the data, concepts relating to variation theory have been used as analytical tools. Data have been analysed in respect of the teachers’ focus on the lesson content, which aspects are ignored and which patterns of dimensions of variations are constituted when the contents are handled by the teachers in the classroom. Also, data have been analysed in respect of the students’ focus when they solve different exercises in a test situation. It can be shown that the meaning of parameters, the unknown quantity in an equation and the function’s argument change several times when the teacher presents the content in the classroom and when the students solve different exercises. It can also be shown that the teachers and the students develop complicated patterns of variation during the lessons and that the ways in which the teachers open up dimensions of variation play an important role in the learning process. The results indicate that there is a convergent variation leading the students to improve their learning. By focusing on some aspects of the objects of learning and create convergent variations, it is possible for the students to understand the difference between various interpretations of these aspects and thereafter focus on the interpretation that fits in a certain context. Furthermore, this variation leads the students to make generalisations in each object of learning (equations and functions) and between these objects of learning. These generalisations remain over time, despite working with new objects of learning. An important result in this study is that the implicit or explicit arguments of a function can make it possible to discern an equation from a function despite the fact that they are constituted by the same algebraic expression.
Wu, Junyu. « A Linguistic Analysis of the Written Production of Second Language Learners : The Variation of Article Usage by Adult Chinese Learners of English ». Thesis, Högskolan Dalarna, Engelska, 2014. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:du-13828.
Texte intégralDrummond, Rob John. « Sociolinguistic variation in a second language : the influence of local accent on the pronunciation of non-native English speakers living in Manchester ». Thesis, University of Manchester, 2010. https://www.research.manchester.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/sociolinguistic-variation-in-a-second-language-the-influence-of-local-accent-on-the-pronunciation-of-nonnative-english-speakers-living-in-manchester(614f2f75-4705-4cc0-a93a-4b1914a88e04).html.
Texte intégralSchmidt, Lili Sara. « "Hon har SFI-uttal" : En perceptionsstudie om hur vuxna andraspråksinlärare uppfattar modersmålssvenska och andraspråkssvenska ». Thesis, Stockholms universitet, Centrum för tvåspråkighetsforskning, 2014. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-104770.
Texte intégralAbstract Being able to identify and understand different varieties of the target language is a fundamental condition for learners of Swedish as a second language in order to gain full language proficiency and become a participant in today’s multilingual Swedish society. The present study is an explorative investigation of how adult second language learners acquire knowledge of native and non-native variation in Swedish and of sociolinguistic awareness. A listening experiment is used to collect and analyse data in three steps, i.e. how L2 listeners judge different speakers, how they explain their judgements, and how they perceive their own Swedish compared to different speakers. A questionnaire is used to collect information about language experience and to find correlations between this and L2 listeners’ judgements. The results indicate that L2 listeners’ knowledge of variation varies to a great extent, and that some of them seem to have identical judgements to the native control group. The data in this study does not support that there are any correlations between L2 listeners’ language experience and judgements. Possible interpretations and explanations of what can affect L2 listeners’ knowledge are presented as well.
Barnatt, John. « The design and distribution of stone circles in Britain : a reflection of variation in social organization in the second and third millennia BC ». Thesis, University of Sheffield, 1988. http://etheses.whiterose.ac.uk/14854/.
Texte intégralOwusu-Ansah, L. K. « Variation according to context in a second language : a study into the effects of formality on the English used by Ghanaian university students ». Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 1992. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/20081.
Texte intégralBérard, Bergery Blandine. « Approximation du temps local et intégration par régularisation ». Thesis, Nancy 1, 2007. http://www.theses.fr/2007NAN10058/document.
Texte intégralThe setting of this work is the integration by regularization of Russo and Vallois. The first part studies schemes of approximation of the local time of continuous semimartingales. If X is a reversible diffusion, the convergence of a first schema of approximation to the local time of X is proven, in probability uniformly on the compact sets. From this first schema, two other schemas of approximation for the local time are found. One converges in the semi-martingale case, the other in the Brownian case. Moreover, in the Brownian case, we estimate the rate of convergence in L^2(Omega) and a result of almost sure convergence is proven. The second part study the forward integral and the generalized quadratic variation, which have been defined by convergence of families of integrals, in probability uniformly on the compacts sets. In the case of Hölder processes, the almost sure convergence is proven. Finally, the second order convergence is studied in many cases
Barra, Melissa Ann. « Teaching Spanish slang, familiar language, and electronic language in the classroom / ». Click here to view full-text, 2007. http://digitalcollections.sit.edu/ipp_collection/12/.
Texte intégralPAPARINI, SILVIA. « Modelli Matematici per Cristalli Liquidi Cromonici ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2022. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/362343.
Texte intégralThis dissertation is concerned with a peculiar class of lyotropics: chromatic liquid crystals (CLCs). Chromonics in the nematic phase enjoy the head-tail symmetry, that is, they are non-chiral materials with a tendency for their constitutive to bundle together so that a director can be defined at a mesoscopic scale which lacks polarity. The ground state of ordinary nematic liquid crystals is attained when the director is uniform in space. When CLCs are confined in capillary cylinders with degenerate boundary conditions that would be compatible with the uniform alignment of the director along the cylinder's axis, they are observed to acquire a nonuniform arrangement instead. Expecially, their ground state in cylindrical capillary, often referred to as escaped twist (ET) ground state, is two-fold; it consists of two symmetric twisted configurations (left- and right-handed), each variant occurring with the same likelihood, as was to be expected from the lack of chirality in the molecular aggregates that constitute these materials. Despite the clear indication that CLCs in three-dimensional space exhibit a different behavior from common nematic liquid crystals, the Oseen-Frank theory for nematics has been applied to rationalize the experiments with capillary tubes and so to determine the configuration of the ET ground state. This is a variational theory which posits a free energy density quadratic in the director gradient that penalizes all distortions of the director away from the unifom alignment. Four basic distortions are classically identified, which correspond to four independent elastic constants; these are the splay K_{11}, twist K_{22}, bend K_{33}, and saddle-splay K_{24} constants, which have recently been re-interpreted in a new light by Selinger. Ericksen's inequalities ensure that Frank's energy density is positive definite, and the spontaneous emergence of chirality in the nematic texture is not conceivable when they hold. Thus, Frank's elastic theory justifies the observed configurations of CLCs under cylindrical confinement only if the relevant Ericksen's inequality, K_{22}>=K_{24}$, is violated, and so only if Frank's free energy functional is unbounded below in 3D Euclidean space. Ideed, the configuration they fall in is an equilibrium one only if this inequality is violated. The alternative form of the Oseen-Frank free energy density proposed by Selinger distributes the saddle-splay contribution in the other elastic modes. In so doing, the pure twist here corresponds to the elastic constant K_{22}-K_{24} (instead of only K_{22} in the previous formulation) and it is termed double twist, as it has no characteristic direction in the plane perpenditular to the director. The negativity of (K_{22}-K_{24}) suggests that the pure double-twisted configuration, the director configuration exiciting only the double twist mode, is the gound state of CLCs in 3D space. This peculiar director field belongs to neither of the families of uniform director; it is only possible along a 1D curve and produces elastic frustration if requested to occupy a particular geometry with particular boundary conditions. As a result, exending the ideal double twisted texture to a tubolar region introduces by necessity a non uniform texture which results from the combination of other fundamental deformation modes with pure double twist only along the axis of the cylinder. This is exatly the configuration experimentally observed for CLCs in cylidrical capillaries subject to degenerate boundary conditions. Taken for granted that the ground state of CLCs is a characteristic double-twist, we say that the so called escaped-twist ground state actually is a `pseudo-ground state' since it is the result of the confinement-induced extension of the pure double twist, which injects a elastic frustration in the system. What is wrong with the application of Frank's elastic theory to CLCs? Why do we need a new elastic theory for them?
Aktürk-Drake, Memet. « Phonological Adoption through Bilingual Borrowing : Comparing Elite Bilinguals and Heritage Bilinguals ». Doctoral thesis, Stockholms universitet, Centrum för tvåspråkighetsforskning, 2015. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-112792.
Texte intégralAt the time of the doctoral defense, the following paper was unpublished and had a status as follows: Paper 3: Submitted.
Riou, Etienne. « La dislocation clitique à gauche en français langue étrangère ». Thesis, Sorbonne Paris Cité, 2017. http://www.theses.fr/2017USPCC104/document.
Texte intégralThe present dissertation deals with didactic description of linguistic variation in a constraint-based approach. In line with the Third Wave movement of variation studies (Eckert, 2012) and Social Meaning Games (Burnett, 2017; accepted), we argue that stylistic constraints are a subset of pragmatic constraints. This approach allows us to consider variation in a functional perspective rather than in a normative perspective and to describe “non-standard” variants as more or less appropriate to certain tasks rather than deviations from the norm. To illustrate our approach, we are applying it to the description of clitic left dislocation in French. We propose that the variation of clitic left dislocation with clefts and canonical construction is constrained by information structure (Lambrecht, 1994), Partially Ordered Set relations (Ward & Prince, 1991) and stylistic stigmatization in formal context (Zribi-Hertz, 1994). We claim that these constraints are all pragmatic in nature and that their interaction weight on the use of clitic left dislocation in French. These claims are tested empirically via a corpus studies, a series of acceptability judgment tests and a matched guise test. Furthermore, we argue that the learning of pragmatic constraints in foreign language is dependent of their explicit teaching and repeated expositions to the construction in felicitous contexts. Following the dynamic interface hypothesis (Ellis, 2005), we suggest that explicit learning of the constraints of clitic left dislocation in the context of the classroom facilitates their implicit learning when the learners find themselves in a situation of communication with French native speakers. The role of exposition is explored empirically by replicating an acceptability judgment test and the matched guise test with non-native participants. Finally, all of our observations are tentatively implemented to didactic discourse with the help of notions and a terminology already used in pedagogical grammars (Germain & Séguin, 1998). Discursive constraints of clitic left dislocation are described using the distinction between old and new information (Capelle & Gidon, 1999; Watorek, 1998). Stylistics constraints are described using existential competencies and sociolinguistics registers (European Framework, 2001)
Viana, dos Santos Gabriela. « Représentations et schémas sociolinguistiques en langue étrangère : l'exemple d'apprenants sinophones et anglophones du FLE ». Thesis, Université Grenoble Alpes, 2021. http://www.theses.fr/2021GRALL003.
Texte intégralOne of the particularities of the process of sociolinguistic acquisition in a foreign language concerns the difficulty of learners to integrate the sociolinguistic norms of the target language. If in their native culture, the learners build a network of associations between linguistic knowledge and social knowledge, in a foreign language, they must develop this capacity. The main objectives of this work are to understand how learners develop schemas that associate sociolinguistic variants with social parameters and how they associate socio-indexical values to varieties. We carried out two studies with FLE learners studying in France. For the first study, we did a repetition task with 24 English-speaking students and 42 Chinese-speaking students. The second study, a subjective reaction test, was conducted among 40 English-speaking students, 41 Chinese-speaking students, and 88 native French-speaking students. Our analysis highlights the tendency of learners to make homogeneous statements that are not homogeneous at the sociolinguistic level. This result suggests that learners construct sociolinguistic schemas of standard and non-standard varieties in a foreign language. We have also found that learners are sensitive to sociolinguistic variations in the target language. Indeed, they judged standard and non-standard French statements differently. Our analysis also reveals differences in the construction of sociolinguistic schemas and in the judgments of varieties between the two nationalities of our study
Camargo, Valeria Sena. « Traços fonético-fonológicos do português para falantes do espanhol e do inglês : segmentos dificultadores para a aquisição do português brasileiro ». Universidade de São Paulo, 2009. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/8/8142/tde-01022010-143929/.
Texte intégralThe increasing interest on learning Brazilian Portuguese(BP) has moved many universities and other national institutions towards researches on different manners of Portuguese acquisition as first language (L1) and some possible influences on acquisition of Portuguese as a second language (L2), as well as a search for teaching strategies and for preparing teachers, making them capable to teach Portuguese as a Foreign Language. The experience with foreign students leads us to a frequent questioning about how to do (how to teach) and what may be the main difficulties offered by BP to those who want to learn it. The sounds of BP, particularly the nasals, fricatives, oclusives and liquids were studied, trying to do a comparative study between the occurence or not of them in the four subjects first language, i.e, Spanish spoken in America and English spoken in the United States and what could be predicted as obstacles to be faced by students who have these languages as their L1 when they produce the sounds of BP. We recorded 4 subjects, identified in this work as subject-learners and analyzed which are the sounds that may be difficult to produce, trying to identify in what situation they occur. As methodology, we chose to record the four subject-learners readings who, in a first moment, read a list of 43 words and, at a second phase of the data collection, read a short text. The conclusions we came up with when analyzing the data confirmed the relevancy of teachers well prepared to teach Portuguese as a foreign language, as well as teaching books that comprise not only grammar and cultural aspects of the BP but also phonetic-phonological aspects pertaining to BP.
Berard, Bergery Blandine. « Approximation du temps local et intégration par régularisation ». Phd thesis, Université Henri Poincaré - Nancy I, 2007. http://tel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-00181777.
Texte intégralJonsson, Signe. « Automaticity in L2 learning : Correlation between vocabulary proficiency and response time in word recognition ». Thesis, Stockholms universitet, Engelska institutionen, 2016. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-130831.
Texte intégralChung, Chiyoon. « A Performer’s Perspective on the Berg Piano Sonata, Op. 1:A Stylistic Analysis and Interpretation ». University of Cincinnati / OhioLINK, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ucin1439282655.
Texte intégralConn, Jeffrey C. « Portland Dialect Study : The Story of /æ/ in Portland ». PDXScholar, 2000. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/4518.
Texte intégralCosta, Raquel Maria da Silva. « A alternÃncia das formas pronominais tu, vocà e o (a) senhor (a) na funÃÃo de sujeito no portuguÃs falado em Cametà - PA ». Universidade Federal do CearÃ, 2016. http://www.teses.ufc.br/tde_busca/arquivo.php?codArquivo=18079.
Texte intégralEsta Tese apresenta um estudo sobre a alternÃncia das formas pronominais de referÃncia à segunda pessoa, Tu/VocÃ/o(a) Senhor(a), na funÃÃo de sujeito, no portuguÃs falado na zona urbana do municÃpio de CametÃ, Nordeste do estado do ParÃ. Adota, como quadro teÃrico, a interface entre dois postulados teÃricos: a Teoria da VariaÃÃo e MudanÃa LinguÃstica e o Funcionalismo linguÃstico, gerando dessa forma uma abordagem caracterizada no Ãmbito dos estudos linguÃsticos de sociofuncionalista. Nossa pesquisa objetiva analisar o papel de fatores linguÃsticos (referÃncia do pronome, estrutura do verbo, paralelismo estrutural, tipo de relato/discurso, tipo de frase, tempo discursivo do verbo e tempo gramatical do verbo) e de fatores extralinguÃsticos ou sociais (sexo/gÃnero, faixa etÃria, nÃvel de escolaridade e relaÃÃo social entre os interlocutores) como motivadores do comportamento variÃvel de tu/vocÃ/o(a) senhor(a) em funÃÃo de sujeito. O corpus contÃm dados de interaÃÃes face a face de 16 grupos focais, cada qual constituÃdo por 04 (quatro) sujeitos participantes, sendo um o informante-base, todos cametaenses, estratificados de acordo com a faixa etÃria (21 a 29 anos e 32 a 42 anos), o sexo/gÃnero (masculino e feminino) e o nÃvel de escolaridade (mÃdio e superior), totalizando 64 participantes. Na fala dos 16 informantes-base, obtivemos 527 dados de uso de Tu, Vocà e o(a) Senhor(a), os quais foram analisados quantitativamente (mediante uso do pacote estatÃstico GOLDVARB) e qualitativamente, seguindo os pressupostos teÃricos da sociolinguÃstica laboviana e do funcionalismo norte-americano. Os resultados apontaram 307 ocorrÃncias da forma pronominal tu, 182 de vocà e apenas 38 da forma o(a) senhor(a), o que corresponde, respectivamente, a 58.3%, 34.5% e 7.2%. Observamos que a forma tu à favorecida quando se encontra em paralelismo sintÃtico tu-tu, pela frase exclamativa, pela referÃncia indireta/direta a um indivÃduo, pelos homens, falantes do ensino mÃdio e nas interaÃÃes socialmente simÃtricas; a forma vocà à tambÃm motivada pelo princÃpio do paralelismo sintÃtico, vocÃ-vocÃ, ocorre com maior frequÃncia em referÃncia indireta a um interlocutor ou grupo de denotaÃÃo genÃrica e na fala prÃpria/discurso direto; em relaÃÃo à forma o(a) senhor(a), observamos que à condicionada pelo tipo de frase interrogativa, referÃncia direta/especÃfica a um indivÃduo, fala reportada (do prÃprio falante e/ou terceira pessoa), preferida pelos mais jovens e pelas mulheres. No Ãmbito do Sociofuncionalismo, ao avaliarmos a correlaÃÃo entre variÃvel dependente e variÃveis independentes pelo princÃpio da marcaÃÃo, verificamos que a forma tu à menos marcada na linguagem cametaense, considerando-se a distribuiÃÃo de frequÃncia e a complexidade estrutural. Portanto, ocorre preferencialmente em ambientes menos marcados, atestando o princÃpio da marcaÃÃo (GIVÃN, 1995), como em: contextos de estruturas paralelas tu-tu e tipo de referÃncia especÃfica direta e indireta a um interlocutor. A forma vocÃ, de menor frequÃncia e maior complexidade estrutural e cognitiva, por isso mais marcada, encontra predileÃÃo, nos contextos mais marcados, como: referÃncia genÃrica e frase do tipo declarativa negativa. Da mesma forma o(a) senhor(a), pronome marcado na lÃngua, ocorre em contextos mais marcados tambÃm, de maior complexidade estrutural e cognitiva, como frases interrogativas e negativas.
This thesis presents a study of the alternation of pronominal forms of reference to the second person, Tu/VocÃ/o(a) Senhor(a), in the subject function, in the Portuguese spoken in the urban area of CametÃ, Northeast of the State of ParÃ. It adopts, as a theoretical framework, the interface between two postulates: Linguistic Variation and Change Theory and Linguistic Functionalism, generating a featured approach within the language studies of social functionalist. Our research intends to analyze the role of linguistic factors (pronoun reference, verbal structure, structural parallelism, type of report/speech, type of sentence, discursive tense and grammatical tense) and of extralinguistic or social factors (sex/gender, age, level of education and social relationship between the interlocutors) as motivators of the variable behavior of tu/vocÃ/o(a) senhor(a) in the subject function. The corpus contains data of face to face interaction of 16 focus groups, each one constituted by 04 (four) participant individuals, with one being the basis informant, all born in CametÃ, stratified by age (21-29 years old and 32-42 years old), gender (male and female) and level of education (high school and higher education), totalizing 64 participants. By the basis informants speech, we got 527 data uses of Tu, Vocà e o(a) Senhor(a), which were quantitatively (via use of the statistical package GOLDVARB) and qualitatively analyzed, following the theoretical assumptions of the labovian sociolinguistic and of the North American functionalism. The results pointed 307 occurrences of the pronominal form tu, 182 of vocà and only 38 of o(a) senhor(a), which represents, respectively, 58.3%, 34.5%, e 7.2%. We noticed that the tu form is favored when in syntactic parallelism tu-tu, by the exclamatory sentence, by the indirect/direct reference to a individual, by men, high school speakers and on socially symmetric interactions; the form vocà is also motivated by the syntactic parallelism vocÃ-vocà principle, it occurs with higher frequency in indirect reference to an interlocutor or a group of generic denotation and in the own speech/direct speech; regarding the form o(a) senhor(a), we noticed that it is conditioned by the interrogative sentence, direct/specific reference to an individual, reported speech (of the own speaker and/or third person), preferred by younger individuals and by the women. Within the Social functionalism, evaluating the correlation between dependent variable and independent variables by the principle of marking, we noted that the form tu is less marked in the language of CametÃ, considering the distribution of frequency and structural complexity. Therefore, it occurs in less marked environments, attesting the principle of marking (GIVÃN, 1995), as in: contexts of parallel structures tu-tu and specific direct and indirect type of reference to an interlocutor. The form vocÃ, that presents less frequency and greater structural and cognitive complexity, hence more marked, finds predilection, in more marked contexts, as: generic reference and negative declarative type of sentence. Similarly o(a) senhor(a), marked pronoun in the language, also occurs in more marked, of greater structural and cognitive complexity, contexts, as interrogative and negative sentences.
Gutierres, Athany. « Variação na aquisição fonológica : análise da produção da nasal velar em inglês (L2) ». reponame:Biblioteca Digital de Teses e Dissertações da UFRGS, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/10183/142958.
Texte intégralThis thesis investigates the variable phonological acquisition of the velar nasal in final syllabic coda (such as in living ‘vivendo’ and singing ‘cantando’, for example) by Brazilian learners of English as a second language. The study is motivated by the different status of the velar nasal in Portuguese (L1, first language) and in English (L2, second language): while the segment is a phonetic realization of assimilation of place of articulation in the L1 (‘atum’, where the bilabial nasal assimilates the feature [+dorsal] of the preceding vowel, being realized as velar), the velar nasal is a phoneme in the L2 (sin ‘pecado’ x sing ‘cantar’, realized as the alveolar and velar nasal, respectively). In some native English-speaking communities, words derived from the suffix {ing} (such as read.ing ‘lendo’, study.ing ‘estudando’, for example) are in linguistic stable variation, in which the speakers alternate the production of the alveolar and velar nasal the velar nasal at the end of words. This variation is both structurally and socially conditioned (LABOV, 1994; 2001). The empirical data of this study were collected through recordings with the informants, who belong to two levels of proficiency: basic and pre-intermediate. Two formal analysis are made: one that explains the observable systematic variation in the Interlanguage, through the software Goldvarb (SANKOFF, TAGLIAMONTE and SMITH, 2015) and another that formalizes the internal organization of this variable grammar, through gradual learning algorithms associated to the Stochastic Optimality Theory, the Gradual Learning Algorithm (GLA) (BOERSMA and HAYES, 2001) and the ORTO Ajuste Paramétrico (DORNELLES FILHO, 2014). The Variable Rule Analysis has demonstrated that interlanguage is a linguistic system subject to ordered variation as in all the other natural languages, conditioned by linguistic aspects (morphological class) and extralinguistic ones (level of proficiency). The variation is verified by the oral production of the learners, examined by hearing analysis, which alternates the palatal nasal (63,6%) The stochastic analysis has revealed an Interlanguage system dominated by Markedness constraints, where variation is due to the increase on the values of selection points for Faithfulness constraints. The constraints directly involved in the variable phonological acquisition of the velar nasal are AGREEplaceVN#, which demands the sequence Vowel+Nasal in final coda to share place of articulation, and IDENTnasal, which demands feature identity [+nasal] between input and output forms. The analyses made, which represent the learners’ language in its internal and external aspects, or, according to Chomsky’s terminology ([1965] 1975), the linguistic competence and performance, have proved the variable nature of the Interlanguage and have enabled a theoretical reflection upon the possibility of dialogue between the structural and the social to the explanation of variable linguistic phenomena.
Georginis, Emmanuel-Gabriel. « Variations of experience : Expatriate British writers in the Middle East during the second world war ». Thesis, Loughborough University, 1989. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.328852.
Texte intégralMassey, Susan R. « Effects of Variations of Text Previews on the Oral Reading of Second Grade Students ». Scholarly Repository, 2008. http://scholarlyrepository.miami.edu/oa_dissertations/121.
Texte intégralHogg, Simon I. « Second-moment-closure calculations of strongly-swirling confined flows with and without density variations ». Thesis, University of Manchester, 1988. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.328638.
Texte intégralDebroux, Noémie. « Mathematical modelling of image processing problems : theoretical studies and applications to joint registration and segmentation ». Thesis, Normandie, 2018. http://www.theses.fr/2018NORMIR02/document.
Texte intégralIn this thesis, we study and jointly address several important image processing problems including registration that aims at aligning images through a deformation, image segmentation whose goal consists in finding the edges delineating the objects inside an image, and image decomposition closely related to image denoising, and attempting to partition an image into a smoother version of it named cartoon and its complementary oscillatory part called texture, with both local and nonlocal variational approaches. The first proposed model addresses the topology-preserving segmentation-guided registration problem in a variational framework. A second joint segmentation and registration model is introduced, theoretically and numerically studied, then tested on various numerical simulations. The last model presented in this work tries to answer a more specific need expressed by the CEREMA (Centre of analysis and expertise on risks, environment, mobility and planning), namely automatic crack recovery detection on bituminous surface images. Due to the image complexity, a joint fine structure decomposition and segmentation model is proposed to deal with this problem. It is then theoretically and numerically justified and validated on the provided images
Gervais, Marie-France. « L'intégration de la variation sociolinguistique en français langue seconde : Une étude de documents pédagogiques ». Thesis, University of Ottawa (Canada), 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/10393/27455.
Texte intégralBruschetta, Mattia. « A variational integrators approach to second order modeling and identification of linear mechanical systems ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli studi di Padova, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/11577/3421654.
Texte intégralL'identicazione di modelli di sistemi meccanici del secondo ordine e stata oggetto di un'intensa attivita di ricerca negli ultimi decenni. In questa tesi ci si focalizza nei sistemi meccanici che si posso descrivere con un modello classic del secondo ordine definito dalle classiche matrici di inerzia generalizzata M, stiffness K e damping D. Tutte le attuali tecniche di identicazione operano a tempo discreto. I dati rumorosi ottenuti dal campionamento del sistema devono essere utilizzati per stimare i parametri sici del sistema a tempo continuo M;K e D. Poiche il processo di identicazione opera a tempo discreto si rende necessaria una conversione del sistema discreto identicato in uno a tempo continuo. Ci sono vincoli strutturali che devono essere imposti per ottenere la struttura del secondo ordine. In breve, la procedure si compone di tre parti principali: 1. Identicazione a tempo discreto, per lo pi metodi a sottospazi, dai dati ingresso-uscita campionati; 2. Implementazione di un set di vincoli che forzi il sistema identicato alla forma; 3. Conversione dal dominio di tempo discreto a quello continuo and conversione dei relativi parametri del sistema. La procedura classica prevede che il sistema identicato a tempo discreto sia ottenuto per discretizzazione di tipo Zero-Order-Hold (ZOH) del sottostante modello continuo. Quest'assunzione porta a gravi problemi di tipo numerico, poiche la conversione dal discreto al continuo (d2c) richiede il calcolo del logaritmo per una matrice 2n 2n. E' noto che tale operazione comporta problemi di malcondizionamento numerico che producono un amplicazione degli errori di stima nel sistema discreto. La soluzione proposta al problema e di introdurre una nuova tecnica di discretizzazione delle equazioni del moto per sistemi meccanici, introdotta da Veselov, e successivamente sviluppata da J.Marden e dai suoi collaboratori. Questa tecnica e stata sviluppata per sistemi meccanici generici e porta a sistemi discreti caratterizzati da una sorta di "struttura meccanica discreta". Diversamente dalle procedure di discretizzazione classiche, familiari nel mondo del controllo, e.g. ZOH, tale metodo porta a una formula di trasformazione algebrica lineare per il recupero dei parametri continui da quelli discreti. Nella tesi gli integratori variazionali sono applicati ai sistemi meccanici lineari del secondo ordine e verra provato che nella discretizzazione vengono preservate proprieta con intrinseco signicato fisico del modello a tempo continuo, ad esempio la passivita.