Thèses sur le sujet « Representational development »
Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres
Consultez les 50 meilleures thèses pour votre recherche sur le sujet « Representational development ».
À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.
Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.
Parcourez les thèses sur diverses disciplines et organisez correctement votre bibliographie.
Spensley, Mary Fiona. « Representational redescription and the development of cognitive flexibility ». Thesis, Open University, 1995. http://oro.open.ac.uk/56458/.
Texte intégralCampbell, Robin N. « Origins and development of representational systems in early childhood ». Thesis, University of Stirling, 1992. http://hdl.handle.net/1893/21580.
Texte intégralGIOVANELLI, CHIARA LIA. « La relazione madre-bambino : mentalizzazione e comunicazione materne e competenze relazionali e rappresentazionali infantili ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/81729.
Texte intégralButler, Cathal. « Evaluating the utility and validity of the representational redescription model as a general model for cognitive development ». Thesis, University of Hertfordshire, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/2299/2455.
Texte intégralMorrison, Sheila. « The relationship between the development of representational perspective-taking and children's understanding of incongruity-resolution humour ». Thesis, University of Strathclyde, 2005. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.424342.
Texte intégralPenner, David Edward. « The development of representational change from three to five years of age in two domains of knowledge ». Thesis, University of British Columbia, 1989. http://hdl.handle.net/2429/28452.
Texte intégralEducation, Faculty of
Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education (ECPS), Department of
Graduate
Ilia, Nikos. « The development of European identity in Cyprus : investigating content and processes in a specific representational and historical context ». Thesis, Queen's University Belfast, 2012. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.579726.
Texte intégralBrown, John F. « The acquisition of number concepts : the role of the representational number line in the development of numerical cognition on preschool children ». Thesis, University of Bristol, 2009. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.508051.
Texte intégralWatt, Sarah Jean. « Teaching algebra-based concepts to students with learning disabilities : the effects of preteaching using a gradual instructional sequence ». Diss., University of Iowa, 2013. https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/2658.
Texte intégralNielson, Dylan Miles. « The Development and Application of Multivariate Analyses for Guiding Clinical Interventions and Mapping Representations of Human Memory ». The Ohio State University, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1428580015.
Texte intégralCharles-Elie, Simon. « Development of a tool allowing to create and use JSON schemas so as to enhance the validation of existing projects ». Thesis, KTH, Skolan för informations- och kommunikationsteknik (ICT), 2017. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:kth:diva-210744.
Texte intégralThompson, Clarissa Ann. « The Representational Alignment Hypothesis of Transfer of Numerical Representations ». The Ohio State University, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1211376719.
Texte intégralPimenta, Marcelo Soares. « Um modelo canonico de ferramenta para desenvolvimento de interface com o usuário ». reponame:Biblioteca Digital de Teses e Dissertações da UFRGS, 1991. http://hdl.handle.net/10183/117095.
Texte intégralHuman-computer interactionf interface f lS a mul tidisciplinary and research issue. also named use r relatively recent An important criteria to user interface design is the separation of interactive program in two components: computational component (application) and dialogue component (which implements the user interface). This separationf interface named dialogue independencef independent creates the user designer role of application programmer role and new components-user communications. The dialogue component is usually constructed by using some User Interface Development Tool (abreviated FIU) to both user interface definition and manipulation. The comercial FIUs available (most of them are toolboxes like MicroSoft Windows and Macintosh Toolbox) f howeverf are often not so easily usable, since they contain literally hundreds of procedures and they confuse the interface designer and application programmer roles. Thus the desirable dialogue independence is prejudiced. Furthermore, an one interactive program is developed directed to use only one specific FIU, since each FIU has its idiosycrasies. In case o f FIU change, several reformulations are needed. The dissertation goal is the purpose of the Canonical FIU. The Canonical FIU allows: a) an user interface definition in more adequate way to its designer-users, more notably the application programmer; and b) interactive programs portability between diferent FIUs. The Canonical FIU main component is its objectoriented representational model, the Canonicus, which contains the needed abstractions to user interface designers. Portability is obtained slnce the Canonical FIU lS an intermediate level between the application and a FIU. The Canonical FIU lS implemented by a translation mechanism, mapping its objects and operations to some subjacent FIU's objects and operations. In this dissertation, the Canonical FIU architecture, its representational model Canonicus and its implementations over two FIUs (MicroSoft Windows and Macintosh Toolbox) are presented.
Tissell, Rachel L. « Child maltreatment risk| Associations with mothers' representations of childhood attachment, trauma, caregiving, and regulation ». Thesis, Mills College, 2016. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10163157.
Texte intégralChild maltreatment models view risk as a complex constellation of factors that emphasize parents’ trauma experiences and regulation processes. Attachment research has shown that mothers’ representations of childhood attachment and caregiving places their children at developmental risk. Studies to date have evaluated contributing factors separately, but little research considers mothers’ past and current experiences combined with relational trauma and familial regulation patterns. The current research adopted an integrated perspective using known maternal risk factors, and extended existing research in several unique ways by examining association with both adult trauma and childhood trauma; caregiving representations; pathological mourning; and capacity for emotion regulation. Seventy-five mothers with children between 19-74 months (40% boys) from diverse socio-economic and cultural backgrounds completed assessments of maltreatment risk, adult attachment, caregiving, relational trauma, parenting stress, and emotion regulation. Maternal representations of attachment were significantly related to risk, with unresolved mothers demonstrating the highest risk. There were significant positive associations between risk and relational trauma – both frequency and subjective distress with parents and partners. Helpless and heightened caregiving representations, parenting stress, and emotion regulation were also all significantly related to risk. This is the first study to consider maltreatment in the context of relational trauma as defined by Bowlby’s (1980) model of pathological mourning. Risk scores were significantly greater for mothers classified in pathological mourning groups than other mothers. These findings punctuate the effects of problems associated with mourning attachment trauma on maternal regulatory capacities and parenting risk. Implications for infant mental health research and intervention are discussed.
DE, FABRITIIS PAOLA. « Lo sviluppo della flessibilità rappresentazionale ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Trieste, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/36599.
Texte intégralPine, Karen Jane. « Implicit and explicit representations in children's learning ». Thesis, University of Hertfordshire, 1997. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.361261.
Texte intégralGraham, S. « Representation development in associative systems ». Thesis, University of Cambridge, 2000. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.599595.
Texte intégralKylili, Anna. « Female Representation and Development : A case study of the gender-sensitive developmental outcomes of female representation in the Rwandan parliament ». Thesis, Uppsala universitet, Statsvetenskapliga institutionen, 2018. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-353081.
Texte intégralKingsbury, Moore Lois Joy. « Reference and representation in Down's syndrome ». Thesis, University of Plymouth, 1996. http://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/2522.
Texte intégralAinsworth, Stephanie. « Development of phonological representations in young children ». Thesis, University of Manchester, 2015. https://www.research.manchester.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/development-of-phonological-representations-in-young-children(91cb6b48-d2fe-46bd-acf4-f0792df69501).html.
Texte intégralSella, Francesco. « Typical and Atypical Development of Numerical Representation ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli studi di Padova, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/11577/3426396.
Texte intégralCome viene rappresentata l’informazione numerica? Recenti ricerche hanno evidenziato il ruolo fondamentale dei sistemi cognitive preverbali nella rappresentazione numerica: l’Object Tracking System (OTS) e l’Approximate Number System (ANS; o Analogue Magnitude System). Il primo è un meccanismo generale che permette di conservare in memoria le caratteristiche spazio-temporali degli stimoli e la sua capacità è limitata (3-4 elementi). Il secondo è un meccanismo quantitativo che rappresenta ogni numerosità come una distribuzione d’attivazione su teorica linea numerica mentale. Nella presente lavoro di tesi, presenteremo diversi studi volti ad indagare il funzionamento di questi meccanismi in interazione con processi di stima numerica e non-numerica in contesto di sviluppo tipico ed atipico. Nello Studio 1.1, abbiamo utilizzato un compito di imitazione per indagare la capacità di concentrarsi spontaneamente sulla numerosità in bambini di 2 ½ anni. I risultati hanno evidenziato come la maggior parte dei bambini adotti un sistema analogico di quantità quando analizzano spontaneamente delle quantità numeriche. La selezione di questo meccanismo è probabilmente legata sia alla minor richiesta di risorse attentive, sia alla disponibilità di altri indizi quantitativi (non numerici) che covariano con la numerosità. Nello Studio 1.2, bambini di 2 ½ anni hanno svolto un compito di categorizzazione per investigare la loro capacità di stimare la grandezza numerica di insiemi. Le stime dei bambini erano indipendenti dalle caratteristiche visive degli elementi dell’insieme (i.e. perimetro o densità) per le quantità dentro il range di OTS (1-4 elementi). Le stime di quantità più grandi (5-9 elementi) erano invece influenzate dalle caratteristiche visive degli stimoli: in particolare, l’aumento del perimetro con densità costante sembra essere la combinazione di caratteristiche visive degli stimoli che fa aumentare maggiormente la percezione di numerosità. Nello Studio 2, bambini prescolari, di prima primaria e di terza primaria dovevano stimare quantità continue, discrete e simboliche. I risultati suggeriscono la presenza di differenti meccanismi coinvolti nella stima di quantità continue rispetto a quelle numeriche (discrete e simboliche). Nello Studio 3, abbiamo utilizzato il paradigma del doppio compito per studiare la relazione tra memoria visiva a breve termine e subitizing. Dai risultati emerge una marcata corrispondenza tra il numero di elementi memorizzati ed il numero di elementi che possono essere velocemente enumerati attraverso il subitizing. Nello Studio 4.1, bambini con diagnosi di Discalculia Evolutiva (DE) in comorbidità con sindrome non verbale (SNV) e bambini con sviluppo tipico hanno svolto un compito di confronto di quantità numeriche. Abbiamo riscontrato un deficit nella discriminazione di numerosità nel gruppo DE-SNV rispetto ai bambini a sviluppo tipico. In particolare, la capacità di OTS sembra essere ridotta nei bambini con DE-SNV rispetto ai bambini a sviluppo tipico. Nello Studio 4.2, bambini con diagnosi di Discalculia Evolutiva (DE) e bambini con sviluppo tipico hanno completato due compiti di stima sulla linea numerica. I bambini con DE hanno mostrato minor precisione nella stima di quantità simboliche suggerendo una rappresentazione numerica deficitaria rispetto al gruppo con sviluppo tipico. Nello Studio 5, ragazzi con sindrome di Down (SD) e bambini con sviluppo tipico pareggiati per età mentale (EM) ed età cronologica (EC) hanno svolto due compiti numerici per valutare le loro abilità di discriminazione numerica e di conteggio. I ragazzi con SD hanno mostrato un deficit nel discriminare piccole quantità, all’interno del range di OTS, rispetto ai bambini a sviluppo tipico pareggiati sia per EM che per EC. Nella comparazione di numerosità più grandi, i ragazzi con SD hanno ottenuto una performance simile ai bambini pareggiati per EM e minore rispetto ai ragazzi pareggiati per EC. Infine, l’abilità di conteggio appare simile tra i partecipanti con SD e i bambini pareggiati per EM.
Suddendorf, Thomas. « On the ontogeny and phylogeny of the representational mind ». Thesis, University of Auckland, 1998. http://wwwlib.umi.com/dissertations/fullcit/9824385.
Texte intégralSubscription resource available via Digital Dissertations only.
Hartin, Travis L. « REPRESENTATIONAL INERTIA IN PRESCHOOLERS’ OBJECT LABEL LEARNING ». Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2011. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1322573040.
Texte intégralKim, Dan. « From Numerosity to Numeral : Development of Mathematical Concepts ». The Ohio State University, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1558020167528072.
Texte intégralRutkowska, Julie Christine. « Perception, action and representation in infancy : a computational approach ». Thesis, University of Sussex, 1987. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.328320.
Texte intégralPinkelman, Hanna. « Self-Concept and Intimacy Through Social Media Representation ». Capital University Honors Theses / OhioLINK, 2021. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=caphonors1607963787746972.
Texte intégralFlynn, Emma. « Theory of mind, representation and executive control ». Thesis, University of Nottingham, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.364662.
Texte intégralThomson, Jennifer Marie. « Phonological representations in dyslexia : nature, influences and development ». Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2004. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1383592/.
Texte intégralLamont, Alexandra Mary. « The development of cognitive representations of musical pitch ». Thesis, University of Cambridge, 1999. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.624265.
Texte intégralMiller, Scott N., D. Phillip Guertin et Lainie R. Levick. « Influences of Map Scale on Drainage Network Representation ». Arizona-Nevada Academy of Science, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/296536.
Texte intégralFigueras, Irma Clots. « Female political representation and economic development in India ». Thesis, London School of Economics and Political Science (University of London), 2006. http://etheses.lse.ac.uk/2692/.
Texte intégralCritten, Sarah. « Spelling and reading representations in children ». Thesis, University of Hertfordshire, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/2299/2260.
Texte intégralMajava, J. (Jukka). « Product development:drivers, stakeholders, and customer representation during early development ». Doctoral thesis, Oulun yliopisto, 2014. http://urn.fi/urn:isbn:9789526205052.
Texte intégralTiivistelmä Tuotekehityksen merkitys yrityksille on kasvanut globaalin kilpailun ja tuote-elinkaarien lyhentymisten myötä. Lukuisista tutkimuksista ja tuotekehitystoiminnan tärkeydestä liiketoiminnalle huolimatta useilla organisaatioilla on edelleen haasteita kehittää markkinoiden ja asiakkaiden tarpeita vastaavia tuotteita. Lisäksi tuotekehityksessä isolle, kansainväliselle, asiakaskunnalle on mukana useita erilaisia sidosryhmiä, joiden tarpeet ovat ristiriidassa keskenään. Tämän takia tuotekehityksen johtamisesta on tullut entistä monimutkaisempaa. Tämä väitöskirja pyrkii parantamaan tuotekehityksen tulosta selventämällä tekijät, jotka ovat syynä tuotekehityksen aloittamiseen yrityksissä, erilaisten ulkoisten ja sisäisten sidosryhmien suhteet näihin tuotekehitysajureihin sekä miten tärkeimpien sidosryhmien tarpeet selvitetään. Tutkimus keskittyy tuotekehityksen alkuvaiheisiin Suomessa toimivissa yrityksissä, joilla on voimakas panostus tuotekehitykseen ja lisäksi merkittävää kansainvälistä toimintaa sekä suuri määrä asiakkaita. Tutkimus tehtiin keräämällä ja analysoimalla tietoa yritysten tuotekehityskäytännöistä sekä tuotantohyödykemarkkinoilla että kuluttajamarkkinoilla. Tietoa kerättiin myös eri teollisuudenaloilta. Tämä väitöskirja perustuu laadulliseen tutkimukseen ja tutkimusaineiston keräämisessä on hyödynnetty kyselytutkimuksia sekä haastatteluita. Tämä väitöskirja osoittaa, että yritysten tuotekehitykseen löytyy useita merkittäviä ajureita. Tämän lisäksi nämä ajurit eroavat merkittävästi projektien, yritysten ja jopa yksittäisten henkilöiden välillä. Tulosten perusteella yritysten pitäisi selvittää, mitkä ajurit ovat niiden liiketoiminnan kannalta olennaisimpia ja tarkentaa niitä tuotekehityksen päätöksenteon ja painopisteen parantamiseksi. Tutkimus paljastaa myös ulkoisten ja sisäisten sidosryhmien suhteet tuotekehitysajureihin sekä tärkeimmät sidosryhmät yksittäisille ajureille. Tulokset osoittavat, että yritysten pitäisi tunnistaa tärkeimmät sidosryhmät projektien ajureiden perusteella ja ottaa ajurit huomioon sidosryhmäjohtamisessa. Tutkimuksen löydökset tukevat aikaisempia tutkimuksia osoittaen asiakkaiden olevan tärkein ulkoinen sidosryhmä tuotekehityksessä, kun taas sisäisistä sidosryhmistä nousee esille tuotehallinta. Tämä väitöskirja osoittaa, että yritysten tuotekehitysprojekteissa tuotehallinnan tulisi johtaa asiakkaita lähellä oleviin sidosryhmiin liittyvää sidosryhmäyhteistyötä. Tuotehallinnan tehtäviin kuuluu asiakkaan määrittely, edustus ja asiakastarpeiden tunnistaminen. Tuotehallinnan pitää myös tehdä yhteistyötä useiden asiakkaisiin liittyvien sidosryhmien kanssa tuotekehityksen aikana. Tämä väitöskirja tarjoaa uuden johtamisen viitekehityksen, joka oikein toteutettuna voi parantaa merkittävästi tuotekehityksen lopputulosta. Projektin ajurien, tärkeimpien sidosryhmien ja asiakastarpeiden selventäminen varmistaa oikeanlaisen keskittymisen asiakkaisiin ja vähentää tarpeettomia monimutkaisuuksia tuotekehityksessä
Ademolu, Edward. « Rethinking audiences : visual representations of Africa and the Nigerian diaspora ». Thesis, University of Manchester, 2018. https://www.research.manchester.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/rethinking-audiences-visual-representations-of-africa-and-the-nigerian-diaspora(6bac4536-7660-4412-bcc5-c703bd39b9e2).html.
Texte intégralLeith, Jennifer G. « Representation, resources and resettlement as development in eastern Indonesia ». Thesis, University of East Anglia, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.365161.
Texte intégralMavridis, Lazaros. « High throughput virtual drug screening using spherical harmonic molecular surface representations ». Thesis, Available from the University of Aberdeen Library and Historic Collections Digital Resources, 2009. http://digitool.abdn.ac.uk:80/webclient/DeliveryManager?application=DIGITOOL-3&owner=resourcediscovery&custom_att_2=simple_viewer&pid=25936.
Texte intégralChorlton, Elizabeth Jane. « A qualitative study exploring children's illness representations : a developmental and cultural perspective ». Thesis, University of Surrey, 2000. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.325882.
Texte intégralSales, Michael F. « Context Dependent Numerosity Representations in Children ». The Ohio State University, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1557146188226533.
Texte intégralChild, Simon Frederick James. « Investigating the development of cognitive symbolic representation and gestural communication ». Thesis, University of Manchester, 2011. https://www.research.manchester.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/investigating-the-development-of-cognitive-symbolic-representation-and-gestural-communication(225d27ca-0dcb-4205-974b-e57e1547114d).html.
Texte intégralSkerry, Amy Elizabeth. « Abstract Representations of Attributed Emotion : Evidence From Neuroscience and Development ». Thesis, Harvard University, 2015. http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:17467378.
Texte intégralPsychology
Foster, Robin. « Children's use of apparatus in the development of the concept of number ». Thesis, University of Warwick, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.288541.
Texte intégralTuerk, Arin Samantha. « Continuities and discontinuities in working memory representations of collections over ontogeny ». Thesis, Harvard University, 2014. http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:13129561.
Texte intégralPsychology
Hjelmervik, Ove Rustung. « ICT-supported knowledge representation for Development of Routines in industry ». Doctoral thesis, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Department of Productions and Quality Engineering, 2007. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:no:ntnu:diva-1467.
Texte intégralThe ability to develop operating routines through the support of information and communication technology (ICT) is being valued by the business community as a source of competitive advantage in the information economy; and research concerning the facilitating role of such technology in relation to organizational learning and development of routines is therefore required. In this thesis the focus is directed at the relationship between communication technology and the development of routines in an industrial organization leading to enhanced value creation. The impact computer-supported knowledge representation has on an organization’s ability to change through development of operating routines are addressed, and propositions concerning the effects on different aspects of communication technology (e.g. system structure and functionality) and organizational environment (e.g. organizational learning, empowerment, systemic innovation, and absorptive capacity) are developed. The moderating role (i.e. the learning mechanisms) of an organization’s ability to learn from, and share, experience within a multilevel nested organizational structure is also discussed and evaluated.
The main goal of this study has been to reveal and explain how operating routines are developed and learned through ICT-supported knowledge representation, and on this basis build concepts and methods that can be used to improve the development of operating routines in business organizations. In this context we have developed a deliberate organizational learning model (DOLM). The main contributions of this work are the following:
• Development of operating routines may be enhanced through computercommunication given a multilevel nested iterative organization structure applying an ICT-supported deliberate organizational learning model.
• Empowered employees are willing to participate in the development of routines through such communication by sharing experience that may impact on operative and strategic activities, resulting in enhance productivity.
• Employees participating directly, or indirectly, in the design of ICT systems are positive to applying computers for the purpose of organizational learning and development of routines.
• Because of their capacity to absorb new knowledge within a context specific domain, experienced operational personnel understand new routines presented through ICTsupported deliberate organizational learning structure.
Many people take it for granted that computers support organizational learning, yet to the best of our knowledge little empirical proof has been forthcoming through the literature. We will argue that the development of operating routines can be enhanced through the application of a computer-based deliberate organizational learning model. Furthermore, our case identifies a multilevel nested iterative organization structure as a contributing mechanism for such a model to succeed. The current theory on empowerment does not say anything about employees’ willingness to apply ICT, nor does it suggest that employees are willing to share experience through the application of ICT. Our findings clearly indicate that empowered employees are applying ICT in the pursuit of developing routines and are willing to share experience through computers. Furthermore, our findings suggest systemic innovation theory to include employees that are indirectly participating in the design of systems as being positive to using computers. Such indirect participation includes employees knowing of colleagues participating in system design. While some theories argue that employees learn new routines through story-telling within a community-of-practice (COP), our data indicate that new operating routines transferred to experienced operators through ICT can be learned. Experienced operators learn new routines through having an absorptive capacity because knowledge will diffuse more rapidly among employees who have prior experience.
Our case study shows that organizations can develop operating routines supported by knowledge represented in ICT. This research contributes to the understanding that development of routines can take place through an ICT-supported deliberate organizational learning model applied within an employee-empowered multilevel nested iterative organization structure.
A best practice knowledge management (KM) system representing the firm’s operating routines is studied over time as it is being implemented in the business units within a corporation. Our focus is on change processes through development of operating routines by studying how the organization can learn from its experience, share such experience and from accumulated experience develop new routines. This thesis is a longitudinal explorative case study, basing its findings on in-dept interviews at operator, middle and senior management levels. We are basing our observations primarily on the cognitive/behavioural organisational learning theory. Based on our observations we mapped and analyzed if, how and under which circumstances an organization, supported by ICT-represented knowledge, is able to develop operating routines and thus enhance the value creation in the company. On this basis we have developed a set of “within-case” propositions. These propositions predict how and under which circumstances organizations may learn through the support of ICT, leading to development of operating processes and routines for the purpose of enhancing value creation in business organizations.
Some literature argues that knowledge is tacit and organizations learn only through practice. Our findings cannot confirm this. We have through the application of the cognitive/behavioural theory tested out organizational learning. Our research indicates that in context specific situations experienced employees can learn new routines through computer systems support. However, in order for organizations to learn, it is not enough to just implement a computer system. Our findings suggest a need for the implementation of a strategic process where the development of an integrated DOLM is the objective. Furthermore, certain organizational structures need to be in place for such a system to be applied resulting in capturing and sharing accumulated experience. In this sense strategy, change processes, and KM systems are intrinsically linked.
This research is based on a case study of Hydro Aluminium’s BestPracticeSystem (BPS), a successful in-house developed enterprise KM system implemented in the period 2003/4. The case study demonstrates the usefulness of the model to support change processes through development of operating routines, and the improvement in productivity that can be achieved by implementing a deliberate organizational learning model in conjunction with a process oriented manufacturing practice. Knowledge represented through ICT can drive value creation.
Nicol, Craig Andrew. « Development and exploration of a timbre space representation of audio ». Thesis, University of Glasgow, 2005. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.426612.
Texte intégralMatin, A. « Tactic representation and utilisation to shorten product development project plans ». Thesis, Cranfield University, 1994. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.386166.
Texte intégralMaddipudi, Koushik. « Efficient Architectures for Retrieving Mixed Data with Rest Architecture Style and HTML5 Support ». TopSCHOLAR®, 2013. http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses/1251.
Texte intégralSorokina, Alfia. « Re-Inventing the Past, Defining the Future : Historical Representations and Regional Development in the Russian Northwest ». Thesis, Université d'Ottawa / University of Ottawa, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10393/12910.
Texte intégralBorba, Rute Elizabete de Souza Rosa. « The effect of number meanings, conceptual invariants and symbolic representations on children's reasoning about directed numbers ». Thesis, Oxford Brookes University, 2002. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.247603.
Texte intégralMarciani, Francesca. « Numeric Memory : Developing Representations ». The Ohio State University, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1365697597.
Texte intégralGabriel, Florence. « Mental representations of fractions : development, stable state, learning difficulties and intervention ». Doctoral thesis, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/2013/ULB-DIPOT:oai:dipot.ulb.ac.be:2013/209933.
Texte intégralBased on some recent research questions and intense debates in the literature, a first behavioural study examined the mental representations of the magnitude of fractions in educated adults. Behavioural observations from adults can indeed provide a first clue to explain the paradox raised by fractions. Contrary perhaps to most educated adults’ intuition, finding the value of a given fraction is not an easy operation. Fractions are complex symbols, and there is an on-going debate in the literature about how their magnitude (i.e. value) is processed. In a first study, we asked adult volunteers to decide as quickly as possible whether two fractions represent the same magnitude or not. Equivalent fractions (e.g. 1/4 and 2/8) were identified as representing the same number only about half of the time. In another experiment, adults were also asked to decide which of two fractions was larger. This paradigm offered different results, suggesting that participants relied on both the global magnitude of the fraction and the magnitude of the components. Our results showed that fraction processing depends on experimental conditions. Adults appear to use the global magnitude only in restricted circumstances, mostly with easy and familiar fractions.
In another study, we investigated the development of the mental representations of the magnitude of fractions. Previous studies in adults showed that fraction processing can be either based on the magnitude of the numerators and denominators or based on the global magnitude of fractions and the magnitude of their components. The type of processing depends on experimental conditions. In this experiment, 5th, 6th, 7th-graders, and adults were tested with two paradigms. First, they performed a same/different task. Second, they carried out a numerical comparison task in which they had to decide which of two fractions was larger. Results showed that 5th-graders do not rely on the representations of the global magnitude of fractions in the Numerical Comparison task, but those representations develop from grade 6 until grade 7. In the Same/Different task, participants only relied on componential strategies. From grade 6 on, pupils apply the same heuristics as adults in fraction magnitude comparison tasks. Moreover, we have shown that correlations between global distance effect and children’s general fraction achievement were significant.
Fractions are well known to represent a stumbling block for primary school children. In a third study, we tried to identify the difficulties encountered by primary school pupils. We observed that most 4th and 5th-graders had only a very limited notion of the meaning of fractions, basically referring to pieces of cakes or pizzas. The fraction as a notation for numbers appeared particularly hard to grasp.
Building upon these results, we designed an intervention programme. The intervention “From Pies to Numbers” aimed at improving children’s understanding of fractions as numbers. The intervention was based on various games in which children had to estimate, compare, and combine fractions represented either symbolically or as figures. 20 game sessions distributed over 3 months led to 15-20% improvement in tests assessing children's capacity to estimate and compare fractions; conversely, children in the control group who received traditional lessons improved more in procedural skills such as simplification of fractions and arithmetic operations with fractions. Thus, a short classroom intervention inducing children to play with fractions improved their conceptual understanding.
The results are discussed in light of recent research on the mental representation of the magnitude of fractions and educational theories. The importance of multidisciplinary approaches in psychology and education was also discussed.
In sum, by combining behavioural experiments in adults and children, and intervention studies, we hoped to have improved the understanding how the brain processes mathematical symbols, while helping teachers get a better grasp of pupils’ difficulties and develop classroom activities that suit the needs of learners.
Doctorat en Sciences Psychologiques et de l'éducation
info:eu-repo/semantics/nonPublished