Sommaire
Littérature scientifique sur le sujet « Proclitics-Enclitic »
Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres
Consultez les listes thématiques d’articles de revues, de livres, de thèses, de rapports de conférences et d’autres sources académiques sur le sujet « Proclitics-Enclitic ».
À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.
Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.
Articles de revues sur le sujet "Proclitics-Enclitic"
Rezac, Milan. « Mihi est from Brythonic to Breton II ». Indogermanische Forschungen 126, no 1 (8 novembre 2021) : 325–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/if-2021-014.
Texte intégralRezac, Milan. « Mihi est from Brythonic to Breton I ». Indogermanische Forschungen 125, no 1 (1 novembre 2020) : 313–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/if-2020-013.
Texte intégralRusso, Michela. « The possessive enclitics with kinship nouns in Italo-Romance and the possessive determiners in the Francoprovençal of Faeto ». Zeitschrift für romanische Philologie 137, no 1 (1 mars 2021) : 217–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/zrp-2021-0007.
Texte intégralHock, Hans Henrich. « Backernagel is Wackernagel Lite. On the “P-Minus 2” Clitics of Santali ». Lingua Posnaniensis 55, no 2 (1 décembre 2013) : 67–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/linpo-2013-0014.
Texte intégralDemon, Yosef, et Veronika Genua. « Inflexive Marker and Morphophonological Processes in the Lamalera Dialect of Lamaholot Language (LDLL) ». Randwick International of Education and Linguistics Science Journal 2, no 3 (27 septembre 2021) : 383–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.47175/rielsj.v2i3.306.
Texte intégralSetiani, Rulik, et Dewi Sri Kuning. « The Use of Clitics In Novel “Murder on The Orient Express” By Agatha Christee ». Edukasi Lingua Sastra 20, no 1 (29 avril 2022). http://dx.doi.org/10.47637/elsa.v20i1.495.
Texte intégralThèses sur le sujet "Proclitics-Enclitic"
MORNATI, GIULIA. « THE SENSITIVITY AND PRODUCTION OF ARTICLES AND 3rd PERSON DIRECT OBJECT CLITICS : EVIDENCE FROM EYE-MOVEMENTS, ERP RECORDINGS AND ONLINE TESTS IN ITALIAN CHILDREN AND TODDLERS ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2022. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/379211.
Texte intégralFunction words and inflectional morphemes do not have an independent meaning but are highly frequent and usually monosyllabic. Among these, we could find articles, clitic pronouns, nominal and verbal suffixes. They convey structural information and are a critical aid in language acquisition and processing. Although previous research has demonstrated that infants are sensitive to function words from their first months of life, studies on their sensitivity and production in Italian are scarce. This thesis aims to study the sensitivity and production of Italian infants and toddlers to articles and 3rd direct object (3DO) clitics. This choice is motivated by the fact that these two morphemes are critical in the detection of language problems, especially 3DO, in monolingual and bilingual children. Chapter 3 investigated the early sensitivity to articles and 3DO clitics. By recording eye movements, we analysed whether 12 months old infants distinguished between real and pseudo-articles and whether 20 months old toddlers distinguished between real and pseudo-clitics. Results showed that, concerning the articles, infants distinguished between real and pseudo articles, preferring to look at the screen when they heard a real article. Moreover, at already 12 months of age, Italian infants had acquired both definite and indefinite articles. No significant results emerged with 3DO clitics due, probably, to the methodology used. Chapters 4 and 5 deeply investigated the use of an inflection morpheme, gender, on articles in language processing. Through a Looking While Listening task, we showed that infants as young as 12 months could extract the gender information carried by articles to anticipate the upcoming noun (Chapter 4). Finally, ERP recordings showed that children as young as 24 months old could detect a gender violation between a picture and the associated sentence already when they heard that the article did not match the picture presented: they showed a very early posterior positivity (Chapter 5). Finally, we studied the production of 3DO clitics by presenting a helpful computerised test for screening the production of 3DO clitics both in the typical developing (TD) children and in children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD). The test had a good sensitivity in identifying children with DLD. Moreover, this test showed interesting results in the bilingual population: although children who acquire Italian as a second language struggle with the acquisition of 3DO clitics, TD bilingual children performed as well as monolingual peers at the clitic test (Chapter 6). Finally, we have deepened the study of 3DO clitics’ production by analysing the differences in the production of proclitics and enclitics in children aged 4 to 7 years with and without familiarity with language and learning impairments (LLI). Results showed that enclitics were easier to produce at all ages. Moreover, children with a positive family history for LLI produced fewer clitics than children without familiarity, especially at 4 and 5 years.