Thèses sur le sujet « Memoria verbale »
Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres
Consultez les 50 meilleures thèses pour votre recherche sur le sujet « Memoria verbale ».
À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.
Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.
Parcourez les thèses sur diverses disciplines et organisez correctement votre bibliographie.
COMI, ALESSANDRO. « Memoria verbale nei pazienti con glioma cerebrale ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/64636.
Texte intégralGreenauer, Nathan Micheal. « THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL PROCESSING ON SPATIAL MEMORIES : VERBAL OVERSHADOWNIG AND SPATIAL REPRESENTATIONS ». Miami University / OhioLINK, 2006. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=miami1153938077.
Texte intégralGarley, Claire Louise. « The effect of verbal memory impairments on memory for narrative ». Thesis, Royal Holloway, University of London, 2004. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.414062.
Texte intégralKalm, Kristjan. « Chunk formation in verbal short term memory ». Thesis, University of Cambridge, 2012. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.609987.
Texte intégralDewick, Hilary Clare. « Verbal and visual memory in Parkinson's disease ». Thesis, University of Liverpool, 1991. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.292950.
Texte intégralThorn, Annabel S. C. « Language specialisation in verbal short-term memory ». Thesis, University of Bristol, 1998. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.266906.
Texte intégralMikan, Kathrin Angela Maria. « Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning ». Thesis, University of Sussex, 2013. http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/44799/.
Texte intégralGreenauer, Nathan Michael. « The effects of verbal processing on spatial memories verbal overshadownig [sic] and spatial representations / ». Oxford, Ohio : Miami University, 2006. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=miami1153938077.
Texte intégralUguccioni, Ginevra. « La somniloquie : un modèle pour l'étude de la consolidation mnésique verbale pendant le sommeil ». Thesis, Paris 6, 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015PA066271/document.
Texte intégralAccording to the replay theory, sleep improves memory consolidation of recent learning through their reactivation. To test this hypothesis, we used the model of sleep-talking: the words uttered reflecting the mental content of the sleeper and the information he is proceeding. Sleep-talking occur frequently within the context of slow wave sleep (sleepwalking) or REM sleep parasomnias (RBD). We first showed how these two parasomnias corresponded to the setting gestures and words of the mental content of the sleeper, with predominance during slow wave sleep of dreams featuring disasters that sleepwalkers fleeing, and during REM sleep of dreams of attacks by animals or people that subjects counterattack. This supports the concept of evolutionary function of dreams as a virtual drive to “fight or flight” threats. Then, we used sleep-talking to test if a recent verbal was not only consolidated during sleep but also if some words were repeated while sleeping. We first showed that verbal memory consolidation related to sleep was preserved in sleepwalkers as in patients with RBD, even with dementia, compared with normal subjects. Then, we haven’t identified rehearsed sentences of the material learned the day before during slow wave sleep parasomnias, but we identified in a subject with RBD, a semantic component evoking a rehearse of the context of the learned history. Finally, we collected 883 verbatim during sleep and describes the acoustic-phonetic, prosodic and semantic aspects of sleep-talking
Woods, Kristin Renee. « Death-primed memory suppression / ». Read thesis online, 2008. http://library.uco.edu/UCOthesis/WoodsK2008.pdf.
Texte intégralMacDonald, Ian. « Manual and verbal control processes in working memory ». Thesis, Oxford Brookes University, 1997. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.363451.
Texte intégralGreaves, Martin Henry. « Rehearsal-based strategies for updating verbal working memory ». Thesis, University of Hertfordshire, 2008. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.485646.
Texte intégralLobley, Kathryn J. « Working memory, verbal complex span and reading comprehension ». Thesis, University of Bristol, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.390791.
Texte intégralWilliams, Bethany R. « The effects of verbal and nonverbal interference on recall ». Diss., Online access via UMI:, 2006.
Trouver le texte intégralAntonopoulou, Paraskevi. « Spatial descriptions and verbal reasoning problems ». Thesis, University of Sussex, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.390915.
Texte intégralAlkozei, Anna, Ryan Smith, Natalie S. Dailey, Sahil Bajaj et William D. S. Killgore. « Acute exposure to blue wavelength light during memory consolidation improves verbal memory performance ». PUBLIC LIBRARY SCIENCE, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/625801.
Texte intégralChipman, Karen Anne. « No sex difference on incidental picture memory, despite better verbal memory in women ». Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1998. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/tape15/PQDD_0007/MQ32474.pdf.
Texte intégralLetelier, Ruiz Diego, Contreras Pablo Muñoz et Silva Belén Vásquez. « Desempeño de la memoria de trabajo verbal en sujetos con afasia y su asociación al déficit en la comprensión auditiva ». Tesis, Universidad de Chile, 2013. http://www.repositorio.uchile.cl/handle/2250/116837.
Texte intégralIntroduction: Language is an essential skill for the humans because it is the basis of communication. Within the higher brain functions, memory and language are subject to be altered as may be evidenced in aphasia, in this can exist problems with listening comprehension, verbal working memory and language in general. Objectives: This study aims to evaluate, relate and compare the deficit of working memory and alterations in the listening comprehension in fluent and non-fluent aphasic subjects. Methodology: The sample consists of 10 subjects with fluent aphasia and 6 non-fluent with neurologic diagnosis of ischemic or hemorrhagic cerebrovascular accident of middle left cerebral artery, without sensory or cognitive impairments. Data collection was performed using the clinical record, the implementation of the Token Test and Spam direct digits of the Wechsler's Memory Scale. Results: Not exist statistically significant differences in the results in working memory and listening comprehension between fluent and non-fluent aphasic, also not exist a significant relationship between performance in working memory and auditive comprehension deficits. Discussion: A qualitative analysis indicates variations in the performances of the two groups of aphasic in working memory and listening comprehension, this is explained for the anatomical localization of function before evaluated and the lesion sites of the different types of aphasia.
Ternes, Marguerite. « Verbal credibility assesment of incarcerated violent offenders' memory reports ». Thesis, University of British Columbia, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/2429/6660.
Texte intégralNakabayashi, Kazuyo. « The role of verbal processing in face recognition memory ». Thesis, University of Glasgow, 2005. http://theses.gla.ac.uk/1268/.
Texte intégralAnnett, Judith Marion. « Effects of visual and verbal suppression on olfactory memory ». Thesis, University of Ulster, 1993. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.357673.
Texte intégralCaffrey, Jill Teresa. « Verbal learning and memory in closed head injured individuals ». Diss., The University of Arizona, 1992. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/185964.
Texte intégralAlnajashi, Sumyah Abdullah Ibrahim. « The role of the verbal code in visual memory ». Thesis, University of Leeds, 2013. http://etheses.whiterose.ac.uk/5004/.
Texte intégralSchooler, Jonathan Wolf. « Verbalizing non-verbal memories : some things are better left unsaid / ». Thesis, Connect to this title online ; UW restricted, 1987. http://hdl.handle.net/1773/9054.
Texte intégralFredriksen, Lauren E. « The Impact of Memory Stereotype Threat on Memory and Memory Self-Efficacy in Older Adults ». Xavier University Psychology / OhioLINK, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=xupsy1593210266566016.
Texte intégralGiles, David Clifford. « Visual memory and spelling in 13 year olds ». Thesis, University of Bristol, 1996. http://hdl.handle.net/1983/18a85c9b-bb40-4f62-a23f-77988ca36405.
Texte intégralDomahs, Frank. « Semantische Repräsentation, obligatorische Aktivierung und verbale Produktion arithmetischer Fakten ». Phd thesis, Universität Potsdam, 2006. http://opus.kobv.de/ubp/volltexte/2007/1250/.
Texte intégralThe present thesis deals with the representation and processing of arithmetic facts. This domain of semantic knowledge has gained a substantial amount of interest as its components as well as their interrelations are well specified. Thus, cognitive models can be developed with a degree of precision, which cannot be reached in many other domains. Most recent models agree that arithmetic facts are represented in an associative, network-like structure in declarative memory. Despite this general agreement a lot of issues still remain unresolved. The open questions tackled in the present work address three different aspects of arithmetic facts: 1) their neuro-anatomical correlates, 2) neighbourhood consistency effects in their verbal production and 3) their automatic activation. In a combined behavioural and fMRI study the neurofunctional correlates of the acquisition of arithmetic facts in adults were examined. This research was based on the Triple-Code-Model of Dehaene and Cohen, the only recent model which makes explicit assumptions on neuroanatomical correlates of numerical abilities. The Triple-Code-Model assumes that a “perisylvian” region in the left hemisphere including the basal ganglia and the Angular Gyrus is involved in the retrieval of arithmetic facts (Dehaene & Cohen, 1995; Dehaene & Cohen, 1997; Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, & Cohen, 2003). In the present study healthy adults were asked to train complex multiplication problems extensively during one week. Thus, these problems could be solved more and more automatically. It was reasoned that answering these trained problems should more and more rely on the retrieval of facts from declarative memory, whereas answering untrained problems should rely on the application of strategies and procedures, which impose high demands on executive functions including working memory. After the training was finished, participants – as expected – could solve trained problems faster and more accurately than non-trained problems. Participants were also submitted to a functional magnetic resonance imaging examination. In general, this examination added to the evidence for a mainly left hemispheric fronto-parietal network being involved in mental multiplication. Crucially, comparing trained with non-trained problems a shift of activation from frontal to more parietal regions was observed. Thus, the central role of central executive and working memory for complex calculation was highlighted. Moreover, a shift of activation from the Intraparietal Sulcus to the Angular Gyrus took place within the parietal lobe. According to the Triple-Code-Model, this shift may be interpreted to indicate a strategy change from quantity based calculation, relying on the Intraparietal Sulcus, to fact retrieval, relying on the left Angular Gyrus. Are there neighbourhood consistency effects in the verbal production of arithmetic facts similar to what has been described for language production? According to the “Triangle Model” of simple multiplication, proposed by Verguts & Fias (2004), such effects can be expected. According to this model corrects answers can be given more easily if they share digits with many semantically close wrong answers. Moreover, it can be assumed that wrong answers, too, are more likely to be produced if they share a digit with the correct result. In addition to this, the Triangle Model also states that the classical problem size effect in simple multiplication (Zbrodoff & Logan, 2004) can be drawn back to neighbourhood consistency between the correct result and semantically close wrong answers. In fact, a re-analysis of error data from a sample of healthy young adults (Campbell, 1997) and a patient with acalculia (Domahs, Bartha, & Delazer, 2003) provided evidence for the existence of decade consistency effects in the verbal production of multiplication results. Healthy participants and the patient produced significantly more wrong answers which shared the decade digit with the correct result than otherwise comparable wrong answers. This result supports the assumption of separate representations of decade and unit digits in two-digit numbers in multiplication (Verguts & Fias, 2004) and in number processing in general (Nuerk, Weger, & Willmes, 2001; Nuerk & Willmes, 2005). Moreover, an additional regression analysis on the error rates provided first empirical evidence for the hypothesis that the classical problem size effect in the retrieval of multiplication facts may be an artefact of neighbourhood consistency: Although problem size was the first variable to enter the model, it was excluded from the model once a measure for neighbourhood consistency was included. Finally, in a further study the automatic activation of multiplication facts was examined in a number matching task (Galfano, Rusconi, & Umilta, 2003; Lefevre, Bisanz, & Mrkonjic, 1988; Thibodeau, Lefevre, & Bisanz, 1996). This experiment addressed the question how the automatic activation of actual multiplication results (Thibodeau et al., 1996) relates to the activation of semantically close wrong answers (Galfano et al., 2003). Furthermore, using different SOAs the temporal properties of these activations should be disclosed. In general, the results of this study provide evidence for an obligatory and automatic activation of a network of arithmetic facts in healthy educated adults in which correct results are stronger associated with the operands than semantically related wrong answers. Crucially, products of small problems lead to stronger interference effects than products of larger problems while operand errors of large problems lead to stronger interference effects than operand errors of small problems. Such a pattern of activation is in line with predictions of Siegler’s Distribution of Associations Model (Lemaire & Siegler, 1995; Siegler, 1988) which assumes a more peaked distribution of associations between operands and potential results for small compared to large multiplication problems. In sum, the present thesis should shed some light into largely ignored aspects of arithmetic fact retrieval: The neural correlates of its acquisition, the consequences of its implementation in the base 10 place value system, as well as the specific effects of its semantic representation for automatic activation of correct multiplication facts and related results. References Campbell, J. I. (1997). On the relation between skilled performance of simple division and multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1140-1159. Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83-120. Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1997). Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex, 33, 219-250. Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 487-506. Domahs, F., Bartha, L., & Delazer, M. (2003). Rehabilitation of arithmetic abilities: Different intervention strategies for multiplication. Brain and Language, 87, 165-166. Galfano, G., Rusconi, E., & Umilta, C. (2003). Automatic activation of multiplication facts: evidence from the nodes adjacent to the product. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 56, 31-61. Lefevre, J. A., Bisanz, J., & Mrkonjic, L. (1988). Cognitive arithmetic: evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts. Memory and Cognition, 16, 45-53. Lemaire, P. & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to children's learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 83-97. Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82, B25-B33. Nuerk, H. C. & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers. Psychology Science, 47, 52-72. Siegler, R. S. (1988). Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 258-275. Thibodeau, M. H., Lefevre, J. A., & Bisanz, J. (1996). The extension of the interference effect to multiplication. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 393-396. Verguts, T. & Fias, W. (2004). Neighborhood Effects in Mental Arithmetic. Psychology Science. Zbrodoff, N. J. & Logan, G. D. (2004). What everyone finds: The problem-size effect. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp.331-345). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Smith, Richard J. Smith. « A Fuzzy-Trace Theory Approach to Exploring Verbal Overshadowing ». Miami University / OhioLINK, 2017. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=miami1496137671942431.
Texte intégralAntoine, Sophie. « The spatial nature of ordinal information in verbal working memory ». Doctoral thesis, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/2013/ULB-DIPOT:oai:dipot.ulb.ac.be:2013/238833.
Texte intégralDoctorat en Sciences psychologiques et de l'éducation
info:eu-repo/semantics/nonPublished
Hoffman, Paul. « The impact of semantic impairements on verbal short-term memory ». Thesis, University of Manchester, 2008. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.489526.
Texte intégralGeorge, Anjali. « Verbal short-term memory and fluid vocabulary skills in toddlers ». Thesis, University of Canterbury. Department of Communication Disorders, 2012. http://hdl.handle.net/10092/6709.
Texte intégralHartley, Tom. « The role of syllable structure in verbal short-term memory ». Thesis, University College London (University of London), 1995. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1317531/.
Texte intégralGilbert, Rebecca. « Temporal properties of rehearsal in auditory-verbal short-term memory ». Thesis, University of York, 2014. http://etheses.whiterose.ac.uk/8301/.
Texte intégralWood, James Michael. « Implicit and explicit memory for verbal stimuli presented during sleep ». Diss., The University of Arizona, 1990. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/185128.
Texte intégralOyler, James Douglas. « Verbal Learning and Memory Functions in Students with Reading Disabilities ». Diss., The University of Arizona, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/194257.
Texte intégralLeung, Tsz-yan, et 梁芷欣. « Effects of cognitive training on enhancing verbal and visual-spatial working memory in older adults at risk of cognitive decline ». Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10722/208055.
Texte intégralLazendic, Goran Psychology Faculty of Science UNSW. « The locus and source of verbal associations ». Awarded by:University of New South Wales. School of Psychology, 2006. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/25150.
Texte intégralCornuéjols, Martine. « La memoire semantique et ses modes d'acces (verbal, image) : approche pluridisciplinaire ». Paris 11, 1999. http://www.theses.fr/1999PA112067.
Texte intégralNyberg, Sebastian, et Ata Pirmoradi. « A Study of Verbal and Action Memory among Athletes and Non Athletes ». Thesis, Örebro University, School of Law, Psychology and Social Work, 2008. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:oru:diva-6178.
Texte intégralMello, Rael Dill de. « Avaliação das relações entre a memória de trabalho verbal e visuoespacial de adultos saudáveis ». reponame:Repositório Institucional da UFPR, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/1884/45783.
Texte intégralDissertação (mestrado) - Universidade Federal do Paraná, Setor de Ciências Humanas, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia. Defesa: Curitiba, 16/12/2016
Inclui referências : f. 43-59
Linha de pesquisa: Avaliação e reabilitação neuropsicológica
Resumo: A Memória de Trabalho é a ação combinada entre atenção e memória que regula o fluxo de informações sustentadas para a realização de uma atividade, possibilitando reter e manipular informações por um curto período de tempo. O Teste Dígitos, da Escala Wechesler de Inteligência para Adultos, é indicado para avaliar a capacidade de memória de trabalho verbal, enquanto que o Teste dos Cubos de Corsi (TCC) é tido como avaliador da memória de trabalho visuoespacial. Esta pesquisa teve como objetivo avaliar a relação entre estas duas estruturas cognitivas em 107 adultos jovens saudáveis divididos em 2 grupos etários. Além dos testes supracitados, foram utilizados um questionário estruturado e a Escala Wechsler de Inteligência Abreviada. Os resultados indicam que os testes de memória de trabalho verbal e visuoespacial apresentam níveis de dificuldades diferentes na ordem inversa, sendo o TCC mais fácil do que o Dígitos. Estes achados corroboram pesquisa anteriores que questionam a analogia não verbal do TCC ao Dígitos. Palavras-chave: Avaliação Neuropsicológica; Memória de Trabalho; Cubos de Corsi; Saúde Mental.
Abstract: Working Memory is the combined action of attention and memory that regulates the flow of sustained information to perform an activity, allowing to retain and manipulate information for a short period of time. The Digit Span, from the Wechesler Adult Intelligence Scale, is a test indicated to evaluate verbal work memory capacity, while the Corsi Block Test (CBT) is considered as a visual space memory assessor. This study aimed to evaluate the relationship between these two cognitive structures in 107 healthy young adults divided into 2 age groups. In addition to the aforementioned tests, a structured questionnaire and the Wechsler Scale of Abbreviated Intelligence were used. The results indicate that verbal and visuospatial work memory tests have different difficulty levels in the reverse order, with CBT being easier than Digits. These findings corroborate previous research that questions the nonverbal analogy of CBT to Digit Span. Keywords: Neuropsychological Assessment; Working Memory; Corsi Blocks Tapping Task; Mental Health.
Ng, Li Huang Honey. « Evaluating models of verbal serial short-term memory using temporal grouping phenomena ». University of Western Australia. School of Psychology, 2007. http://theses.library.uwa.edu.au/adt-WU2008.0059.
Texte intégralIvarsson, Magnus, et Stefan Strohmayer. « Working memory training improves arithmetic skills and verbal working memory capacity in children with ADHD ». Thesis, Stockholms universitet, Psykologiska institutionen, 2010. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-49618.
Texte intégralKornisch, Myriam. « Visual and Verbal Short-Term Memory Correlates of Variability in Vocabulary Size ». Thesis, University of Canterbury. Communication Disorders, 2012. http://hdl.handle.net/10092/6737.
Texte intégralWilson, Anna J. « The role of spatial and verbal working memory in approximation and addition / ». view abstract or download file of text, 2003. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/uoregon/fullcit?p3102195.
Texte intégralTypescript. Includes vita and abstract. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 135-145). Also available for download via the World Wide Web; free to University of Oregon users.
Papoutsi, Marina. « Phonetic encoding, verbal working memory and the role of Broca's area ». Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/3078.
Texte intégralWilliamson, Victoria Jane. « Comparing short-term memory for sequences of verbal and tonal materials ». Thesis, University of York, 2008. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.550492.
Texte intégralDelcenserie, Audrey. « Language and verbal memory abilities of internationally adopted children from China ». Thesis, McGill University, 2014. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=123052.
Texte intégralL'objectif du présent programme de recherche était d'évaluer, à l'âge scolaire, les habiletés langagières d'enfants adoptés internationalement (AI) afin de déterminer si les difficultés rapportées chez des enfants AI d'âge préscolaire persistent (e.g., Gauthier & Genesee, 2011) ou si elles diminuent avec plus d'exposition à la langue d'adoption. Un autre objectif était de déterminer si les enfants AI ont des difficultés de mémoire verbale en plus de leurs retards de langage. Notez que ces études ont toutes comparées les enfants AI à des enfants non-adoptés, monolingues francophones et appariés sur l'âge, le sexe et le statut socioéconomique. L'étude 1 a évalué les habiletés langagières d'enfants AI de Chine par des familles francophones, ainsi que leurs habiletés cognitives non-verbales, leur développement socio-émotionnel et leur santé. Les enfants étaient âgés de 7-8 ans au moment de l'étude. Les enfants AI étaient âgés de 6-21 mois lors de leur adoption. Les résultats ont démontré que la performance des enfants AI aux tests de vocabulaire expressif, de définition de mots, de grammaire réceptive et de répétition de phrases sont significativement plus faibles que celle des contrôles. L'étude 2 a été réalisée dans le but d'évaluer la maitrise des clitiques objets par les enfants AI d'âge scolaire. Le but de cette étude était de déterminer si, avec une exposition suffisante à leur langue d'adoption, les enfants AI sont capables de maitriser cet aspect du français. Les enfants AI ont été évalués à l'aide d'un test d'élicitation de clitiques. Les participants de l'étude 2 étaient les mêmes que ceux de l'étude 1. Les résultats ont indiqués que les enfants AI omettent significativement plus de clitiques objets et produisent significativement plus de clitiques objets contenant des erreurs en genre et/ou nombre que les enfants non-adoptés. L'étude 3 a été entreprise dans le but d'examiner la mémoire chez les enfants AI. La mémoire verbale à court-terme, la mémoire verbale de travail, la mémoire verbale à long-terme, la mémoire non-verbale à court-terme, la mémoire de travail non-verbale,le développement cognitif non-verbal, le développement socio-émotionnel et les habiletés langagières des enfants ont été évalués. Les enfants étaient âges de 9-12 ans au moment de l'étude. Les enfants AI étaient âgés de 6-24 mois au moment de leur adoption. Les résultats de cette étude ont démontré que, malgré le fait que la performance des groupes aux tests de développement cognitif non-verbal et de développement socio-émotionnel soit similaire, les enfants AI performent significativement plus faiblement que les enfants contrôles aux test de vocabulaire expressif et réceptif, de grammaire réceptive, d'association de mots ainsi qu'aux tests de mémoire verbale à court-terme, de mémoire verbale de travail et de mémoire verbale à long-terme. Les groupes ont toutefois performé de façon similaire aux tests de mémoire non-verbale à court-terme et de mémoire de travail non-verbale, suggérant des difficultés spécifiques au langage. Des analyses de régressions ont démontré que les habiletés langagières des enfants AI sont mieux prédites par leur mémoire verbale, la mémoire verbale à court-terme en particulier, alors que, pour les contrôles, le meilleur prédicteur de leur performance aux tests de langage est la durée de leur exposition au français. De façon générale, les résultats suggèrent que, bien que le développement général des enfants AI est normal, leur mémoire verbale et certains aspects de leurs habiletés langagières sont en-deçà de celles des enfants contrôles appariés pour l'âge, le sexe et le statut-socioéconomique. Ces difficultés suggèrent des effets très précoces de « l'âge d'acquisition » sur le langage et sur la mémoire verbale, mais également que les délais de mémoire verbale des enfants AI pourraient expliquer leurs retards de langage, au moins de façon proximale.
Walker, Ian. « The role of language-processing mechanisms in verbal short-term memory ». Thesis, University of York, 1999. http://opus.bath.ac.uk/26789/.
Texte intégralNorman, Tricia Ann. « Factors influencing the development of visual/verbal memory codes in children ». Thesis, Queen's University Belfast, 1995. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.282608.
Texte intégralCarney, Rosemary Gai. « The contribution of phonological processes to implicit memory for verbal stimuli ». Thesis, University of Liverpool, 2002. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.272782.
Texte intégral