Articles de revues sur le sujet « Learning disabled children »

Pour voir les autres types de publications sur ce sujet consultez le lien suivant : Learning disabled children.

Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres

Choisissez une source :

Consultez les 50 meilleurs articles de revues pour votre recherche sur le sujet « Learning disabled children ».

À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.

Parcourez les articles de revues sur diverses disciplines et organisez correctement votre bibliographie.

1

Dr. Sharmista, Dr Sharmista. « Concept development in learning disabled children ». International Journal of Scientific Research 2, no 8 (1 juin 2012) : 117–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.15373/22778179/aug2013/39.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Bender, William N., et Lorri B. Golden. « Adaptive Behavior of Learning Disabled and Non-Learning Disabled Children ». Learning Disability Quarterly 11, no 1 (février 1988) : 55–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1511037.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Adaptive behavior, problem behavior, and self-perception of behavior of 54 learning disabled children and 54 non-learning disabled children were compared to revealed the differences mainstream teachers perceive between the two groups. Multivariate analysis revealed differences in the first two measures. That is, the groups were different in each subscale of adaptive behavior, including social coping, relationships, pragmatics and production. Analysis of the problem-behavior scale indicated differences between the groups on three of five subscales. In each case, the scores for the LD group were higher indicating less desirable adaptive behavior and more problem behavior in the classroom. Implications of these results are presented as well as recommended use of adaptive-behavior measures for identification and the need to prepare mainstream teachers to deal with maladaptive behaviors in the classroom.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Silver, L. « Therapies for Learning-Disabled Children ». Nurse Practitioner 12, no 10 (octobre 1987) : 48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006205-198710000-00014.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Jacobsen, Barbara, Barbara Lowery et Joseph DuCette. « Attributions of learning disabled children. » Journal of Educational Psychology 78, no 1 (1986) : 59–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.78.1.59.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Kelly, Greg. « Motivation in Learning Disabled Children ». British Journal of Occupational Therapy 48, no 11 (novembre 1985) : 340–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/030802268504801111.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Brainard, Richene Bevilaqua, Donald J. Viglione et Jared R. D. Wilke. « A Comparison of Learning-Disabled Children and Non-Learning-Disabled Children on the Rorschach ». Rorschachiana 35, no 1 (janvier 2014) : 66–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604/a000049.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
The purpose of this study was to examine the differences between learning-disabled (LD) and non-learning-disabled (non-LD) students on the Rorschach inkblot test to help determine how differently the two groups process information. Using the cognitive triad in Exner’s Comprehensive System (CS), the variables consistent with past research and most representative of each of the three stages of the cognitive triad were investigated. The sample consisted of 62 schoolchildren in the age range of 7–12 years in the US state of California. Thirty-one children were identified as LD and were matched on age, gender, and ethnicity with a student who was identified as non-LD. The groups’ responses were compared using a one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) to determine whether differences existed between the groups for each of the Rorschach variables F%, W+, XA%, and WSum6. This study concluded that LD children are unable to perceive, interpret, and synthesize information from their environment in a clear and realistic way when compared with their non-LD peers.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Pirani, Zainab, Vasiqullah Molvizadah, Mohammad Abbas Sayyed et Sasikumar M. « E-Learning Framework for Learning Disabled Children ». International Journal of Computer Applications 63, no 19 (15 février 2013) : 38–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.5120/10577-5703.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Bender, William N. « Teachability and Behavior of Learning Disabled Children ». Psychological Reports 59, no 2 (octobre 1986) : 471–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1986.59.2.471.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Teachers' ratings of teachability and behavior for 38 learning disabled children in Grades 3 to 6 were compared with ratings for 38 nondisabled children. Only two correlations between behavior and teachability for the disabled group and six for the nondisabled group were significant. Analysis of covariance indicated that the disabled children received less positive teachability ratings on school-appropriate behavior. Also the disabled group was rated less positively on three overt problem behaviors, acting out, distractibility, and immaturity. Research and educational implications were discussed.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Goh, David S., et Joy M. Hanson Wood. « Development of Conservation and Academic Achievement in Learning Disabled Children ». Psychological Reports 60, no 1 (février 1987) : 71–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1987.60.1.71.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
To investigate learning disabled children's development of conservation concepts and its relationship with academic achievement 24 normal and 23 learning disabled children were individually administered the Concept Assessment Kit-Conservation, the Peabody Individual Achievement Test, and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Analysis indicated that learning disabled children showed slower development in acquisition of conservation concepts than their normal peers. Age affected conservation development of learning disabled children. Correlations were moderate to high for conservation concepts with reading and mathematics achievement in learning disabled children. Educational implications of the findings were discussed.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Dudley-Marling, Curtis C., et Rebecca Edmiaston. « Social Status of Learning Disabled Children and Adolescents : A Review ». Learning Disability Quarterly 8, no 3 (août 1985) : 189–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1510893.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Low social status is frequently ascribed to learning disabled children and adolescents. This article reviews published investigations of social status among learning disabled children, adolescents, and adults. Results indicate that, contrary to prevailing assumptions, not all or even most learning disabled persons are held in low esteem by their teachers, parents, and peers. In fact, some learning disabled students are popular. Therefore, as a group, the learning disabled may merely be at greater risk for attaining low social status.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Millichap, J. Gordon. « Behavioral Problems in Learning Disabled Children ». Pediatric Neurology Briefs 13, no 4 (1 avril 1999) : 26. http://dx.doi.org/10.15844/pedneurbriefs-13-4-3.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Swanson, H. Lee, et Marcy Trahan. « Naturalistic Memory in Learning Disabled Children ». Learning Disability Quarterly 13, no 2 (mai 1990) : 82–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1510653.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Elliott, Colin D. « Cognitive profiles of learning disabled children ». British Journal of Developmental Psychology 7, no 2 (juin 1989) : 171–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.1989.tb00797.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Anderson, Vicki, et Gordon Stanley. « Ability Profiles of Learning Disabled Children ». Australian Psychologist 27, no 1 (mars 1992) : 48–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00050069208257574.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

DeFrancesco, John J. « Family therapy for learning disabled children. » American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 59, no 1 (1989) : 153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1989.tb01642.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

Kerr, Robert, et Kathy Hughes. « Movement Difficulty and Learning Disabled Children ». Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly 4, no 1 (janvier 1987) : 72–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/apaq.4.1.72.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Results of recent research have implicated information processing deficits in explaining the poor academic performance of learning disabled children. However, the motor difficulties of these children have not been extensively studied from a processing framework, yet cognitive skills are inherent to the successful performance of motor skills. Sixteen learning disabled and sixteen control subjects ranging in age from 6 to 8 years were tested on a Fitts’ reciprocal tapping task using 16 different target combinations with the ID ranging from 1.50 to 6.64 bits. Analysis of the slope and intercept coefficients showed a significant difference for intercept but not for slope. These data suggest that the problem may not be a major processing deficit, as the learning disabled children were able to handle the increased task difficulty in the same manner as the controls. Instead the problem may exist at the very early input stage of the processing mechanism: getting the information into the system.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

Harmony, Thalia, et Thalia Fernandez. « 30. QEEG in learning disabled children ». Clinical Neurophysiology 127, no 9 (septembre 2016) : e310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2016.05.305.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

Gfeller, Kate E. « Musicals : Mnemonics for Learning Disabled Children ». TEACHING Exceptional Children 19, no 1 (septembre 1986) : 28–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/004005998601900106.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Rourke, Byron P. « Socioemotional disturbances of learning disabled children. » Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 56, no 6 (1988) : 801–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.56.6.801.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Yewchuk, Carolyn R. « Identification of Gifted/Learning Disabled Children ». School Psychology International 7, no 1 (janvier 1986) : 61–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014303438600700108.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Until very recently, little attention has been focused on children who are both gifted and learning disabled. Because of their contradictory characteristics, these children are usually undetected by teachers or school psychologists. Close analysis of WISC-R responses and profiles in the context of a very broad clinical assessment for learning disability can assist the school psychologist in identifying these children so that they can be placed in educational programmes appropriate to their unique needs.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Yewchuk, Carolyn R. « Gifted/Learning Disabled Children : an Overview ». Gifted Education International 3, no 2 (septembre 1985) : 122–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/026142948500300211.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Obrzut, John E., et Carol A. Boliek. « Lateralization Characteristics in Learning Disabled Children ». Journal of Learning Disabilities 19, no 5 (mai 1986) : 308–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221948601900511.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

Harrington, Robert G., et Edward Gibson. « Preassessment Procedures for Learning Disabled Children ». Journal of Learning Disabilities 19, no 9 (novembre 1986) : 538–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221948601900904.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Nodine, Barbara F., Edna Barenbaum et Phyllis Newcomer. « Story Composition by Learning Disabled, Reading Disabled, and Normal Children ». Learning Disability Quarterly 8, no 3 (août 1985) : 167–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1510891.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Investigations into children's ability to comprehend and compose stories have been increasing steadily during the past 10 years. As a result, a body of information exists that has important implications for educators. In this article we will present background information about the most influential sources in children's understanding of stories. Also, we will report an investigation of the written composition skills of normal and handicapped learners.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Taylor, H. Gerry, Debra Lean et Sybil Schwartz. « Pseudoword repetition ability in learning-disabled children ». Applied Psycholinguistics 10, no 2 (juin 1989) : 203–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716400008523.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
ABSTRACTA test of pseudoword repetition was developed and administered to a group of 24 learning-disabled children. Twenty nondisabled children were also assessed. The major objective was to investigate associations between learning-disabled children's ability to repeat pseudowords and their performances on other measures of phonological processing, as well as on measures of academic achievement. Results revealed that the repetition procedure had good psychometric characteristics, including the ability to distinguish disabled and nondisabled groups independent of IQ. Within the learning-disabled sample, repetition ability was more closely related to reading and spelling skills than to math achievement. Measures of phonological skill were moderately associated with one another. Findings suggest that the repetition task may be useful in identifying one important correlate of childhood learning disabilities. Results justify further research on the mechanisms responsible for repetition difficulties and on the significance of these difficulties for reading and spelling acquisition.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

Bender, W. N. « Differences between Learning Disabled and Non-Learning Disabled Children in Temperament and Behavior ». Learning Disability Quarterly 8, no 1 (février 1985) : 11–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1510903.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Rodriguez, Christina M., et Donald K. Routh. « Depression, Anxiety, and Attributional Style in Learning-Disabled and Non-Learning-Disabled Children ». Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 18, no 4 (décembre 1989) : 299–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp1804_2.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

Zhang, Huafeng, et Stein T. Holden. « Numeracy skills learning of children in Africa:—Are disabled children lagging behind ? » PLOS ONE 18, no 4 (20 avril 2023) : e0284821. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0284821.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Significant progress has been achieved in universal basic education in African countries since the late 1990s. This study provides empirical evidence on the within- and across-country variation in numeracy skills performance among children based on nationally representative data from eight African countries (DR Congo, The Gambia, Ghana, Lesotho, Sierra Leone, Togo, Tunisia, and Zimbabwe). We assess whether and to what extent children with disabilities lag in numeracy skills and how much it depends on their type of disabilities. More specifically, we explore whether disabled children benefit equally from better school system quality. The assessment is analysed as a natural experiment using the performance of non-disabled children as a benchmark and considering the different types of disabilities as random treatments. We first evaluate the variation in average numeracy skills in the eight African countries. They can roughly be divided into low- and high-numeracy countries. We apply Instrumental Variable (IV) methods to control the endogeneity of completed school years when assessing subjects’ school performance and heterogeneous disability effects. Children with vision and hearing disabilities are not especially challenged in numeracy skills performance. The low numeracy skills among physically and intellectually disabled children are mainly attributable to their limited school attendance. Children with multiple disabilities are constrained both by low school attendance and by poor numeracy skills return to schooling. The average differences in school performance across the high- versus low-numeracy skill country groups are larger than the within-group average differences for disabled versus non-disabled kids. This indicates that school enrolment and quality are crucial for children’s learning of numeracy skills, and that disabled children benefit equally from better school quality across these African countries.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Watson, Marie May, et Bonita Renée Greenberg. « Referential Communication Abilities of Learning-Disabled, Language-Learning-Disabled, and Normal School-Age Children ». Perceptual and Motor Skills 66, no 1 (février 1988) : 11–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1988.66.1.11.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Past research in referential communication has indicated normally developing children show developmental progression in ability to communicate a specific referent to a listener. In one paradigm subjects were given lists of word-pairs in which one member of each pair was designated as the referent. It was shown that communicating about referents found in word-pairs associated in some way was more difficult than communicating about referents in dissimilar word-pairs. The present study extended this methodology to learning-disabled children. Learning-disabled, language-learning-disabled, and normally achieving children were asked to communicate about 30 pictured referents on three different tasks. On Tasks 1 and 2 each subject was asked to give a clue for the referent that would distinguish it from the other picture. Stimuli for Task 1 were 30 pairs of pictures that were related in some way and the stimuli for Task 2 were 30 pairs of unrelated pictures. Task 3 required the subjects to evaluate the adequacy of the examiner's clues for Task 1 stimuli. The disabled subjects were matched to the normally achieving subjects on the basis of receptive vocabulary age. Few differences were noted among the groups' performances on these referential communication tasks. Implications include the importance of vocabulary and concept development to referential communication.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Schachter, Debbie Carol, Barry Pless et Maggie Bruck. « Self-Report of Family Histories of Learning Difficulties ». Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 37, no 1 (février 1992) : 29–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/070674379203700107.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Self-reports of learning difficulties by parents of reading disabled children may be inflated because of recall bias. A retrospective study of the association between specific reading disabilities and familial reports of learning difficulties indicated the relative odds of being reading disabled for those whose relatives reported learning difficulties compared with children whose relatives did not report learning difficulties was statistically significant if fathers or siblings reported learning difficulties. No significant association was found between mothers' self-reports of learning difficulties and a diagnosis of specific learning disabilities in their children. The results support the association between specific reading disabilities and self-reported histories of academic problems in the father or siblings, but not in mothers of learning disabled children. The results suggest that reports of learning difficulties among mothers of reading disabled children may be inflated because of recall bias.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Elliott, Digby, et Bernie Hennessey. « Visual Feature Integration in Learning Disabled Children ». Perceptual and Motor Skills 65, no 3 (décembre 1987) : 927–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1987.65.3.927.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
This study examined the ability of 7 learning disabled children to detect and integrate visual features in a complex display. While the learning disabled children performed more poorly over-all than 6 control children, differences between the two groups were most pronounced when subjects were required to conjoin or integrate visual features to make a decision about the presence of a target item. This finding is discussed with reference to automatic and attention-demanding components of visual perception.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Shulman, Brian B., Steven C. Russell et Michael P. Rastatter. « Syntactic Decoding Times of Learning Disabled Children ». Perceptual and Motor Skills 62, no 1 (février 1986) : 259–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.62.1.259.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Jorgenson, Christabel B., David E. Jorgenson et Wilbon P. Davis. « A Cluster Analysis of Learning Disabled Children ». International Journal of Neuroscience 35, no 1-2 (janvier 1987) : 59–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207458708987110.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Marosi, Erzsébet, Thalia Harmony et Jacqueline Becker. « Brainstem Evoked Potentials in Learning Disabled Children ». International Journal of Neuroscience 50, no 3-4 (janvier 1990) : 233–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459008987177.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

McIntosh, David E., Mardis D. Dunham, Raymond S. Dean et Deborah K. Kundert. « Neuropsychological Characteristics of Learning Disabled/Gifted Children ». International Journal of Neuroscience 83, no 1-2 (janvier 1995) : 123–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459508986331.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Dharma Raja, B. William, et S. Praveen Kumar. « Do Multimedia Applications Benefit Learning-Disabled Children ? » i-manager's Journal of Educational Technology 6, no 4 (15 mars 2010) : 1–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.26634/jet.6.4.1106.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Smith, Sally L. « Typical Academic Problems of Learning-Disabled Children ». Pointer 32, no 3 (avril 1988) : 8–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/05544246.1988.9944777.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Lally, Martha J., Richard D. Lloyd et Janet M. Kulberg. « Is Intelligence Stable in Learning-Disabled Children ? » Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 5, no 4 (décembre 1987) : 411–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/073428298700500410.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Ziegler, Robert G. « "Family therapy for learning disabled children" : Reply. » American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 59, no 1 (1989) : 153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1989.tb01643.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

McIntosh, D. E., D. K. Kundert et R. S. Dean. « Neuropsychological characteristics of learning-disabled/gifted children ». Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 7, no 4 (1 janvier 1992) : 345–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/7.4.345a.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Parfitt, Tom. « Care of learning-disabled Russian children condemned ». Lancet 362, no 9392 (octobre 2003) : 1291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(03)14622-3.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Hanada, Masanori. « Learning of disabled children and their environment ». Brain and Development 8, no 5 (janvier 1986) : 547–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0387-7604(86)80102-4.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Carras, Porto. « Transient cognitive impairment in learning disabled children ». International Journal of Psychophysiology 18, no 2 (novembre 1994) : 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(94)90286-0.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Penge, R., C. Pesola, C. D’Agostini Costa, P. Rampoldi et I. Salvadore. « Neuropsychological profile in Italian learning-disabled children ». Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance et de l'Adolescence 60, no 5 (juillet 2012) : S181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurenf.2012.04.305.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Jäncke, Lutz, Mohamad Yassin Saka, Omer Badawood et Nsreen Alhamadi. « Resting-state electroencephalogram in learning-disabled children ». NeuroReport 30, no 2 (janvier 2019) : 95–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0000000000001166.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Raines, James C. « Social Work Practice with Learning Disabled Children ». Children & ; Schools 11, no 2 (1989) : 89–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cs/11.2.89.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Smith, Richard D. « Abnormal Head Circumference in Learning-disabled Children ». Developmental Medicine & ; Child Neurology 23, no 6 (12 novembre 2008) : 626–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1981.tb02044.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Dudley-Marling, Curtis. « The Pragmatic Skills of Learning Disabled Children ». Journal of Learning Disabilities 18, no 4 (avril 1985) : 193–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221948501800403.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Sutton, Jeffrey P., Joel L. Whitton, Michael Topa et Harvey Moldofsky. « Evoked potential maps in learning disabled children ». Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section 65, no 5 (septembre 1986) : 399–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(86)90019-5.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Amran, Nur Arifah, Puspa Liza Ghazali, Fauzilah Salleh et Nurul Zawani Baharazi. « Applied Integration Model in Education Plan for Learning Recovery ». Journal of Entrepreneurship and Business 4, no 2 (19 janvier 2021) : 70–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.17687/jeb.v4i2.86.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Islamic insurance, which is known as takaful, has been commonly used by people especially in most Muslim countries ever since it was introduced. The system has raised many new inventions to increase the effectiveness and to satisfy the people?s needs. However, the existing models of takaful do not provide plans for learning disabled children, even though their number is increasing year by year. The learning disabled children can achieve a better condition with consistent guidance hence something must be done to help them. This study discusses the takaful the education plan for learning disabled children, which will contribute a lot to their recovery from learning disability. By applying an integrated model of the takaful education plan, a new recovery model of the takaful education plan is proposed, which consists of a plan that may assist the education of the learning disabled children.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Nous offrons des réductions sur tous les plans premium pour les auteurs dont les œuvres sont incluses dans des sélections littéraires thématiques. Contactez-nous pour obtenir un code promo unique!

Vers la bibliographie