Littérature scientifique sur le sujet « Impliciti »
Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres
Consultez les listes thématiques d’articles de revues, de livres, de thèses, de rapports de conférences et d’autres sources académiques sur le sujet « Impliciti ».
À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.
Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.
Articles de revues sur le sujet "Impliciti"
Tosi, Maria Chiara. « Manuali impliciti ». TERRITORIO, no 84 (mai 2018) : 55–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.3280/tr2018-084007.
Texte intégralMittica, M. Paolo. « Attraversare il silenzio. I presupposti impliciti del diritto ». SOCIOLOGIA DEL DIRITTO, no 2 (juillet 2012) : 105–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.3280/sd2012-002006.
Texte intégralDavidde Elio. « Il riconoscimento dell'autorità accessoria della FCC da parte della Corte Suprema degli Stati Uniti : convergenza con l'applicazione della teoria dei poteri impliciti nel diritto brasiliano ». International Journal of Science and Society 4, no 4 (14 octobre 2022) : 40–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.54783/ijsoc.v4i4.550.
Texte intégralCarrozza, Marco. « Manuele File (cap. III, carme 26 Miller) : esegesi di un'aggressione scoptica colta ». Myrtia 37 (28 novembre 2022) : 155–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/myrtia.523131.
Texte intégralLuongo, Salvatore. « «Dio, uomo, mondo : la V parte del Conde Lucanor» ». Revista de Literatura Medieval 29 (21 décembre 2018) : 145–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.37536/rpm.2017.29.0.69398.
Texte intégralGattino, Silvia, Norma De Piccoli, Claudio Tortone et Sandra Dutto. « La valutazione come processo di apprendimento. Riflessioni su alcuni impliciti nella promozione della salute ». PSICOLOGIA DI COMUNITA', no 2 (septembre 2020) : 83–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.3280/psc2020-002006.
Texte intégralFarnaz Farahi. « Quale cultura dell’infanzia ? Riflettere sulla cura educativa nei servizi 0-6 attraverso la clinica della formazione ». IUL Research 2, no 4 (20 décembre 2021) : 133–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.57568/iulres.v2i4.138.
Texte intégralCorcuff, Philippe. « La scommessa democratica e l'individualismo contemporaneo ». SOCIETÀ DEGLI INDIVIDUI (LA), no 37 (avril 2010) : 121–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.3280/las2010-037011.
Texte intégralCalaresu, Emilia. « QUANTO MONDO C’È IN UN TESTO ? REFERENTI, SOTTINTESI E STRATEGIE DI COMPRENSIONE ». Italiano LinguaDue 14, no 1 (26 juillet 2022) : 542–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.54103/2037-3597/18304.
Texte intégralBonomo, Chiara, Guido Bortoluzzi et Stephen Mbieke Ndula. « I contest universitari in europa. Una comparazione e una proposta tipologica ». ECONOMIA E SOCIETÀ REGIONALE, no 3 (décembre 2018) : 28–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.3280/es2018-003003.
Texte intégralThèses sur le sujet "Impliciti"
Pantalone, P. « PRINCIPIO DI LEGALITÀ E MERCATO AL COSPETTO DEI POTERI IMPLICITI ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/2434/352702.
Texte intégralPALMA, MANUELA LAURA. « Il soggetto e la pratica. Tematizzazione di un implicito della clinica della formazione ». Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/14338.
Texte intégralGould, Wren. « Implicit essentialism : genetic concepts are implicitly associated with fate concepts ». Thesis, University of British Columbia, 2012. http://hdl.handle.net/2429/42870.
Texte intégralPopa-Roch, Maria-Antoneta. « Validation expérimentale d'une mesure implicite du préjugé : le test d'associations implicites ». Grenoble 2, 2008. http://www.theses.fr/2008GRE29001.
Texte intégralThis thesis aimed at contributing to the topic of construct validity of the Implicit Association Test (IAT, Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), considered as a prejudice measure. Although the IAT is currently employed for both research and applied purposes, the mechanism underlying the IAT effect is not yet fully understood. We adopted an experimental approach of the IAT construct validity in order to improve the comprehension of the IAT functioning. The IAT task was adapted to the French social context, where the North African minority is most frequently the target of prejudice. Considered together, Experiments 2, 3, 4 and 5 suggested that the IAT experimental situation has a threatening potential through the transparency of its purpose. The consequence would be an artificial inflation of the positive IAT effects (in the sense of in-group preferences). Experiments 6 and 7 confirmed that attention processes implied by binary categorisations could account, at least partially, for the IAT effect. Furthermore, we predicted that due to participant inclusion in one of the two target categories (i. E. , French versus North-African), categorisation of stimuli related to the "Self" may be an available heuristic with an important contribution to positive IAT effects reflecting in-group favouritism. Experiments 8 and 9 are consistent with this prediction. In addition, Experiment 1 demonstrated that personal characteristics such as age, education and ethnicity could exert their own influence on the IAT effect. The weights of the factors leading to the IAT effect is not yet established and, thus, undermine the construct validity of the IAT, considered as a diagnostic tool of prejudice. To conclude, we are arguing that IAT validation as a test (versus procedure) requests a clear specification of the contribution of all IAT components (i. E. , material, instructions, population and experimental conditions) to the IAT effect
Rubiano, Thomas. « Implicit Computational Complexity and Compilers ». Thesis, Sorbonne Paris Cité, 2017. http://www.theses.fr/2017USPCD076/document.
Texte intégralLa théorie de la complexité´e s’intéresse à la gestion des ressources, temps ou espace, consommés par un programmel ors de son exécution. L’analyse statique nous permet de rechercher certains critères syntaxiques afin de catégoriser des familles de programmes. L’une des approches les plus fructueuses dans le domaine consiste à observer le comportement potentiel des données manipulées. Par exemple, la détection de programmes “non size increasing” se base sur le principe très simple de compter le nombre d’allocations et de dé-allocations de mémoire, en particulier au cours de boucles et on arrive ainsi à détecter les programmes calculant en espace constant. Cette méthode s’exprime très bien comme propriété sur les graphes de flot de contrôle. Comme les méthodes de complexité implicite fonctionnent à l’aide de critères purement syntaxiques, ces analyses peuvent être faites au moment de la compilation. Parce qu’elles ne sont ici que statiques, ces analyses ne sont pas toujours calculables ou facilement calculables, des compromis doivent être faits en s’autorisant des approximations. Dans le sillon du “Size-Change Principle” de C. S. Lee, N. D. Jones et A. M. Ben-Amram, beaucoup de recherches reprennent cette méthode de prédiction de terminaison par observation de l’évolution des ressources. Pour le moment, ces méthodes venant des théories de la complexité implicite ont surtout été appliquées sur des langages plus ou moins jouets. Cette thèse tend à porter ces méthodes sur de “vrais” langages de programmation en s’appliquant au niveau des représentations intermédiaires dans des compilateurs largement utilises. Elle fournit à la communauté un outil permettant de traiter une grande quantité d’exemples et d’avoir une idée plus précise de l’expressivité réelle de ces analyses. De plus cette thèse crée un pont entre deux communautés, celle de la complexité implicite et celle de la compilation, montrant ainsi que chacune peut apporter à l’autre
Cleeremans, Axel. « Conscience et apprentissage : une perspective dynamique ». Doctoral thesis, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, 2001. http://hdl.handle.net/2013/ULB-DIPOT:oai:dipot.ulb.ac.be:2013/211510.
Texte intégralDestrebecqz, Arnaud. « Mesures directes et indirectes de l'apprentissage implicite : étude expérimentale et modélisation ». Doctoral thesis, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, 2000. http://hdl.handle.net/2013/ULB-DIPOT:oai:dipot.ulb.ac.be:2013/211744.
Texte intégralFessard, Brunelle. « Les obligations non matérialisées dans les contrats ». Thesis, Montpellier, 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015MONTD010/document.
Texte intégralThe analysis of practice and of case-law shows that, independent of the existence or not of a legal instrument, certain obligations the contents of which have not been made in writing and which do not fall under the category of imposed obligations are binding on the parties. The identification of a double condition of qualification shows the existence of a specific obligational category which, not having been envisaged as such by positive law, calls for analysis.The analysis of their exteriorisation proves to be necessary in order to understand not only the way they function but also the foundations of their enforceability. Some, qualified as non-materialized obligations by explicit reference, are identified by the stipulation, by the parties, of a clause by reference in the instrumentum which expressly refers to their enforceability. If their binding nature is, therefore, justified by the binding nature of the contract in which the contractual stipulation is provided, the contents binding vis-à-vis the contractual relationship is not, however, transcribed in the main instrument in writing. The others, qualified as non-materialized obligations by implicit reference, are those which are binding on the contracting parties without the latter justifying any explicit wish to comply with them. If this definition is similar to that of imposed obligations in that they are not necessarily expressly bound to the common intention of the parties, these non-materialized obligations can be distinguished by their foundation. Whereas imposed obligations are justified by the letter or the carrying out of a legal provision, non-materialized obligations by implicit reference can be explained by the notion of utility. The revelation of non-materialized obligations in contracts can be linked to a practical interest as, at the end of this process, a certain visibility concerning their effects enables both the shortcomings caused by their binding nature and hence the seemingly appropriate solutions to apply to be identified. The nature and the importance of the difficulties revealed justify, therefore, the necessity of setting up legal treatment, but also, of identifying the elements indispensable to find a proposition adapted to them. It can be seen that the insufficiencies related to the binding nature of non-materialized obligations are not specific to them in that they can, because of their general nature, be found in other contractual situations. The ways of treating this put forward in this study can therefore be applied to all contractual matters
Reuter, Robert. « Direct and indirect measures of learning in visual search ». Doctoral thesis, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/2013/ULB-DIPOT:oai:dipot.ulb.ac.be:2013/209542.
Texte intégraland (4) to investigate whether such knowledge about the association between visual contexts and “meaningful” locations can be (automatically) transferred to other tasks, namely a change detection task.
In a first series of four experiments, we tried to replicate the documented contextual cueing effect using a wide range of various direct measures of learning (tasks that are supposed to be related to explicit knowledge) and we systematically varied the distinctiveness of context configurations to study its effect on both direct and indirect measures of learning.
We also ran a series of neural network simulations (briefly described in the general discussion of this thesis), based on a very simple association-learning mechanism, that not only account for the observed contextual cueing effect, but also yield rather specific predictions about future experimental data: contextual cueing effects should also be observed when repetitions of context configurations are not perfect, i.e. the networks were able to react to slightly distorted versions of repeating contexts in a similar way than they did to completely identical contexts. Human participants, we conjectured, should therefore (if the simple connectionist model captures some relevant aspects of the contextual cueing effect) become faster at detecting targets surrounded by context configurations that are only partially identical from trial to trial compared to those trials where the context configurations were randomly generated. These predictions were tested in a second series of experiments using pseudo-repeated context configurations, where some distractor items were either displaced from trial to trial or their orientation changed, while conserving their global layout.
In a third series of experiments, we used more realistic images of natural landscapes as background contexts to establish the robustness of the contextual cueing effect as well as its ecological relevance claimed by Chun and colleagues. We furthermore added a second task to these experiments to study whether the acquired knowledge about the background-target location associations would (automatically) transfer to another visual search task, namely a change detection task. If participants have learned that certain locations of the repeated images are “important”, since they contain the target item to look for, then changes occurring at those specific locations should lead to less “change blindness” than changes occurring at other irrelevant locations. We used two different types of instructions to introduce this second task after the visual search task, where we either stressed the link between the two tasks, i.e. telling them that remembering the “important” locations for each image could be used to find the changes faster, or we simply told them to perform the second task without any reference to the first one.
We will close this thesis with a general discussion, combining findings based on our review of the existing research literature and findings based on our own experimental explorations of the contextual cueing effect. By this we will discuss the implications of our empirical studies for the scientific investigation of contextual cueing and implicit learning, in terms of theoretical, empirical and methodological issues.
Doctorat en sciences psychologiques
info:eu-repo/semantics/nonPublished
Arborelius, Jon. « Den implicite författaren : En diskussion kring begreppet "implicit författare", med en exemplifierande analys av Karl Ove Knausgårds roman Min kamp ». Thesis, Uppsala universitet, Litteraturvetenskapliga institutionen, 2014. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-242763.
Texte intégralLivres sur le sujet "Impliciti"
Pivanti, Augusto. I nomi impliciti del tempo. Faloppio (Co) [i.e. Como, Italy] : LietoColle, 2011.
Trouver le texte intégralBassi, Nicola. Principio di legalit'a e poteri amministrativi impliciti. Milano : Giuffr'e, 2001.
Trouver le texte intégralSacchetti, Francesco. Processi di categorizzazione in etnografia : Il ruolo degli impliciti e delle categorie ex ante. Acireale : Bonanno editore, 2014.
Trouver le texte intégralAxel, Cleeremans, dir. Implicit learning and consciousness : An empirical, philosophical, and computational consensus in the making. New York : Psychology Press, 2002.
Trouver le texte intégralWestmoreland, Peter. Silence, Implicite et Non-Dit chez Rousseau : Silence, the implicit and the unspoken in Rousseau. Leiden : Brill Rodopi, 2020.
Trouver le texte intégralP, Dienes Zoltan, dir. Implicit learning : Theoretical and empirical issues. Hove, UK : L. Erlbaum Associates, 1993.
Trouver le texte intégralPhD, Graf Peter, et Masson Michael E. J, dir. Implicit memory : New directions in cognition, development, and neuropsychology. Hillsdale, N.J : L. Erlbaum Associates, 1993.
Trouver le texte intégralL' implicite. Paris : A. Colin, 1986.
Trouver le texte intégralSchultheiss, Oliver C. Implicit motives. Oxford : Oxford University Press, 2010.
Trouver le texte intégralImplicit motives. Oxford : Oxford University Press, 2010.
Trouver le texte intégralChapitres de livres sur le sujet "Impliciti"
Fancelli, Maria. « Luoghi impliciti ». Dans Biblioteca di Studi di Filologia Moderna, 21–26. Florence : Firenze University Press, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.36253/978-88-5518-338-3.06.
Texte intégralMarescaux, Pierre-Jean, Marie Izaute et Patrick Chambres. « How Implicit is Implicitly Acquired Knowledge ». Dans Metacognition, 191–202. Boston, MA : Springer US, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1099-4_13.
Texte intégralGallicchio, James, Yong Kiam Tan, Stefan Mitsch et André Platzer. « Implicit Definitions with Differential Equations for KeYmaera X ». Dans Automated Reasoning, 723–33. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-10769-6_42.
Texte intégralOhtaka, Mizuka. « EVALUATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF EXPOSURE TO COUNTERSTEREOTYPIC FATHERS ON REDUCING IMPLICIT FATHER AND MOTHER STEREOTYPES IN JAPAN ». Dans Advances in Psychology and Psychological Trends, 67–72. inScience Press, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.36315/2021pad07.
Texte intégralBerry, Dianne C. « Introduction ». Dans How Implicit Is Implicit Learning ?, 1–12. Oxford University Press, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0001.
Texte intégralMathews, Robert C., et Lewis G. Roussel. « Abstractness of implicit knowledge : A cognitive evolutionary perspective ». Dans How Implicit Is Implicit Learning ?, 13–47. Oxford University Press, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0002.
Texte intégralLewicki, Pawel, et Maria Czyzewska. « Nonconscious information processing and personality ». Dans How Implicit Is Implicit Learning ?, 48–72. Oxford University Press, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0003.
Texte intégralManza, Louis, et Arthur S. Reber. « Representing artificial grammars : Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities ». Dans How Implicit Is Implicit Learning ?, 73–106. Oxford University Press, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0004.
Texte intégralDienes, Zoltán, et Gerry Altmann. « Transfer of implicit knowledge across domains : How implicit and how abstract ? » Dans How Implicit Is Implicit Learning ?, 107–23. Oxford University Press, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0005.
Texte intégralPerruchet, Pierre, et Jorge Gallego. « A subjective unit formation account of implicit learning ». Dans How Implicit Is Implicit Learning ?, 124–61. Oxford University Press, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0006.
Texte intégralActes de conférences sur le sujet "Impliciti"
Bolander, Thomas, Lasse Dissing et Nicolai Herrmann. « DEL-based Epistemic Planning for Human-Robot Collaboration : Theory and Implementation ». Dans 18th International Conference on Principles of Knowledge Representation and Reasoning {KR-2021}. California : International Joint Conferences on Artificial Intelligence Organization, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.24963/kr.2021/12.
Texte intégralHong, Jon-Chao, Ming-Yueh Hwang, Shueh-Chen Huang et Kai-Hsin Tai. « Smartphones being implicitly used : How implicit knowledge affects the usage of a smartphone ». Dans 2013 IEEE 63rd Annual Conference International Council for Educational Media (ICEM). IEEE, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cicem.2013.6820145.
Texte intégralYi, Sung-Jae, et Moon-Sun Chung. « An Advanced Semi-Implicit Method for Two-Phase Flow Calculation ». Dans 2010 14th International Heat Transfer Conference. ASMEDC, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/ihtc14-22009.
Texte intégralBexte, Marie, Andrea Horbach et Torsten Zesch. « Implicit Phenomena in Short-answer Scoring Data ». Dans Proceedings of the 1st Workshop on Understanding Implicit and Underspecified Language. Stroudsburg, PA, USA : Association for Computational Linguistics, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/2021.unimplicit-1.2.
Texte intégralStengel-Eskin, Elias, Jimena Guallar-Blasco et Benjamin Van Durme. « Human-Model Divergence in the Handling of Vagueness ». Dans Proceedings of the 1st Workshop on Understanding Implicit and Underspecified Language. Stroudsburg, PA, USA : Association for Computational Linguistics, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/2021.unimplicit-1.6.
Texte intégralMa, Weicheng, Ruibo Liu, Lili Wang et Soroush Vosoughi. « Improvements and Extensions on Metaphor Detection ». Dans Proceedings of the 1st Workshop on Understanding Implicit and Underspecified Language. Stroudsburg, PA, USA : Association for Computational Linguistics, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/2021.unimplicit-1.5.
Texte intégralEisenstadt, Roy, et Michael Elhadad. « Evaluation Guidelines to Deal with Implicit Phenomena to Assess Factuality in Data-to-Text Generation ». Dans Proceedings of the 1st Workshop on Understanding Implicit and Underspecified Language. Stroudsburg, PA, USA : Association for Computational Linguistics, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/2021.unimplicit-1.3.
Texte intégralRoth, Michael, et Talita Anthonio. « UnImplicit Shared Task Report : Detecting Clarification Requirements in Instructional Text ». Dans Proceedings of the 1st Workshop on Understanding Implicit and Underspecified Language. Stroudsburg, PA, USA : Association for Computational Linguistics, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/2021.unimplicit-1.4.
Texte intégralWiriyathammabhum, Peratham. « TTCB System Description to a Shared Task on Implicit and Underspecified Language 2021 ». Dans Proceedings of the 1st Workshop on Understanding Implicit and Underspecified Language. Stroudsburg, PA, USA : Association for Computational Linguistics, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/2021.unimplicit-1.8.
Texte intégralKurfalı, Murathan, et Robert Östling. « Let’s be explicit about that : Distant supervision for implicit discourse relation classification via connective prediction ». Dans Proceedings of the 1st Workshop on Understanding Implicit and Underspecified Language. Stroudsburg, PA, USA : Association for Computational Linguistics, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/2021.unimplicit-1.1.
Texte intégralRapports d'organisations sur le sujet "Impliciti"
Southworth, Benjamin, Tomasso Buvoli, Oliver Krzysik, Will Pazner et Hans De Sterck. Fully Implicit Time Integration : Fast Solvers and Implicit-Explicit Schemes. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), septembre 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/1820062.
Texte intégralRosen, Sherwin. Implicit Contracts : A Survey. Cambridge, MA : National Bureau of Economic Research, juin 1985. http://dx.doi.org/10.3386/w1635.
Texte intégralBurby, Joshua. Principles for implicit integration. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), juin 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/1788386.
Texte intégralClotfelter, Charles, et Philip Cook. Implicit Taxation in Lottery Finance. Cambridge, MA : National Bureau of Economic Research, mai 1987. http://dx.doi.org/10.3386/w2246.
Texte intégralMahoney, Neale. Bankruptcy as Implicit Health Insurance. Cambridge, MA : National Bureau of Economic Research, mai 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.3386/w18105.
Texte intégralWilson, Prescott. Training to Foster Implicit Communications. Fort Belvoir, VA : Defense Technical Information Center, janvier 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada505964.
Texte intégralGustman, Alan, et Thomas Steinmeier. Pensions, Unions and Implicit Contracts. Cambridge, MA : National Bureau of Economic Research, octobre 1986. http://dx.doi.org/10.3386/w2036.
Texte intégralMcKinley, M. S., E. D. Brooks, III et A. Szoke. Comparison of Implicit and Symbolic Implicit Monte Carlo Line Transport with Frequency Weight Vector Extension. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), décembre 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/15003240.
Texte intégralMcKinley, M. S., E. D. Brooks, III et A. Szoke. Comparison of Implicit and Symbolic Implicit Monte Carlo Line Transport With Frequency Weight Vector Extension. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), mars 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/15004652.
Texte intégralVan Wieren, Jack. Using Diagonally Implicit Multistage Integration Methods for Solving Ordinary Differential Equations. Part 2 : Implicit Methods. Fort Belvoir, VA : Defense Technical Information Center, août 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada328947.
Texte intégral