Pour voir les autres types de publications sur ce sujet consultez le lien suivant : Exogenous and endogenous attention.

Articles de revues sur le sujet « Exogenous and endogenous attention »

Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres

Choisissez une source :

Consultez les 50 meilleurs articles de revues pour votre recherche sur le sujet « Exogenous and endogenous attention ».

À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.

Parcourez les articles de revues sur diverses disciplines et organisez correctement votre bibliographie.

1

Grubb, M. A., A. White, D. J. Heeger, and M. Carrasco. "Does exogenous attention modulate endogenous attention?" Journal of Vision 13, no. 9 (2013): 473. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/13.9.473.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Zhu, Ping, Qingqing Yang, Luo Chen, et al. "Working-Memory-Guided Attention Competes with Exogenous Attention but Not with Endogenous Attention." Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 5 (2023): 426. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/bs13050426.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Recent research has extensively investigated working memory (WM)-guided attention, which is the phenomenon of attention being directed towards information in the external environment that matches the content stored in WM. While prior studies have focused on the potential influencing factors of WM-guided attention, little is known about the nature of it. This attention system exhibits characteristics of two classical distinct attention systems: exogenous attention and endogenous attention, as it can operate automatically like exogenous attention yet persist for a long time and be modulated by c
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Wang, Teng, Haifeng Hu, and Chen He. "Image Caption with Endogenous–Exogenous Attention." Neural Processing Letters 50, no. 1 (2019): 431–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11063-019-09979-7.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Santangelo, Valerio, Marta Olivetti Belardinelli, Charles Spence, and Emiliano Macaluso. "Interactions between Voluntary and Stimulus-driven Spatial Attention Mechanisms across Sensory Modalities." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21, no. 12 (2009): 2384–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21178.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
In everyday life, the allocation of spatial attention typically entails the interplay between voluntary (endogenous) and stimulus-driven (exogenous) attention. Furthermore, stimuli in different sensory modalities can jointly influence the direction of spatial attention, due to the existence of cross-sensory links in attentional control. Using fMRI, we examined the physiological basis of these interactions. We induced exogenous shifts of auditory spatial attention while participants engaged in an endogenous visuospatial cueing task. Participants discriminated visual targets in the left or right
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Gong, Mingliang, Tingyu Liu, Yingbing Chen, and Yingying Sun. "Dissociable Effects of Endogenous and Exogenous Attention on Crowding: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials." Brain Sciences 14, no. 10 (2024): 956. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14100956.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Background/Objectives: Crowding is a common visual phenomenon that can significantly impair the recognition of objects in peripheral vision. Two recent behavioral studies have revealed that both exogenous and endogenous attention can alleviate crowding, but exogenous attention seems to be more effective. Methods: The present study employed the event-related potential (ERP) technique to explore the electrophysiological characteristics of the influence of these two types of attention on crowding. In the experiment, participants were required to judge whether the letter “T” was upright or inverte
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Jigo, Michael, David J. Heeger, and Marisa Carrasco. "An image-computable model of how endogenous and exogenous attention differentially alter visual perception." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118, no. 33 (2021): e2106436118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2106436118.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Attention alters perception across the visual field. Typically, endogenous (voluntary) and exogenous (involuntary) attention similarly improve performance in many visual tasks, but they have differential effects in some tasks. Extant models of visual attention assume that the effects of these two types of attention are identical and consequently do not explain differences between them. Here, we develop a model of spatial resolution and attention that distinguishes between endogenous and exogenous attention. We focus on texture-based segmentation as a model system because it has revealed a clea
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Berger, Andrea, and Avishai Henik. "The Endogenous Modulation of IOR is Nasal-Temporal Asymmetric." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, no. 3 (2000): 421–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892900562246.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Inhibition of return (IOR) refers to a reflexive mechanism mediated by phylogenetically primitive extrageniculate visuomotor pathways, which apparently serves to favor novel spatial locations by inhibiting those recently sampled. We demonstrate an asymmetry between temporal and nasal hemifields in the strategic modulation of IOR by endogenously controlled attention. Exogenous and endogenous precues were manipulated independently on each trial such that precues to initiate endogenous spatial orienting were presented after IOR had been activated by exogenous visual signals. Both types of precues
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Reeck, Crystal, Kevin S. LaBar, and Tobias Egner. "Neural Mechanisms Mediating Contingent Capture of Attention by Affective Stimuli." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24, no. 5 (2012): 1113–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00211.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Attention is attracted exogenously by physically salient stimuli, but this effect can be dampened by endogenous attention settings, a phenomenon called “contingent capture.” Emotionally salient stimuli are also thought to exert a strong exogenous influence on attention, especially in anxious individuals, but whether and how top–down attention can ameliorate bottom–up capture by affective stimuli is currently unknown. Here, we paired a novel spatial cueing task with fMRI to investigate contingent capture as a function of the affective salience of bottom–up cues (face stimuli) and individual dif
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

MacLean, Katherine A., Stephen R. Aichele, David A. Bridwell, George R. Mangun, Ewa Wojciulik, and Clifford D. Saron. "Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention during vigilance." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 71, no. 5 (2009): 1042–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/app.71.5.1042.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

McCormick, C. R., R. S. Redden, M. A. Lawrence, and R. M. Klein. "The independence of endogenous and exogenous temporal attention." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 80, no. 8 (2018): 1885–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1575-y.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Zhao, Chen, Kan Zhang, and Huahai Yang. "Studies on Endogenous and Exogenous Visual Selective Attention." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 44, no. 1 (2000): 144–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193120004400138.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Lawrence, Michael A., and Raymond M. Klein. "Isolating exogenous and endogenous modes of temporal attention." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142, no. 2 (2013): 560–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029023.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Koenig-Robert, R., and R. VanRullen. "Spatio-temporal mapping of exogenous and endogenous attention." Journal of Vision 10, no. 7 (2010): 1280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/10.7.1280.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Theeuwes, Jan. "Endogenous and Exogenous Control of Visual Selection." Perception 23, no. 4 (1994): 429–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p230429.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Among the most fundamental issues of visual attention research is the extent to which visual selection is controlled by properties of the stimulus or by the intentions, goals, and beliefs of the observer. Before selective attention operates, preattentive processes perform some basic analyses segmenting the visual field into functional perceptual units. The crucial question is whether the allocation of attention to these perceptual units is under the endogenous control of the observer (intentions, goals, beliefs) or under the exogenous control of stimulation. In this article evidence is discuss
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

Chong, Sang Chul, and Randolph Blake. "Exogenous attention and endogenous attention influence initial dominance in binocular rivalry." Vision Research 46, no. 11 (2006): 1794–803. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2005.10.031.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

Agaoglu, Sevda, Bruno Breitmeyer, and Haluk Ogmen. "Effects of Exogenous and Endogenous Attention on Metacontrast Masking." Vision 2, no. 4 (2018): 39. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/vision2040039.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
To efficiently use its finite resources, the visual system selects for further processing only a subset of the rich sensory information. Visual masking and spatial attention control the information transfer from visual sensory-memory to visual short-term memory. There is still a debate whether these two processes operate independently or interact, with empirical evidence supporting both arguments. However, recent studies pointed out that earlier studies showing significant interactions between common-onset masking and attention suffered from ceiling and/or floor effects. Our review of previous
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

FILOTEO, J. VINCENT, DEAN C. DELIS, DAVID P. SALMON, THERESA DEMADURA, MARY J. ROMAN, and CLIFFORD W. SHULTS. "An examination of the nature of attentional deficits in patients with Parkinson's disease: Evidence from a spatial orienting task." Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 3, no. 4 (1997): 337–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1355617797003378.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Endogenous and exogenous shifts of attention were examined in nondemented patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). In the endogenous condition, an arrow was used to cue participants' attention to the possible location of an impending target, whereas in the exogenous condition, a brightened box was used to cue attention. Cues were either valid (i.e., the target appeared in the cued location) or invalid (i.e., the target appeared in a noncued location). The time between cue onset and target onset (stimulus onset asynchrony or SOA) was varied in each condition. The results indicated that PD patien
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

Şentürk, Gözde, Adam S. Greenberg, and Taosheng Liu. "Saccade latency indexes exogenous and endogenous object-based attention." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 78, no. 7 (2016): 1998–2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1136-1.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Jones, Alexander, and Bettina Forster. "Independent effects of endogenous and exogenous attention in touch." Somatosensory & Motor Research 30, no. 4 (2013): 161–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/08990220.2013.779243.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Sætrevik, Bjørn, and Kenneth Hugdahl. "Endogenous and exogenous control of attention in dichotic listening." Neuropsychology 21, no. 3 (2007): 285–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.21.3.285.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Roelofs, Karin, Gerard P. van Galen, Paul Eling, Ger P. J. Keijsers, and Cees A. L. Hoogduin. "Endogenous and Exogenous Attention in Patients with Conversion Paresis." Cognitive Neuropsychology 20, no. 8 (2003): 733–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643290342000069.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Berger, Andrea, Avishai Henik, and Robert Rafal. "Competition Between Endogenous and Exogenous Orienting of Visual Attention." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 134, no. 2 (2005): 207–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.207.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

CHEN, XIAOXI, QI CHEN, DINGGUO GAO, and ZHENZHU YUE. "Interaction between endogenous and exogenous orienting in crossmodal attention." Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 53, no. 4 (2012): 303–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00957.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Chennu, Srivas, Paola Finoia, Evelyn Kamau, et al. "Dissociable endogenous and exogenous attention in disorders of consciousness." NeuroImage: Clinical 3 (2013): 450–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.10.008.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Le Bigot, Nathalie, and Marc Grosjean. "Exogenous and endogenous shifts of attention in perihand space." Psychological Research 80, no. 4 (2015): 677–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0680-y.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

Gowen, E., R. V. Abadi, E. Poliakoff, P. C. Hansen, and R. C. Miall. "Modulation of saccadic intrusions by exogenous and endogenous attention." Brain Research 1141 (April 2007): 154–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.047.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Collins, Lindsay, and James Schirillo. "Attention to endogenous and exogenous cues affects auditory localization." Experimental Brain Research 231, no. 1 (2013): 13–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3663-5.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

Renner, Peggy, Laura Grofer Klinger, and Mark R. Klinger. "Exogenous and Endogenous Attention Orienting in Autism Spectrum Disorders." Child Neuropsychology 12, no. 4-5 (2006): 361–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297040600770753.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Rosen, Allyson C., Stephen M. Rao, Paolo Caffarra, et al. "Neural Basis of Endogenous and Exogenous Spatial Orienting: A Functional MRI Study." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11, no. 2 (1999): 135–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892999563283.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Whole-brain functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was used to examine the neural substrates of internally (endogenous) and externally (exogenous) induced covert shifts of attention. Thirteen normal subjects performed three orienting conditions: endogenous (location of peripheral target predicted by a central arrow 80% of the time), exogenous (peripheral target preceded by a noninformative peripheral cue), and control (peripheral target preceded by noninformative central cue). Behavioral results indicated faster reaction times (RTs) for valid than for invalid trials for the endogenous con
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

TANG, Xiaoyu, Jiageng TONG, Hong YU, and Aijun WANG. "Effects of endogenous spatial attention and exogenous spatial attention on multisensory integration." Acta Psychologica Sinica 53, no. 11 (2021): 1173. http://dx.doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.01173.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Cansino, Selene. "The effects of endogenous and exogenus orienting of attention on source memory." Acta de Investigación Psicológica 8, no. 2 (2018): 80–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.22201/fpsi.20074719e.2018.2.07.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
The aim of this study was to determine the effects of endogenous and exogenous orienting of attention on episodic memory. Thirty healthy participants performed a cueing attention paradigm during encoding, in which images of common objects were presented either to the left or to the right of the center of the screen. Before the presentation of each image, three types of symbolic cues were displayed to indicate the location in which the stimuli would appear: valid cues to elicit endogenous orientation, invalid cues to prompt exogenous orientation and neutral or uncued trials. The participants’ t
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Magosso, Elisa, Andrea Serino, Giuseppe di Pellegrino, and Mauro Ursino. "Crossmodal Links between Vision and Touch in Spatial Attention: A Computational Modelling Study." Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2010 (2010): 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2010/304941.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Many studies have revealed that attention operates across different sensory modalities, to facilitate the selection of relevant information in the multimodal situations of every-day life. Cross-modal links have been observed either when attention is directed voluntarily (endogenous) or involuntarily (exogenous). The neural basis of cross-modal attention presents a significant challenge to cognitive neuroscience. Here, we used a neural network model to elucidate the neural correlates of visual-tactile interactions in exogenous and endogenous attention. The model includes two unimodal (visual an
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Hurst, Austin J., Michael A. Lawrence, and Raymond M. Klein. "How Does Spatial Attention Influence the Probability and Fidelity of Colour Perception?" Vision 3, no. 2 (2019): 31. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/vision3020031.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Existing research has found that spatial attention alters how various stimulus properties are perceived (e.g., luminance, saturation), but few have explored whether it improves the accuracy of perception. To address this question, we performed two experiments using modified Posner cueing tasks, wherein participants made speeded detection responses to peripheral colour targets and then indicated their perceived colours on a colour wheel. In E1, cues were central and endogenous (i.e., prompted voluntary attention) and the interval between cues and targets (stimulus onset asynchrony, or SOA) was
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Casteau, Soazig, and Daniel T. Smith. "Associations and Dissociations between Oculomotor Readiness and Covert Attention." Vision 3, no. 2 (2019): 17. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/vision3020017.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
The idea that covert mental processes such as spatial attention are fundamentally dependent on systems that control overt movements of the eyes has had a profound influence on theoretical models of spatial attention. However, theories such as Klein’s Oculomotor Readiness Hypothesis (OMRH) and Rizzolatti’s Premotor Theory have not gone unchallenged. We previously argued that although OMRH/Premotor theory is inadequate to explain pre-saccadic attention and endogenous covert orienting, it may still be tenable as a theory of exogenous covert orienting. In this article we briefly reiterate the key
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

Jones, Alexander, and Bettina Forster. "Neural correlates of endogenous attention, exogenous attention and inhibition of return in touch." European Journal of Neuroscience 40, no. 2 (2014): 2389–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12583.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Chakravarthi, R., and R. VanRullen. "Bullet trains and steam engines: Exogenous attention zips but endogenous attention chugs along." Journal of Vision 11, no. 4 (2011): 12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/11.4.12.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Smith, Daniel T., Thomas Schenk, and Chris Rorden. "Saccade preparation is required for exogenous attention but not endogenous attention or IOR." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 38, no. 6 (2012): 1438–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027794.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Chakravarthi, R., and R. VanRullen. "Beam me up, Scotty! Exogenous attention teleports but endogenous attention takes the shuttle." Journal of Vision 10, no. 7 (2010): 244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/10.7.244.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Fernández, Antonio, Sara Okun, and Marisa Carrasco. "Differential Effects of Endogenous and Exogenous Attention on Sensory Tuning." Journal of Neuroscience 42, no. 7 (2021): 1316–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0892-21.2021.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Fernandez, Antonio, Sara Okun, and Marisa Carrasco. "Differential effects of endogenous and exogenous attention on sensory tuning." Journal of Vision 21, no. 9 (2021): 2431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.9.2431.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Jigo, Michael, and Marisa Carrasco. "Endogenous and exogenous covert attention differentially modulate second-order textures." Journal of Vision 18, no. 10 (2018): 1259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/18.10.1259.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Rohenkohl, Gustavo, Jennifer T. Coull, and Anna C. Nobre. "Behavioural Dissociation between Exogenous and Endogenous Temporal Orienting of Attention." PLoS ONE 6, no. 1 (2011): e14620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014620.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Chong, S. C., and R. Blake. "Exogenous and endogenous attention influence initial dominance in binocular rivalry." Journal of Vision 5, no. 8 (2005): 1045. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/5.8.1045.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Ziegler, N. E., and D. Kerzel. "Exogenous and endogenous attention shifts during smooth pursuit eye movements." Journal of Vision 5, no. 8 (2010): 694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/5.8.694.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Hopfinger, Joseph B., and Vicki M. West. "Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing." NeuroImage 31, no. 2 (2006): 774–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.049.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Durand, Robert B., Manapon Limkriangkrai, and Lucia Fung. "Exogenous and Endogenous Attention and the Convergence of Analysts’ Forecasts." Journal of Behavioral Finance 20, no. 2 (2019): 154–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15427560.2018.1504783.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Botta, Fabiano, Valerio Santangelo, Antonino Raffone, Juan Lupiáñez, and Marta Olivetti Belardinelli. "Exogenous and endogenous spatial attention effects on visuospatial working memory." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63, no. 8 (2010): 1590–602. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210903443836.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Schmidt, T., and A. Seydell. "Modulation of cortical feedfoward dynamics by endogenous and exogenous attention." Journal of Vision 6, no. 6 (2010): 745. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/6.6.745.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Tang, Xiaoyu, Jinglong Wu, and Yong Shen. "The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention." Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 61 (February 2016): 208–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.002.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Cohen, Michael A., Patrick Cavanagh, Marvin M. Chun, and Ken Nakayama. "Response to Tsuchiya et al.: considering endogenous and exogenous attention." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, no. 11 (2012): 528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.002.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Nous offrons des réductions sur tous les plans premium pour les auteurs dont les œuvres sont incluses dans des sélections littéraires thématiques. Contactez-nous pour obtenir un code promo unique!