Articles de revues sur le sujet « Cotton-top tamarins »

Pour voir les autres types de publications sur ce sujet consultez le lien suivant : Cotton-top tamarins.

Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres

Choisissez une source :

Consultez les 50 meilleurs articles de revues pour votre recherche sur le sujet « Cotton-top tamarins ».

À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.

Parcourez les articles de revues sur diverses disciplines et organisez correctement votre bibliographie.

1

Evans, David T., Leslie A. Knapp, Peicheng Jing, Marian S. Piekarczyk, Virginia S. Hinshaw et David I. Watkins. « Three Different MHC Class I Molecules Bind the Same CTL Epitope of the Influenza Virus in a Primate Species with Limited MHC Class I Diversity ». Journal of Immunology 162, no 7 (1 avril 1999) : 3970–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.4049/jimmunol.162.7.3970.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Abstract One of the most remarkable features of the MHC class I loci of most outbred mammalian populations is their exceptional diversity, yet the functional importance of this diversity remains to be fully understood. The cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) is unusual in having MHC class I loci that exhibit both limited polymorphism and sequence variation. To investigate the functional implications of limited MHC class I diversity in this outbred primate species, we infected five tamarins with influenza virus and defined the CTL epitopes recognized by each individual. In addition to an immunodominant epitope of the viral nucleoprotein (NP) that was recognized by all individuals, two tamarins also made a response to the same epitope of the matrix (M1) protein. Surprisingly, these two tamarins used different MHC class I molecules, Saoe-G*02 and -G*04, to present the M1 epitope. In addition, CTLs from one of the tamarins recognized target cells that expressed neither Saoe-G*02 nor -G*04, but, rather, a third MHC class I molecule, Saoe-G*12. Sequence analysis revealed that Saoe-G*12 differs from both Saoe-G*02 and -G*04 by only two nucleotides and was probably generated by recombination between these two alleles. These results demonstrate that at least three of the tamarin’s MHC class I molecules can present the same epitope to virus-specific CTLs. Thus, four of the tamarin’s 12 MHC class I molecules bound only two influenza virus CTL epitopes. Therefore, the functional diversity of cotton-top tamarin’s MHC class I loci may be even more limited than their genetic diversity suggests.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Evans, D. T., M. S. Piekarczyk, T. M. Allen, J. E. Boyson, M. Yeager, A. L. Hughes, F. M. Gotch, V. S. Hinshaw et D. I. Watkins. « Immunodominance of a single CTL epitope in a primate species with limited MHC class I polymorphism. » Journal of Immunology 159, no 3 (1 août 1997) : 1374–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.4049/jimmunol.159.3.1374.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Abstract MHC class I molecules play a crucial role in immunity to viral infections by presenting viral peptides to cytotoxic T lymphocytes. One of the hallmarks of MHC class I genes in outbred populations is their extraordinary polymorphism, yet the significance of this diversity is poorly understood. Certain species with reduced MHC class I diversity, such as the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus), are more susceptible to fatal viral infections. To explore the relationship between this primate's limited MHC class I diversity and its susceptibility to viruses, we infected five cotton-top tamarins with influenza virus. Every tamarin recognized the same immunodominant CTL epitope of the influenza nucleoprotein. Surprisingly, this nucleoprotein peptide was bound by Saoe-G*08, an MHC class I molecule expressed by every cotton-top tamarin. Two tamarins also made a subdominant response to an epitope of the matrix (M1) protein. This peptide appeared to be bound by another common MHC class I molecule. With the exception of an additional subdominant response to the polymerase (PB2) protein in one individual, no other influenza-specific CTL responses were detected. In populations or species with limited MHC class I polymorphism like the cotton-top tamarin, a dependence on shared MHC class I molecules may enhance susceptibility to viral infection, since viruses that evade MHC class I-restricted recognition in one individual will likely evade recognition in the majority of individuals.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Hauser, Marc D., Daniel Weiss et Gary Marcus. « RETRACTED : Rule learning by cotton-top tamarins ». Cognition 86, no 1 (novembre 2002) : B15—B22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00139-7.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Buchanan-Smith, H. M., D. A. Anderson et C. W. Ryan. « Responses of Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus Oedipus) to Faecal Scents of Predators and Non-Predators ». Animal Welfare 2, no 1 (février 1993) : 17–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0962728600015438.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
AbstractThe responses of 56 cotton-top tamarin monkeys (Saguinus oedipus) to the faecal scent of predators and non-predators were recorded to determine if there was a differential response. Methylene chloride extracts were prepared from the faeces of suspected predators (margay and tayra) and non-predators (capybara and paca) known to co-exist with the tamarins in the wild The faecal extracts were presented to the tamarins on wooden dowels in their enclosures. Untreated dowel and dowel treated with methylene chloride served as controls. The tamarins exhibited high anxiety responses to predator scent compared to non-predator scent which produced low anxiety responses. No sex differences were found but an age difference was apparent: younger individuals were more curious than their elders. The response pattern was observed in captive-born individuals and was not affected by whether or not their parents were wild-caught or captive-born. This indicates that the discrimination of predator and non-predator scents is innate. However, this should not be taken to mean that captive cotton-top tamarins should be re-introduced to the wild without prior predator avoidance training. The implication of this study for animal welfare is that in captive environments where both predator and prey species are kept, it is important that predators, and their faeces, are not situated where prey species can detect their presence through olfaction, because prey species may suffer continual levels of heightened anxiety with possible detrimental effects.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Brack, M. « Lectin Histochemistry and Carcinoembryonic Antigen in Spontaneous Colonic Cancers of Cotton-top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Veterinary Pathology 32, no 6 (novembre 1995) : 668–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/030098589503200608.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
The glycoconjugate-binding properties and the expression of carcinoembryonic antigen in colonic cancers of cotton-top tamarins at the German Primate Center are described. Deviating from findings in non-tumorous colonic epithelia of cotton-top tamarins was the presence of UEA-, SBA-, or DBA-binding sites in the infranuclear cytoplasm of colonic epithelial cells, staining of entire cells, and the occurrence of subepithelial extracellular glycoconjugates in and adjacent to colonic cancers. Carcinoembryonic antigen, which normally was limited to the fuzzy coat of the colonic epithelium, was demonstrated in the cytoplasm of the cancer cells.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Locurto, Charles, Matthew Gagne et Kathryn Levesque. « Implicit chaining in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). » Journal of Experimental Psychology : Animal Behavior Processes 35, no 1 (2009) : 116–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012924.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Versace, Elisabetta, Jessica R. Rogge, Natalie Shelton-May et Andrea Ravignani. « Positional encoding in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Animal Cognition 22, no 5 (1 juillet 2019) : 825–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-019-01277-y.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Weiss, Daniel J., Jerald D. Kralik et Marc D. Hauser. « Face processing in cotton-top tamarins ( Saguinus oedipus ) ». Animal Cognition 3, no 4 (1 mars 2001) : 191–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s100710000076.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Miller, Jared, Marc Hauser, Cory Miller et Ricardo Gil-Da-Costa. « Selective Phonotaxis by Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus Oedipus) ». Behaviour 138, no 7 (2001) : 811–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853901753172665.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Hayes, Sheryl L., et Charles T. Snowdon. « Predator recognition in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». American Journal of Primatology 20, no 4 (1990) : 283–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350200404.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Snowdon, Charles, et Nicole Castro. « DEVELOPMENT OF VOCAL RESPONSES IN INFANT COTTON-TOP TAMARINS ». Behaviour 137, no 5 (2000) : 629–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853900502259.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
AbstractTo examine the development of production, comprehension and usage of chirp vocalizations in infant cotton-top tamarins, we designed four experimental settings to elicit five of the eight chirp types of adult cotton-top tamarins. We tested non-reproductive adult pairs and family groups before and after the birth of infants. We observed responses of infants in family groups during the first 20 weeks following birth. The five chirp types were rarely given in control conditions, but each type occurred at significant levels among adults and subadults immediately following presentation of an appropriate stimulus. Each chirp type was specific to one experimental manipulation. We found no differences between non-reproductive pairs and families prior to the birth of infants, but two chirp types were produced at significantly lower rates following the birth of infants. Infants produced a form of chirp (prototype chirp) not seen in adults, but infants rarely produced adult forms of chirps. If an adult-appropriate chirp was produced by an infant in one session, there was a low probability of that chirp appearing in subsequent tests with the same infant. Infants used most frequently the form of chirp (D chirp) that was most frequently directed toward them by adults, and infants reduced their rates of prototype chirps following presentation of visual and auditory alarm stimuli. Although infant tamarins under 20 weeks of age appear unable to use adult types of chirps consistently, they did produce chirps with adult form, and they appeared to show some comprehension of chirps produced by adults.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Roush, Rebecca S., et Charles T. Snowdon. « Ontogeny of food-associated calls in cotton-top tamarins ». Animal Behaviour 47, no 2 (février 1994) : 263–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1038.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Bardi, Massimo, Andrew J. Petto et David E. Lee-Parritz. « Parental failure in captive cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus Oedipus) ». American Journal of Primatology 54, no 3 (2001) : 159–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1020.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Price, Eluned C. « Competition To Carry Infants in Captive Families of Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus Oedipus) ». Behaviour 118, no 1-2 (1991) : 66–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853991x00201.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
AbstractThe Callitrichidae (marmosets and tamarins) typically produce twins, and have communal rearing systems in which all group members help care for the infants. It has been hypothesised that helpers benefit in some way from assisting in infant care. If so, then competition to carry infants would be predicted. This was tested in a study of 14 litters of captive cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). All occurrences of infant transfers (movements of infants from one caretaker to another) were recorded from birth to 12 weeks. Individuals in larger groups were less likely to reject infants, more likely to actively take infants, more likely to resist attempts by others to take, and more likely to intervene in transfers, suggesting increased competition to carry in large groups. Singletons were rejected less than twin infants, again suggesting the existence of competition amongst caretakers. Mothers rejected infants more frequently than fathers; young tamarins rejected infants more than older tamarins. There was evidence that carrying by juvenile siblings and by adult daughters was limited by other group members. There was evidence that adult sons and sub-adult sons and daughters competed most strongly, and were more likely to attempt to limit carrying by other group members. These results were interpreted in the light of hypotheses suggesting that the benefits to be gained from helping may differ amongst age-sex classes.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

Gokcekus, Samin, Rahel K. Brügger et Judith M. Burkart. « Active sharing of a novel, arbitrary innovation in captive cotton-top tamarins ? » Behaviour 158, no 1 (14 décembre 2020) : 51–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10049.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Abstract Most cultural behaviours in primates stem from innovations that are beneficial since they provide access to food or comfort. Innovations that are seemingly purposeless and arbitrary, and nevertheless spread through a social group, are rarer but particularly relevant to understanding the evolutionary origin of culture. Here, we provide an anecdotal report of a series of non-instrumental woodchip manipulation and modification events in captive cotton-top tamarins. Intriguingly, woodchips were preferentially manipulated in a position that was readily visible to a partner in a different enclosure, and the innovation apparently spread to other individuals. Together, this suggests that the arbitrary innovation was actively shared with a conspecific, which is consistent with the pattern of transmission of another arbitrary innovation in cotton-top tamarins, namely stick-weaving.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

KURETA, Yoichi. « Vocal exchanges between familiar cotton-top tamarins during social isolation ». Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology 50, no 1 (2000) : 95–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.2502/janip.50.95.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

Price, Eluned C., et W. C. McGrew. « Departures from Monogamy in Colonies of Captive Cotton-Top Tamarins ». Folia Primatologica 57, no 1 (1991) : 16–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000156559.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

Mansfield, Keith G., Kuei‐Chin Lin, Dongling Xia, Joseph V. Newman, David B. Schauer, John MacKey, Andrew A. Lackner et Angela Carville. « EnteropathogenicEscherichia coliand Ulcerative Colitis in Cotton‐Top Tamarins(Saguinus oedipus) ». Journal of Infectious Diseases 184, no 6 (15 septembre 2001) : 803–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/322990.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Uller, Claudia, Marc Hauser et Susan Carey. « Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins ( Saguinus oedipus). » Journal of Comparative Psychology 115, no 3 (2001) : 248–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.115.3.248.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Moore, Kirsty, Jennifer Cleland et W. C. McGrew. « Visual encounters between families of cotton-top tamarins,Saguinus oedipus ». Primates 32, no 1 (janvier 1991) : 23–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02381598.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Locurto, Charles, Matthew Gagne et Lauren Nutile. « Characteristics of implicit chaining in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Animal Cognition 13, no 4 (7 février 2010) : 617–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0312-2.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Leong, Kirsten M., Scott P. Terrell et Anne Savage. « Causes of mortality in captive cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Zoo Biology 23, no 2 (2004) : 127–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/zoo.10121.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

McConnell, Patricia B., et Charles T. Snowdon. « Vocal Interactions Between Unfamiliar Groups of Captive Cotton-Top Tamarins ». Behaviour 97, no 3-4 (1986) : 273–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853986x00649.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Scott, Jillian J., Kirsten L. Carlson et Charles T. Snowdon. « Labile sex differences in long calling in cotton-top tamarins ». American Journal of Primatology 68, no 2 (février 2006) : 153–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20213.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Roian Egnor, S. E., et Marc D. Hauser. « Noise-induced vocal modulation in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». American Journal of Primatology 68, no 12 (2006) : 1183–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20317.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

DuFrain, Russell J. « Is cancer of the colon familial in cotton-top tamarins ? » Cancer Genetics and Cytogenetics 14, no 1-2 (janvier 1985) : 83–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0165-4608(85)90218-3.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Kostan, Karen M., et Charles T. Snowdon. « Attachment and social preferences in cooperatively-reared cotton-top tamarins ». American Journal of Primatology 57, no 3 (25 juin 2002) : 131–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10040.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

Price, Eluned C. « The costs of infant carrying in captive cotton-top tamarins ». American Journal of Primatology 26, no 1 (1992) : 23–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350260106.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Keller, Karen L., R. Scott Fritz, Carlie M. Zoubek, Erica H. Kennedy, Katherine A. Cronin, Emily S. Rothwell et Thomas L. Serfass. « Effects of Transport on Fecal Glucocorticoid Levels in Captive-Bred Cotton-Top Tamarins (Sanguinus oedipus) ». Journal of the Pennsylvania Academy of Science 88, no 2 (1 juin 2014) : 84–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.5325/jpennacadscie.88.2.0084.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
ABSTRACT The relocation of animals can induce stress when animals are placed in novel environmental conditions. The movement of captive animals among facilities is common, especially for non-human primates used in research. The stress response begins with the activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis which results in the release of glucocorticoid hormones (GC), which at chronic levels could lead to deleterious physiological effects. There is a substantial body of data concerning GC levels and reproduction, and rank and aggression in primates. However, the effect of transport has received much less attention. Fecal samples from eight (four male and four female) captive-bred cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) were collected at four different time points (two pre-transport and two post-transport). The fecal samples were analyzed using an immunoassay to determine GC levels. A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) demonstrated that GC levels differed among transport times (p = 0.009), but not between sexes (p = 0.963). Five of the eight tamarins exhibited an increase in GC levels after transport. Seven of the eight tamarins exhibited a decrease in GC levels from three to six days post-transport to three weeks post-transport. Most values returned to pre- transport levels after three weeks. The results indicate that these tamarins experienced elevated GC levels following transport, but these increases were of short duration. This outcome would suggest that the negative effects of elevated GC levels were also of short duration.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Snowdon, Charles, et Gretchen Achenbach. « Response to Sibling Birth in Juvenile Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus Oedipus) ». Behaviour 135, no 7 (1998) : 845–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853998792640369.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Brack, M. « Adrenal gland tumours in two cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus oedipus) ». Laboratory Animals 34, no 1 (janvier 2000) : 106–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/002367700780577948.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Brack, M., et H. Klensang. « Pachyonychia congenita‐like disorder in cotton‐top tamarins ( Saguinus oedipus oedipus ) ». Journal of Medical Primatology 20, no 8 (octobre 1991) : 394–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0684.1991.tb00558.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

PORTER, T. A., et C. T. SNOWDON. « Female Reproductive Status and Male Pairmate Behavior in Cotton-Top Tamarins ». Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 807, no 1 Integrative N (janvier 1997) : 556–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb51967.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Elowson, A. Margaret, Pamela L. Tannenbaum et Charles T. Snowdon. « Food-associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins ». Animal Behaviour 42, no 6 (décembre 1991) : 931–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80145-9.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

Price, Eluned C. « Parturition and perinatal behaviour in captive cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Primates 31, no 4 (octobre 1990) : 523–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02382535.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Clapp, N., L. Adams et J. Fuhr. « Pathogenesis of spontaneous idiopathic colitis in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Agents and Actions 41, S2 (août 1994) : C238—C240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf01987652.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

McAuliffe, Katherine, Natalie Shelton et Lauren Stone. « Does effort influence inequity aversion in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ? » Animal Cognition 17, no 6 (4 juin 2014) : 1289–301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0764-x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Feistner, A. T. C., et A. S. Chamove. « High motivation toward food increases food-sharing in cotton-top tamarins ». Developmental Psychobiology 19, no 5 (septembre 1986) : 439–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.420190505.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Madara, James L. « Structural characterization of spontaneous colitis in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Digestive Diseases and Sciences 30, no 12 (décembre 1985) : 52S—53S. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf01296975.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Clapp, N., M. Henke, R. Hansard, R. Carson et D. Fretland. « Anti-colitic efficacy of SC-41930 in colitic cotton-top tamarins ». Agents and Actions 39, S1 (mars 1993) : C36—C38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf01972713.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Joyce, Stella M., et Charles T. Snowdon. « Developmental changes in food transfers in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». American Journal of Primatology 69, no 9 (2007) : 955–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20393.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Savage, Anne, Susan E. Shideler, Luis H. Soto, Jaime Causado, L. Humberto Giraldo, Bill L. Lasley et Charles T. Snowdon. « Reproductive events of wild cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) in Colombia ». American Journal of Primatology 43, no 4 (1997) : 329–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-2345(1997)43:4<329 ::aid-ajp4>3.0.co;2-z.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Locurto, Charles, Maura Fox et Andrea Mazzella. « Implicit learning in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and pigeons (Columba livia) ». Learning & ; Behavior 43, no 2 (12 février 2015) : 129–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0167-0.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Weiss, Daniel J., et Jason Wark. « Hysteresis effects in a motor task with cotton-top tamarins (Sanguinus oedipus). » Journal of Experimental Psychology : Animal Behavior Processes 35, no 3 (2009) : 427–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013964.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Cheverud, James, Eric Routman, Cashell Jaquish, Suzette Tardif, Gloria Peterson, Natasha Belfiore et Lisa Forman. « Quantitative and Molecular Genetic Variation in Captive Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Conservation Biology 8, no 1 (mars 1994) : 95–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1523-1739.1994.08010095.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Price, Eluned C. « Infant carrying as a courtship strategy of breeding male cotton-top tamarins ». Animal Behaviour 40, no 4 (octobre 1990) : 784–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80711-0.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Stevens, Jeffrey R. « Donor payoffs and other-regarding preferences in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Animal Cognition 13, no 4 (4 février 2010) : 663–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0309-x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Ziegler, Toni E., Kate F. Washabaugh et Charles T. Snowdon. « Responsiveness of expectant male cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus, to mate's pregnancy ». Hormones and Behavior 45, no 2 (février 2004) : 84–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.09.003.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Kalthoff, Donata, Wulf-Iwo Bock, Franziska Hühn, Martin Beer et Bernd Hoffmann. « Fatal Cowpox Virus Infection in Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) in Germany ». Vector-Borne and Zoonotic Diseases 14, no 4 (avril 2014) : 303–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/vbz.2013.1442.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Fernandez, Eduardo J., Nicole Dorey et Jesus Rosales-Ruiz. « A Two-Choice Preference Assessment With Five Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) ». Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7, no 3 (juillet 2004) : 163–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0703_2.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Nous offrons des réductions sur tous les plans premium pour les auteurs dont les œuvres sont incluses dans des sélections littéraires thématiques. Contactez-nous pour obtenir un code promo unique!

Vers la bibliographie