Littérature scientifique sur le sujet « Attentional tracking »

Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres

Choisissez une source :

Consultez les listes thématiques d’articles de revues, de livres, de thèses, de rapports de conférences et d’autres sources académiques sur le sujet « Attentional tracking ».

À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.

Articles de revues sur le sujet "Attentional tracking"

1

Seiffert, A. E. « Attentional tracking across display translations ». Journal of Vision 5, no 8 (17 mars 2010) : 643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/5.8.643.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Pichlmeier, Sebastian, et Till Pfeiffer. « Attentional capture in multiple object tracking ». Journal of Vision 21, no 8 (11 août 2021) : 16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.8.16.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Drew, Trafton, Andrew W. McCollough, Todd S. Horowitz et Edward K. Vogel. « Attentional enhancement during multiple-object tracking ». Psychonomic Bulletin & ; Review 16, no 2 (avril 2009) : 411–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pbr.16.2.411.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Makin, Simon J., Anthony J. Watkins et Andrew P. Raimond. « Attentional tracking in real‐room reverberation. » Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 129, no 4 (avril 2011) : 2623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.3588717.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Watkins, Anthony J., Simon J. Makin et Andrew Raimond. « Room‐acoustic factors in attentional tracking ». Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123, no 5 (mai 2008) : 2976. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.2932481.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Mounts, J. R. W., B. S. Amos, M. A. Moschetta et E. C. Page. « Multiple object tracking and attentional capture ». Journal of Vision 6, no 6 (24 mars 2010) : 769. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/6.6.769.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Sears, Christopher R., et Zenon W. Pylyshyn. « Multiple object tracking and attentional processing. » Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 54, no 1 (2000) : 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087326.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Tombu, Michael, et Adriane E. Seiffert. « Attentional costs in multiple-object tracking ». Cognition 108, no 1 (juillet 2008) : 1–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.014.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Chen, Nigel T. M., Patrick J. F. Clarke, Tamara L. Watson, Colin MacLeod et Adam J. Guastella. « Attentional bias modification facilitates attentional control mechanisms : Evidence from eye tracking ». Biological Psychology 104 (janvier 2015) : 139–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.002.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Seiffert, A. E. « Dissociating neural correlates of attentional tracking and attention to visual motion ». Journal of Vision 3, no 9 (18 mars 2010) : 868. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/3.9.868.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.

Thèses sur le sujet "Attentional tracking"

1

Drew, Trafton 1980. « Electrophysiological measures of attentional tracking and working memory ». Thesis, University of Oregon, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/1794/10198.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
xiii, 155 p. : ill. A print copy of this thesis is available through the UO Libraries. Search the library catalog for the location and call number.
In the multiple object tracking (MOT) task, observers are presented with multiple identical objects, some of which are temporarily identified as targets. After a selection period, all objects move randomly and independently for several seconds. At the end the motion period, all objects stop and observers must identify the target objects again. This task has been used to study a variety of important cognitive questions from object-based attention to cognitive development, divided attention and the development of expertise. Yet, surprisingly little is known about the neural mechanisms that underlie the ability to track multiple targets independently. Although a number of researchers have used fMRI (functional magnetic imaging) to examine what areas are active during MOT, the current set of studies is the first to employ ERPs (event-related potentials) to examine the neural mechanisms of MOT. With excellent temporal resolution, the ERP methodology allows researchers to delineate the time course of different phases of a single task with millisecond precision, something not possible with fMRI. In Chapter II, we manipulated the number of targets and difficulty of tracking and observed a lateralized contralateral negativity that was sensitive to the number of targets but not difficulty of tracking. Chapter III examined the effect of irrelevant white probes flashed briefly throughout the trial while observers tracked. We observed modulations of early visual components that indicated that during tracking, spatial attention focused on targets but did not differentiate between distractors and empty space. Finally, in Chapter IV, we examined the relationship between visual working memory (VWM) and MOT by manipulating the presence or absence of task relevant motion. We found that the waveforms evoked by an MOT task in the absence of task-relevant motion were nearly identical to waveforms evoked by the VWM task, suggesting that VWM is an important part of the typical MOT task. This thesis includes previously published and unpublished material.
Committee in Charge: Edward Vogel, Chairperson, Psychology; Edward Awh, Member, Psychology; Ulrich Mayr, Member, Psychology; Paul van Donkelaar, Outside Member, Human Physiology
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Valcheff, Danielle. « RESILIENCE AND ATTENTIONAL BIASES : WHAT YOU SEE MAY BE WHAT YOU GET ». Thesis, Laurentian University of Sudbury, 2014. https://zone.biblio.laurentian.ca/dspace/handle/10219/2143.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Research suggests that, during stress, resilient individuals use positive emotion regulation strategies and experience a greater number of positive emotions than those who are less resilient. Therefore, differences could be expected in attentional biases towards emotional stimuli based on resilience. The current study investigated attentional biases towards neutral, negative and positive images in response to varying levels of resilence and mood induction conditions (neutral, negative and positive). Sixty participants viewed a series of pre and post-mood induction slides in order to measure attentional biases to emotional stimuli. The study provided evidence for the presence of trait and state congruent attentional biases. More resilient individuals demonstrated an initial bias towards positive stimuli and once emotion was aroused, the bias was away from negative stimuli. Additionally, mood congruent attentional biases were observed for participants induced into positive and negative mood states. Implications as they apply to research and clinical practice are discussed.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Mullen, Mairead. « Attentional bias in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder : an eye-tracking methodology ». Thesis, Queen's University Belfast, 2016. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.709841.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
The P300 in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Meta-Analysis The P300 event-related potential (ERP) component has potential utility as a neurological marker of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD). Currently there is no reliable consensus relating to the direction and magnitude of P300 waveform differences between OCD and healthy control samples. We sought to combine previous research which documented P300 amplitude and latency data within OCD and healthy control groups using the Auditory Oddball (AO) task. A systematic search was conducted for studies reporting P300 amplitude and latency data of target trials in the AO task in patient (OCD) and control groups. Pooled effect sizes were calculated. Individuals with OCD demonstrated significantly reduced P300 latencies during target trials compared to healthy controls. Differences in the magnitude of P300 amplitude between individuals with OCD and healthy controls were non-significant. Attentional Bias in Individuals with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: An Eye-Tracking Methodology Attentional biases are implicated in theoretical models of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, however they are not consistently detected using conventional reaction time paradigms. Eye-tracking methods enable greater precision in revealing patterns of attentional bias. Participants with OCD (n=16) were compared with a healthy control sample (n=16). Measures of vigilance, disengagement and maintenance bias were investigated by recording eye-movements during a free-gaze task in which pairs of neutral- and OCD, or neutral- and aversive images were presented. The OCD group demonstrated no evidence of vigilance or delayed disengagement biases. However there were significant group differences in measures of maintenance attentional bias. Evidence of hypervigilant orienting to all stimuli was observed during early visual processing.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Qwillbard, Tony. « Less information, more thinking : How attentional behavior predicts learning in mathematics ». Thesis, Umeå universitet, Institutionen för psykologi, 2014. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-100999.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
It has been shown in experiments that a method of teaching where students are encouraged to create their own solution methods to mathematical problems (creative mathematically founded reasoning, CMR) results in better learning and proficiency than one where students are provided with solution methods for them to practice by repetition (algorithmic reasoning, AR). The present study investigated whether students in an AR practice condition pay less attention to information relevant for mathematical problem solving than students in a CMR condition. To test this, attentional behavior during practice was measured using eye-tracking equipment. These measurements were then associated with task proficiency in a follow-up test one week after the practice session. The findings support the theory and confirm previous studies in that CMR leads to better task performance in the follow-up test. The findings also suggest that students within the CMR condition whom focus less on extraneous information perform better.
Experiment har visat att en undervisningsmetod i vilken elever uppmuntras att själva komma på lösningsmetoder till matematiska problem (creative mathematically founded reasoning, CMR) resulterar i bättre inlärning och färdighet än en metod i vilken eleverna ges en färdig en lösningsmetod att öva på genom repetition (algorithmic reasoning, AR). Denna studie undersöker om elever under en AR-träningsbetingelse ägnar mindre uppmärksamhet åt information som är relevant för matematisk problemlösning än vad elever under en CMR-träningsbetingelse gör. För att testa detta mättes elevernas uppmärksamhetsbeteende under träning med hjälp av ögonrörelsekamera. Måtten ställdes sedan i relation till uppgiftsfärdighet i ett uppföljningstest en vecka efter träningssessionen. Resultaten stödjer teorin och bekräftar tidigare studier som visat att CMR leder till bättre prestation i uppföljningstestet. Resultaten tyder även på att de elever under CMR-betingelsen som fokuserar minst på ovidkommande information presterar bättre.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Amar, Kaur. « Attentional biases associated with health threat, and their modification ». Thesis, The University of Sydney, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/2123/12678.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
This thesis has two related aims: (i) to investigate whether situational health threat influences individuals’ pattern of attentional bias; and (ii) to examine the causal contribution of attentional biases to anxiety vulnerability following health threat, by modifying these biases. Results suggest that health threat, as compared to health reassurance, is associated with a greater bias towards all negative words; both in terms of the initial orienting of eye-gaze and the bias indices on an attentional probe task that presented stimuli for 500 ms. Although eye-tracking data do not indicate group differences in the maintenance of attention, bias indices following 1500 ms stimulus presentations were specific to negative health-related stimuli, suggesting that bias may become concern-specific between 500 and 1500 ms. Group differences were not found on an emotional Stroop task. In both attentional bias modification (ABM) studies, training tasks were not effective in modifying attention; therefore, conclusions regarding the effects of bias modification cannot be made. Nevertheless, those who completed a task designed to train attention towards negative health-related words displayed a greater increase in skin conductance following health threat, relative to those who completed a task designed to train attention towards neutral words. Groups did not differ on heart rate response or self-report outcome measures. A task designed to train attention towards negative general words did not lead to group differences in outcome measures as compared to a task designed to train attention towards neutral words. In line with theoretical models, health-related feedback appears to trigger attentional bias. However, further research examining the role of attentional biases in the context of health threat and health anxiety is warranted, as is ABM research that explores the mechanisms of change, and individual differences that may influence the effects of ABM.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Mallon, Peadar. « An experimental manipulation of attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli : an eye tracking study ». Thesis, Queen's University Belfast, 2014. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.675852.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
A Comparison of Measures of Attentional Bias to Alcohol in Social Drinkers: A Systematic Review. Research focusing on implicit cognitive processes, over the past two decades, has highlighted the role of Attentional Bias (AB) in addiction. No review has systematically addressed the question of how consistently AB is found in the social drinking population nor have they compared the use of the main paradigms in social drinkers. This review aimed to further understanding of the development of addiction and provide future directions for research. 15 studies were identified for inclusion in this review. Results indicated that AB to alcohol was inconsistently found and that methodological issues within and between paradigms may contribute to this. These findings raise questions with regards the robustness of conclusions which are drawn from previous studies using social drinkers as I comparison groups. Methodological considerations have been identified and attempts made to address these and provide direction for future research in this area. An experimental manipulation of Attentional Bias (AB) to alcohol related stimuli: An Eye Tracking (ET) Study. The study used ET technology and a Visual Probe Task (VPT) to measure the effects of an attentional training exercise on AB towards alcohol and subjective levels of craving. The training exercise used a modified version of the VPT with 45 heavy social drinkers were randomly allocated to one of three groups; Alcohol-Attend (attention trained to alcohol stimuli), Alcohol-Avoid (attention trained away from alcohol stimuli), and Control (attention not manipulated). AB and subjective levels of craving were recorded pre- and post-training exercise. The Attend-Alcohol group had significantly increased; fixations times to alcohol stimuli, and increased craving. Findings highlight the benefits of more technologically advanced and direct measures of AB. They support future research in a clinical population to examine the potential of AB training exercises in problem drinkers.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Rytwinski, Nina Katherine. « Do people with symptoms of depression exhibit a negative attentional bias or depressive evenhandedness ? » Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2010. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1276562437.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Sedall, Stephanie Nicole Sedall. « Aging and Emotion Recognition : An Examination of Stimulus and Attentional Mechanisms ». Cleveland State University / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=csu1463498266.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Monem, Ramey G. « ATTENTIONAL BIAS TO ALCOHOL IN AN IN VIVO SETTING ». UKnowledge, 2018. https://uknowledge.uky.edu/psychology_etds/146.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
The phenomenon of attentional bias to alcohol, where drinkers demonstrate a preference in allocating visual attention towards alcohol-related stimuli rather than neutral stimuli, is well-established. Studies detecting this phenomenon typically utilize computer-administered stimulus presentation tasks such as the visual dot probe task. Despite their frequency of use, these tasks do not represent the ways in which individuals typically encounter alcohol outside of the laboratory. Typical environments where alcohol is present allow individuals to move about freely and encounter alcohol while also being exposed to many other stimuli. This dissertation sought to implement a novel approach to assessing attentional bias in vivo, and identify how alcohol consumption might influence such in vivo attentional bias. This two-study dissertation utilized an in vivo task where participants looked freely around a room representing a recreational setting containing numerous objects while portable eye-tracking glasses monitored what an individual looked at and for how long. Target items of alcohol and neutral beverages were placed throughout the environment and fixation time spent on these objects was recorded. The first study of this dissertation examined attentional bias to alcohol-related objects across two identical testing sessions to understand the impact of novelty on allocation of in vivo attention. The second study tested individuals using the same in vivo assessment following a 0.30 g/kg dose of alcohol, a 0.65 g/kg dose of alcohol and a placebo. Participants also completed the visual dot probe task in order to measure and compare their attentional bias in a more traditionally implemented task to the novel in vivo approach. Results from the first study indicate that as the novelty of stimuli begins to wane and habituation to neutral stimuli occurs, attentional bias to alcohol-related objects emerges. This attentional bias was shown to be related to drinking habits, where heavier drinkers demonstrated increased attentional bias. The second study in this research found no discernible effect of alcohol consumption on in vivo attentional bias, but did identify a satiating effect of consumption on bias as measured by the visual dot probe task. Additional visual dot probe findings suggest the specificity of the effect of alcohol consumption on attentional bias. Together, these findings help inform whether there is benefit in utilizing an ecological model of measuring attentional bias and how the phenomenon might be measured in laboratory settings in the future.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Correa, John Bernard. « An Experimental Evaluation of the Relationship Between In-Vivo Stimuli and Attentional Bias to Smoking and Food Cues Among Female Smokers ». Scholar Commons, 2015. http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/5670.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Background: Cross-sectional and experimental research has shown that female smokers more frequently report using cigarettes to control negative affect, manage dietary restraint, and suppress body image dissatisfaction. However, there has been little research to identify cognitive mechanisms that may underlie these effects. Cross-stimulus attentional bias is one such mechanism. Aims and Hypotheses: We hypothesized that, when compared to neutral stimuli, in-vivo appetitive stimuli would enhance motivation to obtain a particular substance. More specifically, in-vivo smoking stimuli would increase attentional bias to smoking-related pictorial cues, whereas in-vivo food stimuli would increase attention to smoking-related and food-related pictorial cues. We also hypothesized that environmental tobacco smoke exposure history, negative affect, dietary restraint, body image dissatisfaction, and perceived appetite suppression of smoking would influence these attentional biases, such that higher levels of these characteristics would produce greater attentional biases. Method: Thirty-five female smokers were exposed to visual stimuli containing two independent pictorial cues: smoking/neutral, smoking/food, neutral/food, or neutral/neutral. Twenty images were presented in 3 counter-balanced, within-subjects sets differentiated by smoking (cigarette pack), food (snack) and neutral (jewelry) in-vivo stimuli. Attentional bias was measured using eye-tracking technology. Dietary restraint, body image dissatisfaction, negative affect, and environmental tobacco smoke exposure were assessed with self-report measures before the manipulations. Results: Effects counter to the hypotheses were observed, as in-vivo cigarettes and snack foods did not cause participants to differentially attend to pictorial smoking or food stimuli. Initial and maintained attention to smoking pictorial cues was greater than attention to food and neutral cues only when participants were administered a non-appetitive in-vivo stimulus. None of the theoretically hypothesized personality characteristics served as predictors or moderators of attentional bias. Discussion: Findings with the neutral in-vivo stimulus replicate and extend previous research identifying attentional bias for smoking cues among smokers. Results also enhance understanding of how attentional bias may change when smokers encounter other types of appetitive stimuli. These findings encourage further theoretical and clinical exploration of how the relationship between motivation and attentional bias can be conceptualized and translated from the laboratory to the natural environment.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.

Livres sur le sujet "Attentional tracking"

1

Pal, Rajarshi. Innovative research in attention modeling and computer vision applications. Hershey, PA : Information Science Reference, 2016.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Context-aware and Attentional Visual Tracking : Concepts, Algorithms, Experiments. VDM Verlag Dr. Müller, 2010.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Kim, Munchurl. Focus of attention based on gamma kernels for automatic target recognition. 1996.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Lu, Zhong-Lin, et George Sperling. Attention-Generated Apparent Motion. Oxford University Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199794607.003.0072.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
This chapter explores attention-generated apparent motion. A flickering display can seem to appear to move in opposite directions depending on which feature the observer attends to in the display. The illusory motion, generated by attention, demonstrates the mechanism of the third-order motion system: a dynamic salience map of the locations of the most salient stimulus features is determined jointly by stimulus strength (bottom-up) and by selective attention (top-down). Motion is computed directly and automatically from the salience map. Concepts covered in this chapter include apparent motion, first-order motion and second-order motion, feature tracking, salience maps, bottom-up processing, and top-down processing.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Chest, Visual. Visual Tracking Practice : Exercises for Improving Cognitive Skills : Reading, Paying Attention and Remembering. Independently Published, 2022.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

N&b Workout. Energy Flows Where Attention Goes : Running and Bodybuilding Log Book with Diet / Warm up / Regeneration Tracking for Women, Girls - Green. Independently Published, 2019.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

N&B WORKOUT. Energy Flows Where Attention Goes : Running and Bodybuilding Log Book with Diet / Warm up / Regeneration Tracking for Women, Girls - Mint. Independently Published, 2019.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Key, The. Visual Tracking Exercise Workbook for Visual Discrimination : Improve Cognitive Skills, Concentration, and Attention with Visual Therapy Activities and Practice Worksheets. Independently Published, 2022.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

N&b Workout. Energy Flows Where Attention Goes : Running and Bodybuilding Log Book with Diet / Warm up / Regeneration Tracking for Women, Girls - Purple. Independently Published, 2019.

Trouver le texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Boydstun, Amber E., et Annelise Russell. From Crisis to Stasis : Media Dynamics and Issue Attention in the News. Oxford University Press, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.56.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Media coverage does not ebb and flow. Rather, media coverage rapidly moves from crisis to stasis and back again. The result of these attention dynamics is news reporting that is disproportional to the breadth and pace of policy problems in the world, where some balloon in the news beyond expectations and others fade quickly (or never make the news at all). These patterns of news coverage result from the powerful role that momentum plays in the news-generation process. Forces of positive feedback drive news outlets to chase each new hot story quickly, while negative feedback forces drive news outlets to stay locked onto a hot story at hand. Together, these forces drive news coverage to lurch and fixate, lurch and fixate, again and again. Thus, although previous research has conceived of the news-generation process functioning either as a “patrol” system (where news outlets act as sentinels, tracking each policy problem as it unfolds in the world) or as an “alarm” system (where news outlets move in quick bursts from one policy problem to the next, with little to no in-depth coverage), both these previous models tell only half the story. Rather, the news-generation process is best understood through the alarm/patrol hybrid model, where news outlets often lurch from one hot item to the next but sometimes become entrenched in an unfolding storyline. The alarm/patrol hybrid model helps explain the particular phenomenon of “media storms” that can occur, where a sudden surge in media attention can vault a previously ignored issue into the center of public and political attention; think of the Catholic priest abuse scandal, or the scene in Ferguson, Missouri, after Michael Brown’s death. The lurching/fixating dynamics of media attention have far-ranging implications for citizen information and political response, contributing to a wider system of disproportionate information processing where some topics are attended to and others are largely ignored. In particular, because policymakers take so many of their cues from the news, it is likely the case that the lurching/fixating patterns of our media system exacerbate the punctuated patterns of government in turn.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.

Chapitres de livres sur le sujet "Attentional tracking"

1

Tan, Ke, et Zhenzhong Wei. « Visual Tracking with Attentional Convolutional Siamese Networks ». Dans Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 369–80. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-34120-6_30.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Young, William R., et Toby J. Ellmers. « Translating Attentional Control Theory to Applied Psychological Eye Tracking Research ». Dans Neuromethods, 131–49. New York, NY : Springer US, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-0716-2391-6_8.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Argudo-Vásconez, Andrea, O. Alvarado-Cando, Cristian Caldero, Franklin Buele, P. Ortega-Chasi et M. Cobos-Cali. « Measure of the Attentional Bias in Children Using Eye Tracking During a Psychological Test ». Dans Lecture Notes in Networks and Systems, 809–16. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-80091-8_96.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Figueiredo, Germano R., Daniel Campos, Wagner L. Ripka et Leandra Ulbricht. « Attentional Bias for Faces in Relation to Severity of Depressive Symptoms : An Eye-Tracking Study ». Dans XXVI Brazilian Congress on Biomedical Engineering, 415–19. Singapore : Springer Singapore, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-2119-1_63.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Duchowski, Andrew T. « Visual Attention ». Dans Eye Tracking Methodology, 3–13. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57883-5_1.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Hüsser, Andreas, et Werner Wirth. « Do Investors Show an Attentional Bias toward Past Performance ? An Eye-Tracking Experiment on Visual Attention to Mutual Fund Disclosures in Simplified Fund Prospectuses ». Dans Financial Literacy and the Limits of Financial Decision-Making, 77–102. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-30886-9_5.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Marougkas, Isidoros, Petros Koutras, Nikos Kardaris, Georgios Retsinas, Georgia Chalvatzaki et Petros Maragos. « How to Track Your Dragon : A Multi-attentional Framework for Real-Time RGB-D 6-DOF Object Pose Tracking ». Dans Computer Vision – ECCV 2020 Workshops, 682–99. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-66096-3_45.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Rosa, Pedro J., Diogo Morais, Jorge Oliveira, Pedro Gamito, Olivia Smyth et Matthew Pavlovic. « Assessment of Attentional and Mnesic Processes Through Gaze Tracking Analysis : Inferences from Comparative Search Tasks Embedded in VR Serious Games ». Dans Communications in Computer and Information Science, 26–34. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69694-2_3.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Gutierrez-Maldonado, José, Mar Clua i Sánchez, Bruno Porras-Garcia, Marta Ferrer-Garcia, Eduardo Serrano, Marta Carulla, Franck Meschberger-Annweiler et Mariarca Ascione. « Body-Related Attentional Bias in Anorexia Nervosa and Body Dissatisfaction in Females : An Eye-Tracking and Virtual Reality New Paradigm ». Dans Virtual, Augmented and Mixed Reality : Design and Development, 443–54. Cham : Springer International Publishing, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-05939-1_30.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Duchowski, Andrew T. « Visual Attention ». Dans Eye Tracking Methodology : Theory and Practice, 3–15. London : Springer London, 2003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-3750-4_1.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.

Actes de conférences sur le sujet "Attentional tracking"

1

Mazzei, Andrea, Tabea Koll, Frédéric Kaplan et Pierre Dillenbourg. « Attentional processes in natural reading ». Dans ETRA '14 : Eye Tracking Research and Applications. New York, NY, USA : ACM, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2578153.2578195.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Rusanowska, Marzena. « Attentional retraining in depressive disorders ». Dans ETRA '14 : Eye Tracking Research and Applications. New York, NY, USA : ACM, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2578153.2583040.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Yang, Ming, Junsong Yuan et Ying Wu. « Spatial selection for attentional visual tracking ». Dans 2007 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition. IEEE, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cvpr.2007.383178.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Kong, Xiangdong, et Xianbin Cao. « Attentional convolutional neural networks for object tracking ». Dans 2018 Integrated Communications, Navigation, Surveillance Conference (ICNS). IEEE, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icnsurv.2018.8384994.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Kong, Xiangdong, Baochang Zhang, Lei Yue et Zehao Xiao. « Attentional convolutional neural networks for object tracking ». Dans 2018 Integrated Communications, Navigation, Surveillance Conference (ICNS). IEEE, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icnsurv.2018.8384903.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Mahadevan, Vijay, et Nuno Vasconcelos. « Automatic initialization and tracking using attentional mechanisms ». Dans 2011 IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition Workshops (CVPR Workshops). IEEE, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cvprw.2011.5981782.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Gao, Junyan, Zhenguo Yang et Wenyin Liu. « Template Attentional Siamese Network for Object Tracking ». Dans ICVIP 2018 : 2018 the 2nd International Conference on Video and Image Processing. New York, NY, USA : ACM, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3301506.3301544.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Soret, Rébaï, Christophe Hurter et Vsevolod Peysakhovich. « Attentional orienting in real and virtual 360-degree environments ». Dans ETRA '19 : 2019 Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications. New York, NY, USA : ACM, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3314111.3322871.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Wang, Qiang, Zhu Teng, Junliang Xing, Jin Gao, Weiming Hu et Stephen Maybank. « Learning Attentions : Residual Attentional Siamese Network for High Performance Online Visual Tracking ». Dans 2018 IEEE/CVF Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR). IEEE, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cvpr.2018.00510.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Choi, Jongwon, Hyung Jin Chang, Sangdoo Yun, Tobias Fischer, Yiannis Demiris et Jin Young Choi. « Attentional Correlation Filter Network for Adaptive Visual Tracking ». Dans 2017 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR). IEEE, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cvpr.2017.513.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.

Rapports d'organisations sur le sujet "Attentional tracking"

1

Dutra, Lauren M., James Nonnemaker, Nathaniel Taylor, Ashley Feld, Brian Bradfield, John Holloway, Edward (Chip) Hill et Annice Kim. Visual Attention to Tobacco-Related Stimuli in a 3D Virtual Store. RTI Press, mai 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.3768/rtipress.2020.rr.0036.2005.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
We used eye tracking to measure visual attention to tobacco products and pro- and anti-tobacco advertisements (pro-ads and anti-ads) during a shopping task in a three-dimensional virtual convenience store. We used eye-tracking hardware to track the percentage of fixations (number of times the eye was essentially stationary; F) and dwell time (time spent looking at an object; DT) for several categories of objects and ads for 30 adult current cigarette smokers. We used Wald F-tests to compare fixations and dwell time across categories, adjusting comparisons of ads by the number of each type of ad. Overall, unadjusted for the number of each object, participants focused significantly greater attention on snacks and drinks and tobacco products than ads (all P<0.005). Adjusting for the number of each type of ad viewed, participants devoted significantly greater visual attention to pro-ads than anti-ads or ads unrelated to tobacco (P<0.001). Visual attention for anti-ads was significantly greater when the ads were placed on the store’s external walls or hung from the ceiling than when placed on the gas pump or floor (P<0.005). In a cluttered convenience store environment, anti-ads at the point of sale have to compete with many other stimuli. Restrictions on tobacco product displays and advertisements at the point of sale could reduce the stimuli that attract smokers’ attention away from anti-ads.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Chaparadza, Diana. An Analysis of Patient-Generated Health Data in Assisting Nurses and Physicians to Better Treat Patients with Hypertension. University of Tennessee Health Science Center, novembre 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.21007/chp.hiim.0080.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Patient Generated Health Data (PGHD is not new but it has gained more attention these past years due to the advent of smart devices, remote monitoring devices and many applications on various smart devices. PGHD reflects medications and treatment, lifestyle choices, and health history. Unlike traditional medical visits, where clinicians collect and manage data within their offices, PGHD is collected by patients throughout the course of their day and provides an insight of how they are responding to treatments or lifestyle choices. Examples include blood glucose monitoring or blood pressure readings using home health equipment, exercise and diet tracking using mobile applications or wearable devices such as the Fitbit or other smart watches.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Anthony, Ian, Fei Su et Lora Saalman. Naval Incident Management in Europe, East Asia and South East Asia. Stockholm International Peace Research Institute, mars 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.55163/zzbg6990.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Unprecedented global turbulence in 2022 has demonstrated the need to pay increased attention to naval operations. Enhanced military capability allows naval power projection far beyond home waters. New threats and challenges are emerging from technological advances and new applications, not least the vulnerability of warships and naval facilities to cyber intrusions and cyberattacks. As states implement the programmes they need to protect and promote their interests at sea, there is also likely to be an increase in the number of close tracking incidents. How effective current risk reduction mechanisms will be at dealing with incidents at sea is unclear. This Insights Paper provides a preliminary assessment of the existing mechanisms and suggests areas for further improvement.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Nous offrons des réductions sur tous les plans premium pour les auteurs dont les œuvres sont incluses dans des sélections littéraires thématiques. Contactez-nous pour obtenir un code promo unique!

Vers la bibliographie