Articles de revues sur le sujet « Action and perception »

Pour voir les autres types de publications sur ce sujet consultez le lien suivant : Action and perception.

Créez une référence correcte selon les styles APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard et plusieurs autres

Choisissez une source :

Consultez les 50 meilleurs articles de revues pour votre recherche sur le sujet « Action and perception ».

À côté de chaque source dans la liste de références il y a un bouton « Ajouter à la bibliographie ». Cliquez sur ce bouton, et nous générerons automatiquement la référence bibliographique pour la source choisie selon votre style de citation préféré : APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

Vous pouvez aussi télécharger le texte intégral de la publication scolaire au format pdf et consulter son résumé en ligne lorsque ces informations sont inclues dans les métadonnées.

Parcourez les articles de revues sur diverses disciplines et organisez correctement votre bibliographie.

1

Creem-Regehr, Sarah H. « Perception by action versus perception for action ». Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, no 11 (novembre 2005) : 510–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.004.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Bohg, Jeannette, Karol Hausman, Bharath Sankaran, Oliver Brock, Danica Kragic, Stefan Schaal et Gaurav S. Sukhatme. « Interactive Perception : Leveraging Action in Perception and Perception in Action ». IEEE Transactions on Robotics 33, no 6 (décembre 2017) : 1273–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/tro.2017.2721939.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Bailey, Heather R., Christopher A. Kurby, Tania Giovannetti et Jeffrey M. Zacks. « Action perception predicts action performance ». Neuropsychologia 51, no 11 (septembre 2013) : 2294–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.022.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Brown, Alastair. « Perception to action ». Nature Climate Change 3, no 2 (29 janvier 2013) : 98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1819.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Nanay, Bence. « Action-oriented Perception ». European Journal of Philosophy 20, no 3 (27 mai 2010) : 430–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0378.2010.00407.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Creem‐Regehr, Sarah H., et Benjamin R. Kunz. « Perception and action ». Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews : Cognitive Science 1, no 6 (14 mai 2010) : 800–810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.82.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Fantoni, Carlo, Sara Rigutti et Walter Gerbino. « Bodily action penetrates affective perception ». PeerJ 4 (15 février 2016) : e1677. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1677.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Fantoni & Gerbino (2014) showed that subtle postural shifts associated with reaching can have a strong hedonic impact and affect how actors experience facial expressions of emotion. Using a novel Motor Action Mood Induction Procedure (MAMIP), they found consistent congruency effects in participants who performed a facial emotionidentificationtask after a sequence of visually-guided reaches: a face perceived as neutral in a baseline condition appeared slightly happy after comfortable actions and slightly angry after uncomfortable actions. However, skeptics about the penetrability of perception (Zeimbekis & Raftopoulos, 2015) would consider such evidence insufficient to demonstrate that observer’s internal states induced by action comfort/discomfort affect perception in a top-down fashion. The action-modulated mood might have produced a back-end memory effect capable of affecting post-perceptual and decision processing, but not front-end perception.Here, we present evidence that performing a facial emotiondetection(not identification) task after MAMIP exhibits systematic mood-congruentsensitivitychanges, rather than responsebiaschanges attributable to cognitive set shifts; i.e., we show that observer’s internal states induced by bodily action can modulate affective perception. The detection threshold forhappinesswas lower after fifty comfortable than uncomfortable reaches; while the detection threshold forangerwas lower after fifty uncomfortable than comfortable reaches. Action valence induced an overall sensitivity improvement in detecting subtle variations of congruent facial expressions (happiness afterpositivecomfortable actions, anger afternegativeuncomfortable actions), in the absence of significant response bias shifts. Notably, both comfortable and uncomfortable reaches impact sensitivity in an approximately symmetric way relative to a baseline inaction condition. All of these constitute compelling evidence of a genuine top-down effect on perception: specifically, facial expressions of emotion arepenetrableby action-induced mood. Affective priming by action valence is a candidate mechanism for the influence of observer’s internal states on properties experienced as phenomenally objective and yet loaded with meaning.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Press, Clare, Elena Gherri, Cecilia Heyes et Martin Eimer. « Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action ». Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, no 10 (octobre 2010) : 2198–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21409.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Several theories of the mechanisms linking perception and action require that the links are bidirectional, but there is a lack of consensus on the effects that action has on perception. We investigated this by measuring visual event-related brain potentials to observed hand actions while participants prepared responses that were spatially compatible (e.g., both were on the left side of the body) or incompatible and action type compatible (e.g., both were finger taps) or incompatible, with observed actions. An early enhanced processing of spatially compatible stimuli was observed, which is likely due to spatial attention. This was followed by an attenuation of processing for both spatially and action type compatible stimuli, likely to be driven by efference copy signals that attenuate processing of predicted sensory consequences of actions. Attenuation was not response-modality specific; it was found for manual stimuli when participants prepared manual and vocal responses, in line with the hypothesis that action control is hierarchically organized. These results indicate that spatial attention and forward model prediction mechanisms have opposite, but temporally distinct, effects on perception. This hypothesis can explain the inconsistency of recent findings on action–perception links and thereby supports the view that sensorimotor links are bidirectional. Such effects of action on perception are likely to be crucial, not only for the control of our own actions but also in sociocultural interaction, allowing us to predict the reactions of others to our own actions.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Cañal-Bruland, Rouwen, et John van der Kamp. « Action goals influence action-specific perception ». Psychonomic Bulletin & ; Review 16, no 6 (décembre 2009) : 1100–1105. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pbr.16.6.1100.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Beets, I. A. M., B. M. ’t Hart, F. Rösler, D. Y. P. Henriques, W. Einhäuser et K. Fiehler. « Online action-to-perception transfer : Only percept-dependent action affects perception ». Vision Research 50, no 24 (décembre 2010) : 2633–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.004.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Vogt, Stefan. « Imagery and perception-action mediation in imitative actions ». Cognitive Brain Research 3, no 2 (mars 1996) : 79–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0926-6410(95)00032-1.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Le Runigo, Cyrille, Nicolas Benguigui et Benoit G. Bardy. « Perception–action coupling and expertise in interceptive actions ». Human Movement Science 24, no 3 (juin 2005) : 429–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.008.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Scott, Blake D. « From Perception to Action ». Sartre Studies International 26, no 2 (1 décembre 2020) : 51–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.3167/ssi.2020.260205.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
This paper re-examines the well-known problem of how it is possible to have an “intuition of absences” in Sartre’s example of Pierre. I argue that this problem is symptomatic of an overly theoretical interpretation of Sartre’s use of intentionality. First, I review Husserl’s notion of evidence within his phenomenology. Next, I introduce Sartre’s Pierre example and highlight some difficulties with interpreting it as a problem of perception. By focusing on Sartre’s notion of the project, I argue instead that the problem is better understood at the level of action. In support of this interpretation, I conclude with a brief comparison to the early work of Paul Ricoeur. By borrowing some of Ricoeur’s phenomenological vocabulary tailored to action, I reinterpret Sartre’s example as a practical problem.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Zaporozhets, A. V. « Perception, Movement, and Action ». Journal of Russian & ; East European Psychology 35, no 1 (janvier 1997) : 18–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/rpo1061-0405350118.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

Zaporozhets, A. V. « Perception, Movement, and Action ». Journal of Russian & ; East European Psychology 40, no 4 (juillet 2002) : 53–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/rpo1061-0405400453.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

Lebon, Florent, Aymeric Guillot, Christian Collet et Charalambos Papaxanthis. « Perception, Observation et Action ». Movement & ; Sport Sciences 89, no 3 (2015) : 43. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/sm.089.0043.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

VALLÉE, R. « PERCEPTION, DECISION AND ACTION ». Journal of Biological Systems 02, no 01 (mars 1994) : 43–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s0218339094000052.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
We consider a system S which is able to perceive itself and its environment E, decide and act. In S we have the observational chain, implying an “observation operator”, followed by the decisional chain with a decision operator, but these two chains can be replaced by one pragmatical chain with its “pragmatical operator”. The pragmatical chain is in contact with the effectors which modify directly S and influence E, creating a feed-back loop. The observational chain, considered alone, is concerned with the epistemological images S may have of the co-evolution of itself and its environment, indiscernibility, resolving power as well as other aspects of its subjectivity such as those generated by the inverse transfer of properties of S to possible co-evolutions of S and E. If, more realistically, the pragmatical chain is considered, other concepts appear involving pragmatical “subjective inverse transfer” and indiscernability. If the feed-back loop is considered, the evolution of the universe constituted by S and E is involved. At this meta-level some conceptual simplifications occur, the evolution of U appears as a fixed point of an operator taking into account the observational and decisional properties of system S, which may be a living being, and so introducing the outlines of an “epistemo-praxiology”.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

De-Wit, Lee. « Review : Action in Perception ». Perception 35, no 2 (février 2006) : 285–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p3502rvw.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Prinz, Wolfgang. « Perception and Action Planning ». European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9, no 2 (juin 1997) : 129–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713752551.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Matthen, Mohan. « Review : Action in Perception ». Mind 115, no 460 (1 octobre 2006) : 1160–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/mind/fzl1160.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Lebon, Florent, Aymeric Guillot, Christian Collet et Charalambos Papaxanthis. « Perception, Observation et Action ». Movement & ; Sport Sciences - Science & ; Motricité, no 89 (2015) : 43–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1051/sm/2015002.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Chan, D., M. Peterson, M. Barense et J. Pratt. « Action Influences Object Perception ». Journal of Vision 13, no 9 (25 juillet 2013) : 1007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/13.9.1007.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

Voorhorst, F., D. Meijer, C. Overbeeke et G. Smets. « Depth perception in laparoscopy through perception-action coupling ». Minimally Invasive Therapy & ; Allied Technologies 7, no 4 (janvier 1998) : 325–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/13645709809152876.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Hommel, Bernhard, Jochen Müsseler, Gisa Aschersleben et Wolfgang Prinz. « The Theory of Event Coding (TEC) : A framework for perception and action planning ». Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, no 5 (octobre 2001) : 849–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01000103.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Traditional approaches to human information processing tend to deal with perception and action planning in isolation, so that an adequate account of the perception-action interface is still missing. On the perceptual side, the dominant cognitive view largely underestimates, and thus fails to account for, the impact of action-related processes on both the processing of perceptual information and on perceptual learning. On the action side, most approaches conceive of action planning as a mere continuation of stimulus processing, thus failing to account for the goal-directedness of even the simplest reaction in an experimental task. We propose a new framework for a more adequate theoretical treatment of perception and action planning, in which perceptual contents and action plans are coded in a common representational medium by feature codes with distal reference. Perceived events (perceptions) and to-be-produced events (actions) are equally represented by integrated, task-tuned networks of feature codes – cognitive structures we call event codes. We give an overview of evidence from a wide variety of empirical domains, such as spatial stimulus-response compatibility, sensorimotor synchronization, and ideomotor action, showing that our main assumptions are well supported by the data.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Bortoletto, Marta, Jason B. Mattingley et Ross Cunnington. « Action intentions modulate visual processing during action perception ». Neuropsychologia 49, no 7 (juin 2011) : 2097–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.004.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

Westwood, David A., et Melvyn A. Goodale. « Perception and action planning : Getting it together ». Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, no 5 (octobre 2001) : 907–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01490108.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Hommel et al. propose that high-level perception and action planning share a common representational domain, which facilitates the control of intentional actions. On the surface, this point of view appears quite different from an alternative account that suggests that “action” and “perception” are functionally and neurologically dissociable processes. But it is difficult to reconcile these apparently different perspectives, because Hommel et al. do not clearly specify what they mean by “perception” and “action planning.” With respect to the visual control of action, a distinction must be made between conscious visual perception and unconscious visuomotor processing. Hommel et al. must also distinguish between the what and how aspects of action planning, that is, planning what to do versus planning how to do it.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Matsushima, Elton, Paula Chiaretti, Daniel Kreling, Murilo Lima, José Aparecido Da Silva et Nilton Ribeiro-Filho. « Perception-action interaction and bissection ». Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto) 14, no 27 (avril 2004) : 83–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s0103-863x2004000100011.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
Several studies using visually directed actions as indicators of perceived distance showed that people could accurately walk toward targets far up to 22m. Those results, summed up to those related to perceptual measures of perceived distance, showed that those responses were controlled by a single internal variable, namely visually perceived location. In the present study, we compared performance in bisection tasks, performed by open-loop walking or by perceptual matching. Observers (N=20) walked toward or adjust a pointer to the mean point of an egocentric distance (5, 10 or 15m), under binocular viewing. Results indicated accuracy on both responses, with no reliable differences between them, supporting the hypothesis of a single internal variable controlling action and perception. This invariant may be determined by a weighted set of sources of information.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

Zelaznik, Howard N., Herbert Heuer et Andries F. Sanders. « Perspectives on Perception and Action ». American Journal of Psychology 102, no 3 (1989) : 424. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1423061.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Abrahamse, Elger L., Luis Jiménez, Willem B. Verwey et Benjamin A. Clegg. « Representing serial action and perception ». Psychonomic Bulletin & ; Review 17, no 5 (octobre 2010) : 603–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pbr.17.5.603.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Zhang, Ruohan, Jake Whritner, Zhuode Liu, Luxin Zhang, Karl Muller, Mary Hayhoe et Dana Ballard. « Modelling complex perception-action choices ». Journal of Vision 18, no 10 (1 septembre 2018) : 533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/18.10.533.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Repp, Bruno H., et Günther Knoblich. « Action Can Affect Auditory Perception ». Psychological Science 18, no 1 (janvier 2007) : 6–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01839.x.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Gottlieb, J. « NEUROSCIENCE : Action, Illusion, and Perception ». Science 303, no 5656 (16 janvier 2004) : 317–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1093922.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Hommel, Bernhard, Bianca Pösse et Florian Waszak. « Contextualization in Perception and Action ». Psychologica Belgica 40, no 4 (1 janvier 2000) : 227. http://dx.doi.org/10.5334/pb.964.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Laasik, Kristjan. « Action and Variation in Perception ». European Journal of Philosophy 25, no 4 (22 février 2017) : 1364–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ejop.12216.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

Antonisse, Jim, et Harry Wechsler. « Integrating perception, action and learning ». ACM SIGART Bulletin 2, no 4 (juillet 1991) : 25–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/122344.122347.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

van der Kamp, John, et Geert Savelsbergh. « Action and perception in infancy ». Infant Behavior and Development 23, no 3-4 (mars 2000) : 237–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(01)00071-6.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Bengson, John. « Practical Perception and Intelligent Action ». Philosophical Issues 26, no 1 (octobre 2016) : 25–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/phis.12081.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Smeets, Jeroen B. J., et Eli Brenner. « Perception and Action Are Inseparable ». Ecological Psychology 13, no 2 (avril 2001) : 163–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1302_8.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Peacocke, Christopher. « Perception, Biology, Action, and Knowledge ». Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 88, no 2 (mars 2014) : 477–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/phpr.12092.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Bremmer, Frank. « The Perception of Inferred Action ». Neuron 31, no 1 (juillet 2001) : 6–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00351-8.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Armstrong, D. M. « Perception-consciousness and action-consciousness ? » Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18, no 2 (juin 1995) : 247–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0003819x.

Texte intégral
Résumé :
AbstractBlock's distinction between phenomenal and access consciousness is accepted, and it is agreed that one may be found without the other, but his account of the distinction is challenged. Phenomenal consciousness is perceptual consciousness, and it is a matter of gaining information of a detailed, nonverbal sort about the subject's body and environment. Access consciousness is good, old-fashioned introspection.
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Destro, M. F., P. Avanzini, A. Pascarella, L. Cattaneo et G. Rizzolatti. « Action perception : Top-down effects ». International Journal of Psychophysiology 85, no 3 (septembre 2012) : 370–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.023.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Vastano, Roberta, Alberto Inuggi, Claudia D. Vargas, Gabriel Baud-Bovy, Marco Jacono et Thierry Pozzo. « Tactile perception during action observation ». Experimental Brain Research 234, no 9 (9 mai 2016) : 2585–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4663-z.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Donnarumma, Francesco, Marcello Costantini, Ettore Ambrosini, Karl Friston et Giovanni Pezzulo. « Action perception as hypothesis testing ». Cortex 89 (avril 2017) : 45–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.016.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Zimmermann, M., I. Toni et F. P. de Lange. « Body Posture Modulates Action Perception ». Journal of Neuroscience 33, no 14 (3 avril 2013) : 5930–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5570-12.2013.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Enns, James T. « Section 1 : Perception et action. » Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 55, no 2 (2001) : 85–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0088097.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Enns, James T. « Section 1 : Perception and action. » Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 55, no 2 (2001) : 83–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0088098.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Carey, D. P., H. C. Dijkerman et A. D. Milner. « Perception and Action in Depth ». Consciousness and Cognition 7, no 3 (septembre 1998) : 438–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1998.0366.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Storozhuk, Anna. « Perception : Mirror-Image or Action ? » Journal for General Philosophy of Science 38, no 2 (novembre 2007) : 369–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10838-007-9044-7.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Fogel, Alan. « Co-regulation, perception and action ». Human Movement Science 11, no 4 (août 1992) : 505–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)90031-6.

Texte intégral
Styles APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Nous offrons des réductions sur tous les plans premium pour les auteurs dont les œuvres sont incluses dans des sélections littéraires thématiques. Contactez-nous pour obtenir un code promo unique!

Vers la bibliographie