Artículos de revistas sobre el tema "Unilateral brain damage"

Siga este enlace para ver otros tipos de publicaciones sobre el tema: Unilateral brain damage.

Crea una cita precisa en los estilos APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard y otros

Elija tipo de fuente:

Consulte los 50 mejores artículos de revistas para su investigación sobre el tema "Unilateral brain damage".

Junto a cada fuente en la lista de referencias hay un botón "Agregar a la bibliografía". Pulsa este botón, y generaremos automáticamente la referencia bibliográfica para la obra elegida en el estilo de cita que necesites: APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

También puede descargar el texto completo de la publicación académica en formato pdf y leer en línea su resumen siempre que esté disponible en los metadatos.

Explore artículos de revistas sobre una amplia variedad de disciplinas y organice su bibliografía correctamente.

1

Winstein, C. "Motor learning after unilateral brain damage". Neuropsychologia 37, n.º 8 (1 de julio de 1999): 975–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00145-6.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Schapiro, Anna C., James L. McClelland, Stephen R. Welbourne, Timothy T. Rogers y Matthew A. Lambon Ralph. "Why Bilateral Damage Is Worse than Unilateral Damage to the Brain". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25, n.º 12 (diciembre de 2013): 2107–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00441.

Texto completo
Resumen
Human and animal lesion studies have shown that behavior can be catastrophically impaired after bilateral lesions but that unilateral damage often produces little or no effect, even controlling for lesion extent. This pattern is found across many different sensory, motor, and memory domains. Despite these findings, there has been no systematic, computational explanation. We found that the same striking difference between unilateral and bilateral damage emerged in a distributed, recurrent attractor neural network. The difference persists in simple feedforward networks, where it can be understood in explicit quantitative terms. In essence, damage both distorts and reduces the magnitude of relevant activity in each hemisphere. Unilateral damage reduces the relative magnitude of the contribution to performance of the damaged side, allowing the intact side to dominate performance. In contrast, balanced bilateral damage distorts representations on both sides, which contribute equally, resulting in degraded performance. The model's ability to account for relevant patient data suggests that mechanisms similar to those in the model may operate in the brain.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Doricchi, Fabrizio, Gaspare Galati, Lara DeLuca, Daniele Nico y Francesca D’Olimpio. "Horizontal space misrepresentation in unilateral brain damage". Neuropsychologia 40, n.º 8 (enero de 2002): 1107–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00010-6.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Doricchi, Fabrizio, Alessandra Onida y Paola Guariglia. "Horizontal space misrepresentation in unilateral brain damage". Neuropsychologia 40, n.º 8 (enero de 2002): 1118–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00011-8.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Soffer, Dov, Eldad Melamed, Yaakov Assaf y Shamay Cotev. "Hemispheric brain damage in unilateral status epilepticus". Annals of Neurology 20, n.º 6 (diciembre de 1986): 737–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ana.410200616.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Ludwig, Casimir J. H., Stephen H. Butler, Stephanie Rossit, Monika Harvey y Iain D. Gilchrist. "Modelling contralesional movement slowing after unilateral brain damage". Neuroscience Letters 452, n.º 1 (marzo de 2009): 1–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.01.033.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Kucharska-Pietura, K., B. Loza y G. Kopacz. "Emotional control in schizophrenia and unilateral brain damage". European Neuropsychopharmacology 11 (enero de 2001): S284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0924-977x(01)80368-0.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Noetzel, Michael J. "Theophylline Neurotoxicity Resulting in Significant Unilateral Brain-damage". Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 27, n.º 2 (12 de noviembre de 2008): 242–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1985.tb03775.x.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Gandour, J., S. Ponglorpisit, F. Khunadorn, S. Dechongkit, P. Boongird y R. Boonklam. "Stop voicing in thai after unilateral brain damage". Aphasiology 6, n.º 6 (noviembre de 1992): 535–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687039208249490.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Gandour, Jack, Siripong Potisuk, Suvit Ponglorpisit, Sumalee Dechongkit, Fuangfa Khunadorn y Prasert Boongird. "Tonal Coarticulation in Thai after Unilateral Brain Damage". Brain and Language 52, n.º 3 (marzo de 1996): 505–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0027.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Zaidel, Dahlia W. "Neuronal connectivity, regional differentiation, and brain damage in humans". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, n.º 5 (octubre de 1999): 854–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99502197.

Texto completo
Resumen
When circumscribed brain regions are damaged in humans, highly specific impairments in language, memory, problem solving, and cognition are observed. Neurosurgery such as “split brain” or hemispherectomy, for example, has shown that encompassing regions, the left and right cerebral hemispheres, each control human behavior in unique ways. Observations stretching over 100 years of patients with unilateral focal brain damage have revealed, without the theoretical benefits of “cognitive neuroscience” or “cognitive psychology,” that human behavior is indeed controlled by the brain and its neurons.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Blanke, Olaf, Theodor Landis, Christophe Mermoud, Laurent Spinelli y Avinoam B. Safran. "Direction-selective motion blindness after unilateral posterior brain damage". European Journal of Neuroscience 18, n.º 3 (agosto de 2003): 709–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02771.x.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Borod, Joan C., Ronald L. Bloom, Adam M. Brickman, Luba Nakhutina y Elizabeth A. Curko. "Emotional Processing Deficits in Individuals With Unilateral Brain Damage". Applied Neuropsychology 9, n.º 1 (marzo de 2002): 23–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an0901_4.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Sundet, Kjetil. "Sex differences in cognitive impairment following unilateral brain damage". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 8, n.º 1 (enero de 1986): 51–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688638608401296.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

HERMSDORFER, J., S. ULRICH, C. MARQUARDT, G. GOLDENBERG y N. MAI. "Prehension With the Ipsilesional Hand After Unilateral Brain Damage". Cortex 35, n.º 2 (1999): 139–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70791-3.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

Caggiano, Pietro, Nicoletta Beschin y Gianna Cocchini. "Personal Neglect Following Unilateral Right and Left Brain Damage". Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 (agosto de 2014): 164–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.04.403.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

Gandour, J., S. Dechongkit, S. Ponglorpisit, F. Khunadorn y P. Boongird. "Intraword Timing Relations in Thai after Unilateral Brain Damage". Brain and Language 45, n.º 2 (agosto de 1993): 160–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brln.1993.1041.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

GANDOUR, J. "Lexical tones in Thai after unilateral brain damage*1". Brain and Language 43, n.º 2 (agosto de 1992): 275–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0093-934x(92)90131-w.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Buchanan, Tony W., Daniel Tranel y Ralph Adolphs. "Memories for emotional autobiographical events following unilateral damage to medial temporal lobe". Brain 129, n.º 1 (16 de noviembre de 2005): 115–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh672.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Tompkins, Connie A., Richard Boada y Kathrine McGarry. "The Access and Processing of Familiar Idioms by Brain-Damaged and Normally Aging Adults". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 35, n.º 3 (junio de 1992): 626–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3503.626.

Texto completo
Resumen
Idiom interpretation tasks are routinely used in the clinical evaluation of adults with brain damage, and idiom processing has received increasing attention in the psycholinguistic literature. Clinical evidence suggests that adults with unilateral right-hemisphere damage (RHD) are insensitive to nonliteral meanings conveyed by idiomatic expressions and other figurative forms. However, this portrayal is derived from their terminal responses to tasks that reflect multiple aspects of mental operations (off-line measures), obscuring the source of poor performance. This study used an on-line word-monitoring task to assess RHD, left-hemisphere-damaged, and normally aging adults' implicit knowledge of familiar idiomatic expressions. Brain-damaged subjects performed similarly to normal controls on this task, even though the clinical subjects fared poorly by comparison on an off-line idiom definition measure. These results suggest that adults with unilateral brain damage can activate and retrieve familiar idiomatic forms, and that their idiom-interpretation deficits most likely reflect impairment at some later stage of information processing. Further, error analysis of idiom-definition performance did not support the customary characterization of RHD adults as excessively literal responders The paper discusses clinical implications of the nature and use of idiom interpretation tasks.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Cristinzio, C., K. N'Diaye, M. Seeck, P. Vuilleumier y D. Sander. "Integration of gaze direction and facial expression in patients with unilateral amygdala damage". Brain 133, n.º 1 (14 de octubre de 2009): 248–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp255.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

TARTAGLIONE, A., M. L. INGLESE, F. BANDINI, L. SPADAVECCHIA, K. HAMSHER y E. FAVALE. "HEMISPHERE ASYMMETRY IN DECISION MAKING ABILITIES: AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY IN UNILATERAL BRAIN DAMAGE". Brain 114, n.º 3 (1 de junio de 1991): 1441–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.3.1441.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

Trauner, D. A. "Behavioural profiles of children and adolescents after pre- or perinatal unilateral brain damage". Brain 124, n.º 5 (1 de mayo de 2001): 995–1002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.5.995.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Larrabee, Glenn J. "Another look at viq-piq scores and unilateral brain damage". International Journal of Neuroscience 29, n.º 1-2 (enero de 1986): 141–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207458608985644.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Kempler, Daniel, Diana VanLancker, Virginia Marchman y Elizabeth Bates. "Idiom comprehension in children and adults with unilateral brain damage". Developmental Neuropsychology 15, n.º 3 (enero de 1999): 327–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565649909540753.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

Grabowska, Anna, Anna Nowicka, Olga Szymańska y Iwona Szatkowska. "Subjective contour illusion: sex-related effect of unilateral brain damage". Neuroreport 12, n.º 10 (julio de 2001): 2289–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200107200-00048.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Lukauskiene, R., R. Ruseckaite y B. Mickiene. "624 Object recognition changes in patients with unilateral brain damage". International Journal of Psychophysiology 30, n.º 1-2 (septiembre de 1998): 237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-8760(98)90623-7.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

Trauner, Doris A. "Hemispatial neglect in young children with early unilateral brain damage". Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 45, n.º 3 (13 de febrero de 2007): 160–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2003.tb00924.x.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Borod, J. C., E. Koff, M. P. Lorch y M. Nicholas. "Channels of Emotional Expression in Patients With Unilateral Brain Damage". Archives of Neurology 42, n.º 4 (1 de abril de 1985): 345–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1985.04060040055011.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Tartaglione, Antonio, Giovanni Bino, Marcello Manzino, Luciano Spadavecchia y Emilio Favale. "Simple reaction-time changes in patients with unilateral brain damage". Neuropsychologia 24, n.º 5 (enero de 1986): 649–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(86)90004-7.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Klepousniotou, Ekaterini y Shari R. Baum. "Unilateral brain damage effects on processing homonymous and polysemous words". Brain and Language 93, n.º 3 (junio de 2005): 308–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.011.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

KUCHARSKA-PIETURA, KATARZYNA y ANTHONY S. DAVID. "The perception of emotional chimeric faces in patients with depression, mania and unilateral brain damage". Psychological Medicine 33, n.º 4 (mayo de 2003): 739–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291702007316.

Texto completo
Resumen
Background. Judgements made on chimeric faces elicit reliably a perceptual bias to the left hemispace, presumed to be due to right hemisphere dominance for emotional processes. Major depressive illness has been shown to attenuate this bias. The aim of this work was to examine lateral perceptual bias in bipolar I and II patients in a hypomanic state and unipolar depressed patients and those with unilateral hemisphere damage following stroke.Method. Sixty patients with DSM-IV affective disorder (30 bipolar I or II, currently hypomanic, 30 unipolar depressives), 30 right brain-damaged patients, 30 left brain-damaged patients and 30 healthy controls were given the Happy–Sad Chimeric Faces Test.Results. Right hemisphere damaged and unipolar depressed patients both showed a significantly reduced left hemispatial bias (LHB) compared to controls, bipolars and left brain-damaged patients. No significant difference in mean LHB between controls and both hypomanics and left brain-damaged patients was found. There was no significant association between LHB and clinical variables.Conclusions. The results suggest a physiological distinction between bipolar and unipolar depression. The significantly diminished left hemifacial bias in depressed patients suggests right hemisphere dysfunction.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Oliveri, M., P. M. Rossini, R. Traversa, P. Cicinelli, M. M. Filippi, P. Pasqualetti, F. Tomaiuolo y C. Caltagirone. "Left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces contralesional extinction in patients with unilateral right brain damage". Brain 122, n.º 9 (septiembre de 1999): 1731–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.9.1731.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Walker, Robin y Jason B. Mattingley. "Pathological completion: The blind leading the mind?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, n.º 6 (diciembre de 1998): 778–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x98561754.

Texto completo
Resumen
The taxonomy proposed by Pessoa et al. should be extended to include “pathological” completion phenomena in patients with unilateral brain damage. Patients with visual field defects (hemianopias) may “complete” whole figures, while patients with parietal lobe damage may “complete” partial figures. We argue that the former may be consistent with the brain “filling-in” information, and the latter may be consistent with the brain ignoring the absence of information.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

Winstein, C. J. y S. Onla-or. "Effects of Unilateral Brain Damage on the Control of Aiming Movements". Neurology Report 20, n.º 4 (1996): 21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01253086-199620040-00019.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Kucharska-Pietura, K. y B. Loza. "The assessment of lateral preference in schizophrenia and unilateral brain damage". European Neuropsychopharmacology 11 (enero de 2001): S284—S285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0924-977x(01)80369-2.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Hermsdörfer, J. y G. Goldenberg. "Ipsilesional deficits during fast diadochokinetic hand movements following unilateral brain damage". Neuropsychologia 40, n.º 12 (enero de 2002): 2100–2115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00048-9.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Kucharska-Pietura, Katarzyna, Mary L. Phillips, Wojciech Gernand y Anthony S. David. "Perception of emotions from faces and voices following unilateral brain damage". Neuropsychologia 41, n.º 8 (enero de 2003): 1082–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00294-4.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Barrett, Anna M. "Treatment of Unilateral Neglect in Patients With Right Hemisphere Brain Damage". Perspectives on Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language Disorders 10, n.º 4 (diciembre de 2000): 18–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/nnsld10.4.18.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Gandour, J., S. Ponglorpisit, S. Dechongkit, F. Khunadorn, P. Boongird y S. Potisuk. "Anticipatory Tonal Coarticulation in Thai Noun Compounds after Unilateral Brain Damage". Brain and Language 45, n.º 1 (julio de 1993): 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brln.1993.1030.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Gandour, Jack, Suvit Ponglorpisit, Siripong Potisuk, Fuangfa Khunadorn, Prasert Boongird y Sumalee Dechongkit. "Interaction between Tone and Intonation in Thai after Unilateral Brain Damage". Brain and Language 58, n.º 1 (junio de 1997): 174–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1768.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Mosidze, Vakhtang M., Rusudan A. Mkheidze y Malhas A. Makashvili. "Disorders of visuo-spatial attention in patients with unilateral brain damage". Behavioural Brain Research 65, n.º 1 (noviembre de 1994): 121–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)90081-7.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Hart, Emerson y Michelle Woodbury. "Visual Search Strategy and Balance in Children After Unilateral Brain Damage". Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 104, n.º 3 (marzo de 2023): e64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2022.12.186.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Braun, C. M. J., I. Montour-Proulx, S. Daigneault, I. Rouleau, S. Kuehn, M. Piskopos, G. Desmarais, F. Lussier y C. Rainville. "Prevalence, and Intellectual Outcome of Unilateral Focal Cortical Brain Damage as a Function of Age, Sex and Aetiology". Behavioural Neurology 13, n.º 3-4 (2002): 105–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2002/634764.

Texto completo
Resumen
Neurologists and neuropsychologists are aware that aging men are more at risk than women for brain damage, principally because of the well known male-predominant risk for cardiovascular disease and related cerebrovascular accidents. However, a disproportion in prevalence of brain damage between the sexes in childhood may be less suspected. Furthermore, sex-specific risk for other aetiologies of brain damage may be little known, whether in the pediatric or adult populations. Proposals of a sex difference in cognitive recovery from brain damage have also been controversial. Six hundred and thirty five “consecutive” cases with cortical focal lesions including cases of all ages and both sexes were reviewed. Aetiology of the lesion was determined for each case as was postlesion IQ. Risk was highly male prevalent in all age groups, with a predominance of cardiovascular aetiology explaining much of the adult male prevalence. However, several other aetiological categories were significantly male prevalent in juveniles (mitotic, traumatic, dysplasic) and adults (mitotic, traumatic). There was no sex difference in outcome (i.e., postlesion IQ) of these cortical brain lesions for the cohort as a whole, after statistical removal of the influence of lesion extent, aetiology and presence of epilepsy. Mechanisms potentially responsible for sex differences in prevalence, aetiology of brain damage, and recovery, are reviewed and discussed.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Sakaguchi, Yukitoshi y Yoshio Sakurai. "Paradoxical Enhancement of Spatial Learning Induced by Right Hippocampal Lesion in Rats". Symmetry 13, n.º 11 (10 de noviembre de 2021): 2138. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/sym13112138.

Texto completo
Resumen
The left–right hemispheric differences in some brain functions are well known in humans. Among them, savant syndrome has unique features, such as exceptional abilities in vision, memory, computation, and music, despite brain abnormalities. In cases of acquired savant and transient savant, brain damage or inhibition is often seen in the left hemisphere, suggesting a link between left hemispheric dysfunction and these talents. On the other hand, some functional left–right differences have been reported in rodent brains, and therefore, unilateral damage in rodents may also result in savant-like enhancements. In the present study, we examined the effects of hippocampal damage on spatial learning in rats with left, right, or bilateral hippocampal lesion. The results showed that learning performance was impaired in the bilateral lesion group, and there was no significant difference in the left lesion group, while performance was enhanced in the right lesion group. These results suggest that damage to the right hippocampus in rats may lead to savant-like enhancement in learning and memory. The construction of the savant model through these results will contribute to the neuroscientific elucidation of the paradoxical phenomenon observed in savants, that some abilities are enhanced despite their brain dysfunction.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Borod, J. C., E. Koff, M. P. Lorch, M. Nicholas y J. Welkowitz. "Emotional and non-emotional facial behaviour in patients with unilateral brain damage." Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 51, n.º 6 (1 de junio de 1988): 826–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.51.6.826.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Delis, Dean C., Mark G. Kiefner y Alan J. Fridlund. "Visuospatial dysfunction following unilateral brain damage: Dissociations in hierarchical and hemispatial analysis". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 10, n.º 4 (agosto de 1988): 421–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688638808408250.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Nass, Ruth y Diane Koch. "Temperament differences in toddlers with early unilateral right‐ and left‐brain damage". Developmental Neuropsychology 3, n.º 2 (enero de 1987): 93–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565648709540367.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Salazar-López, E., B. J. Schwaiger y J. Hermsdörfer. "Lesion correlates of impairments in actual tool use following unilateral brain damage". Neuropsychologia 84 (abril de 2016): 167–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.02.007.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Mihaltchev, P., P. S. Archambault, A. G. Feldman y M. F. Levin. "Control of double-joint arm posture in adults with unilateral brain damage". Experimental Brain Research 163, n.º 4 (3 de febrero de 2005): 468–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2202-9.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Ofrecemos descuentos en todos los planes premium para autores cuyas obras están incluidas en selecciones literarias temáticas. ¡Contáctenos para obtener un código promocional único!

Pasar a la bibliografía