Siga este enlace para ver otros tipos de publicaciones sobre el tema: Typological variation.

Artículos de revistas sobre el tema "Typological variation"

Crea una cita precisa en los estilos APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard y otros

Elija tipo de fuente:

Consulte los 50 mejores artículos de revistas para su investigación sobre el tema "Typological variation".

Junto a cada fuente en la lista de referencias hay un botón "Agregar a la bibliografía". Pulsa este botón, y generaremos automáticamente la referencia bibliográfica para la obra elegida en el estilo de cita que necesites: APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

También puede descargar el texto completo de la publicación académica en formato pdf y leer en línea su resumen siempre que esté disponible en los metadatos.

Explore artículos de revistas sobre una amplia variedad de disciplinas y organice su bibliografía correctamente.

1

SARITAŞ, BURCU BAŞOĞLU y CANKUT SARITAŞ. "TYPOLOGICAL VARIATION IN RELATIVE CLAUSES". International Journal of Social Sciences and Management Review 05, n.º 03 (2022): 123–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.37602/ijssmr.2022.5311.

Texto completo
Resumen
According to Foley and Hall (2003), relative clauses are subordinate clauses that refer to the noun of the main clause, identifying it, or adding extra information. This research aims to illustrate how languages vary in the relativization strategies they utilize. It also explains the effects of relative clause structure on L2 acquisition and problems for ESL/EFL students. Now let me start with the characteristics of English Relative Clauses first and then gradually explain the other languages’ relative clauses.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Aloni, Maria y Marco Degano. "(Non-)specificity across languages: constancy, variation, v-variation". Semantics and Linguistic Theory 1 (29 de diciembre de 2022): 185. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/salt.v1i0.5337.

Texto completo
Resumen
Indefinites are known to give rise to different scopal (specific vs non-specific) and epistemic (known vs unknown) uses. Farkas & Brasoveanu (2020) explained these specificity distinctions in terms of stability vs. variability in value assignments of the variable introduced by the indefinite. Typological research (Haspelmath 1997) showed that indefinites have different functional distributions with respect to these uses. In this work, we present a formal framework where Farkas & Brasoveanu (2020)’s ideas are rigorously formalized. We develop a two-sorted team semantics which integrates both scope and epistemic effects. We apply the framework to explain typological variety of indefinites, their restricted distribution and licensing conditions, and some diachronic developments of indefinite forms.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Özçalışkan, Şeyda. "Typological variation in encoding the manner, path, and ground components of a metaphorical motion event". Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 2 (31 de diciembre de 2004): 73–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/arcl.2.03ozc.

Texto completo
Resumen
The paper compares two typologically distinct languages with regard to their lexicalization patterns in encoding metaphorical motion events: (1) verb-framed (V-language, represented by Turkish), in which the preferred pattern for framing motion events is the use of a path verb with an optional manner adjunct (e.g.,enter running), and (2) satellite-framed (S-language, represented by English), in which path is lexicalized in an element associated with the verb, leaving the verb free to encode manner (e.g.,run in). The paper focuses on typological differences in encoding the manner, path, and ground components of metaphorical motion events, using data from novels written originally in English or Turkish, and further extends the applicability of the typological dichotomy to the metaphorical uses of the lexicon.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Whaley, Lindsay J., Artemis Alexiadou y T. Alan Hall. "Studies on Universal Grammar and Typological Variation". Language 75, n.º 1 (marzo de 1999): 204. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/417539.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Primus, Beatrice. "The typological and historical variation of punctuation systems". Constraints on Spelling Changes 10, n.º 2 (31 de diciembre de 2007): 103–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/wll.10.2.07pri.

Texto completo
Resumen
In the literature on punctuation we find a broad typological and historical distinction between prosodically and grammatically determined punctuation. The mainstream historical assumption is that the prosodic system changed into to a grammatical system in some languages. We will show that this view is confronted with serious empirical and conceptual problems. Our assumption is that the typological and historical variation at issue is motivated syntactically in all punctuation systems. The different punctuation systems are mainly distinguished by the comma, which is, therefore, the main topic of the present paper. The major use of the comma will be explained by four constraints, whose interaction may be congenially formulated in optimality-theoretic terms. The close relationship of the comma to prosody arises indirectly from the fact that syntactic structures are marked prosodically in many instances. The stylistic freedom of the comma, that is traditionally assumed for some languages and contexts of use, is a reflex of syntactic optionality.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Newmeyer, Frederick J. "Against a parameter-setting approach to typological variation". Linguistic Variation Yearbook 2004 4 (31 de diciembre de 2004): 181–234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/livy.4.06new.

Texto completo
Resumen
The dominant position among generative grammarians with respect to typological variation is that it should be captured by parameters, which are either directly tied to principles of Universal Grammar (UG) or to functional projections provided by UG. Parameter-setting approaches, however, have failed to live up to their promise. They should be replaced by a model in which language-particular rules take over the work of parameter settings and in which most typological variation follows from independently-needed principles of performance. In such a model, UG specifies the class of possible languages, but not the set of probable languages.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Szeto, Pui Yiu, Umberto Ansaldo y Stephen Matthews. "Typological variation across Mandarin dialects: An areal perspective with a quantitative approach". Linguistic Typology 22, n.º 2 (28 de agosto de 2018): 233–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2018-0009.

Texto completo
Resumen
AbstractThis study explores the range and diversity of the typological features of Mandarin, the largest dialect group within the Sinitic branch of the Sino-Tibetan family. Feeding the typological data of 42 Sinitic varieties into the phylogenetic program NeighborNet, we obtained network diagrams suggesting a north-south divide in the Mandarin dialect group, where dialects within the Amdo Sprachbund cluster at one end and those in the Far Southern area cluster at the other end, highlighting the impact of language contact on the typological profiles of various Mandarin dialects.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Miestamo, Matti. "A typological perspective on negation in Finnish dialects". Nordic Journal of Linguistics 34, n.º 2 (20 de septiembre de 2011): 83–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0332586511000126.

Texto completo
Resumen
This paper looks at negation in Finnish dialects from a typological perspective. The focus is on standard negation, i.e. the negation of declarative verbal main clauses. The dialectal variation that Finnish shows in its negative construction is examined in the light of current typological knowledge of the expression of negation. Developmental trends connected to the micro-typological variation are also discussed, Finnish dialects are compared with related and neighbouring languages, and relevant theoretical and methodological issues relating to the meeting point of typology and dialectology are addressed.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Ponti, Edoardo Maria, Helen O’Horan, Yevgeni Berzak, Ivan Vulić, Roi Reichart, Thierry Poibeau, Ekaterina Shutova y Anna Korhonen. "Modeling Language Variation and Universals: A Survey on Typological Linguistics for Natural Language Processing". Computational Linguistics 45, n.º 3 (septiembre de 2019): 559–601. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/coli_a_00357.

Texto completo
Resumen
Linguistic typology aims to capture structural and semantic variation across the world’s languages. A large-scale typology could provide excellent guidance for multilingual Natural Language Processing (NLP), particularly for languages that suffer from the lack of human labeled resources. We present an extensive literature survey on the use of typological information in the development of NLP techniques. Our survey demonstrates that to date, the use of information in existing typological databases has resulted in consistent but modest improvements in system performance. We show that this is due to both intrinsic limitations of databases (in terms of coverage and feature granularity) and under-utilization of the typological features included in them. We advocate for a new approach that adapts the broad and discrete nature of typological categories to the contextual and continuous nature of machine learning algorithms used in contemporary NLP. In particular, we suggest that such an approach could be facilitated by recent developments in data-driven induction of typological knowledge.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

BRESNAN, JOAN, ASHWINI DEO y DEVYANI SHARMA. "Typology in variation: a probabilistic approach to be and n't in the Survey of English Dialects". English Language and Linguistics 11, n.º 2 (julio de 2007): 301–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1360674307002274.

Texto completo
Resumen
Variation within grammars is a reflection of variation between grammars. Subject agreement and synthetic negation for the verb be show extraordinary local variation in the Survey of English Dialects (Orton et al., 1962–71). Extracting partial grammars of individuals, we confirm leveling patterns across person, number, and negation (Ihalainen, 1991; Cheshire, Edwards & Whittle, 1993; Cheshire, 1996). We find that individual variation bears striking structural resemblances to invariant dialect paradigms, and also reflects typologically observed markedness properties (Aissen, 1999). In the framework of Stochastic Optimality Theory (Boersma & Hayes, 2001), variable outputs of individual speakers are expected to be constrained by the same typological and markedness generalizations found crosslinguistically. The stochastic evaluation of candidate outputs in individual grammars reranks individual constraints by perturbing their ranking values, with the potential for stable variation between two near-identical rankings. The stochastic learning mechanism is sensitive to variable frequencies encountered in the linguistic environment, whether in geographical or social space. In addition to relating individual and group dialectal variation to typological variation (Kortmann, 1999; Anderwald, 2003), the findings suggest that an individual grammar is sensitively tuned to frequencies in the linguistic environment, leading to isolated loci of variability in the grammar rather than complete alternations of paradigms. A characteristic of linguistic variation that has emerged in distinct fields of enquiry is that variation within a single grammar bears a close resemblance to variation across grammars. Sociolinguistic studies, for instance, have long observed that ‘variation within the speech of a single speaker derives from the variation which exists between speakers’ (Bell, 1984: 151). In the present study, individual patterns of variation in subject–verb agreement with affirmative and negative be extracted from the Survey of English Dialects(SED, Orton et al., 1962–71) show striking structural resemblances to patterns of interdialectal, or categorical, variation.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Jeong-Shik Lee. "Speculations on Typological Variation from a Third Factor Perspective". Studies in Generative Grammar 22, n.º 1 (febrero de 2012): 77–112. http://dx.doi.org/10.15860/sigg.22.1.201202.77.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Park, Mihi y Rebecca Lurie Starr. "The acquisition of L3 variation among early bilinguals". Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 10, n.º 5 (8 de enero de 2019): 657–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/lab.17066.par.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract The present study investigates whether prior experience with formal study of an L2 influences L3 Korean learners’ Type 1 variation (i.e., use of obligatory forms) and Type 2 variation (i.e., variation between alternative acceptable variants). The patterns of variation in Korean argument realization of early bilingual learners (English-Chinese/Malay/Indonesian/Tamil) of L3 Korean were assessed in light of the distribution of variants present in classroom input, learners’ prior L2 learning experience and home language background, argument animacy and number, and familiarity of verb structure type. Our findings demonstrate that prior experience with a typologically-similar L2 facilitates acquisition of grammatical patterns as well as acquisition of native-like patterns of variation between grammatical forms that are constrained by a range of internal linguistic factors. Any L2 experience, regardless of typological proximity, is found to facilitate acquisition of internal linguistic constraints, but not acquisition of grammatical patterns.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Jaeger, Hanna. "Variation in Indonesian Sign Language: A typological and sociolinguistic analysis". Folia Linguistica 55, n.º 1 (4 de septiembre de 2020): 281–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/flin-2020-2055.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Mithun, Marianne. "Beyond the Core: Typological Variation in the Identification of Participants". International Journal of American Linguistics 71, n.º 4 (octubre de 2005): 445–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/501247.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

Croft, William y Keith T. Poole. "Inferring universals from grammatical variation: Multidimensional scaling for typological analysis". Theoretical Linguistics 34, n.º 1 (enero de 2008): 1–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/thli.2008.001.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

Sharma, Devyani. "Typological diversity in New Englishes". English World-Wide 30, n.º 2 (11 de junio de 2009): 170–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/eww.30.2.04sha.

Texto completo
Resumen
Recent research has aimed to integrate the investigation of vernacular universals in native English dialects with variation in postcolonial varieties of English and cross-linguistic typology (Chambers 2004; Kortmann 2004). This article assumes that any search for universals in bilingual varieties must include an assessment of the grammatical conditioning of features and a comparison with the relevant substrates. Comparing Indian English and Singapore English, I examine three proposed candidates for English universals (Kortmann and Szmrecsanyi 2004), all of which show some presence in the two varieties — past tense omission, over-extension of the progressive, and copula omission. Past tense omission is found to be genuinely similar in the two varieties and accounted for by typological parallels in the substrates, whereas progressive morphology use and copula omission are found to be divergent in the two varieties and accounted for by typological differences in the substrates. All three variable systems are explicable as substrate-superstrate interactions, tempering claims of universality in both distribution and explanation.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

Ostapovych, O. "Typological Structure of German Phraseology Outside Germany. Quantitative Parameters". Journal of Vasyl Stefanyk Precarpathian National University 3, n.º 4 (30 de diciembre de 2016): 33–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.15330/jpnu.3.4.33-41.

Texto completo
Resumen
The article deals with the modern theoretical concept in study of the variation of German phraseology abroad Germany. It is based on the synthesis of the theory of equal-righted pluricentrism with the new achievements of the cognitive linguistics. As a result the national state linguistic variant is considered as different from the regional, normatively non-codified and dialectal variation, a kind of cluster variant idiomatic thesaurus. The hypothesis of the structural isomorphy of the variant phraseology compared to the common German one has been empirically verified and vice versa - the hypothesis of the quantitative predominance in the Austrian phraseology of the structural model Adj+Sub under the Slavic linguistic influence has also been falsified
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

Manfredi, Stefano. "Pain Constructions in modern Arabic dialects: a typological overview". STUF - Language Typology and Universals 75, n.º 4 (1 de noviembre de 2022): 501–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/stuf-2022-1061.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract Pain Constructions (PCs) constitute a class of experiential constructions expressing situations that involve unpleasant physical experiences (e.g. headache, burning eyes, dizziness, etc.). Previous cross-linguistic comparisons have shown that, though languages do not have dedicated morphosyntactic structures for encoding pain, there are certain constructions that are more likely to express physical experiences. Based on original data elicited by means of a situational questionnaire, this paper aims at analyzing the semantic and syntactic properties of PCs in modern Arabic dialects and to make typological generalizations about their cross-dialect variation. Benefiting from insights from both linguistic typology and contact linguistics, the study eventually shows that, despite considerable lexicosemantic and morphosyntactic variation, PCs in Arabic can be reduced to two main syntactic types: locational and inverse constructions.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Olthof, Marieke. "Formal variation in incorporation: A typological study and a unified approach". Linguistics 58, n.º 1 (25 de febrero de 2020): 131–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ling-2019-0036.

Texto completo
Resumen
AbstractThis study investigates the formal variation in elements involved in incorporation structures. Although it has traditionally been assumed that only stems can be incorporated, several languages also show incorporation of formally more complex elements. Most theoretical approaches to incorporation are, however, limited to incorporated stems and the few that do include more complex forms consider the incorporation of simple and complex elements separate processes. The present study, by contrast, recognizes the many shared characteristics between incorporation structures with simple and complex elements and adopts the approach to incorporation advocated in Functional Discourse Grammar, which proposes a unified account of incorporated simple stems, derived stems, inflected words, phrases and clauses. In addition, the study hypothesizes that these forms constitute an implicational hierarchy, i. e., that more complex incorporated elements only occur in languages that also allow the incorporation of all simpler forms. Results from a typological study of incorporated elements in a variety sample of 30 incorporating languages show that all forms except clauses are indeed found incorporated and that the hypothesized implicational pattern holds. The study thus demonstrates that the incorporation of simple and complex elements is interrelated, supporting a unified treatment of incorporated elements of different degrees of complexity.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Lewandowski, Wojciech y Jaume Mateu. "Thinking for translating and intra-typological variation in satellite-framed languages". Review of Cognitive Linguistics 14, n.º 1 (27 de junio de 2016): 185–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/rcl.14.1.08lew.

Texto completo
Resumen
We analyze the expression of motion in translations of Tolkien’s The Hobbit into Polish and German within the framework of Talmy’s (1991, 2000) typology of macro-events and Slobin’s (1991, 1996) “Thinking for speaking” hypothesis. We show that although both languages pertain to the satellite-framed typological group, Polish provides less diversified Manner and Path descriptions than German, which exploits the satellite lexicalization pattern by far more productively. We relate these contrasts in the rhetorical style to the particular morpho-syntactic and semantic characteristics of the languages under discussion.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Jarema, Gonia, Gary Libben, Wolfgang Dressler y Eva Kehayia. "The Role of Typological Variation in the Processing of Interfixed Compounds". Brain and Language 81, n.º 1-3 (abril de 2002): 736–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2560.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Thwala, Nhlanhla. "Parameters of variation & complement licensing in Bantu". ZAS Papers in Linguistics 43 (1 de enero de 2006): 209–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.21248/zaspil.43.2006.292.

Texto completo
Resumen
In this paper I argue that the syntax of Eastern Bantu does not make reference to the notion 'syntactic object'. That is, there is no linguistic category of objects that is the target of syntactic rules in Eastern Bantu languages. Instead I propose that syntactic rules broadly distinguish complements and adjuncts as well as category type of complement or adjunct. I argue that Bantu languages are typologically special in that (a) the verb complement structure can be expanded by the valency increasing applicative suffix; and (b) that the class of adjuncts can be expanded through verb concord licensing. Because of these properties, Bantu languages have a much-expanded notion of 'complement' and 'adjunct'. Namely, complements consist of (a) inherent complements (subcategorised by the lexical verb), and (b) derived complements (licensed by the applicative suffix). Adjuncts consist of (a) non-subcategorised modifying constituents in the usual sense and (b) phrases that are licensed by verb concord (i.e. Topics in Bresnan and Mchombo (1987)). I propose that most the differences in the licensing of objects in Bantu are due to two causes: (a) the unusual split in the composition of complements and adjuncts and (b) a set of typological parameter settings.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

Anttila, Arto, Vivienne Fong, Štefan Beňuš y Jennifer Nycz. "Variation and opacity in Singapore English consonant clusters". Phonology 25, n.º 2 (agosto de 2008): 181–216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952675708001462.

Texto completo
Resumen
Singapore English consonant clusters undergo phonological processes that exhibit variation and opacity. Quantitative evidence shows that these patterns are genuine and systematic. Two main conclusions emerge. First, a small set of phonological constraints yields a typological structure (T-order) that captures the quantitative patterns, independently of specific assumptions about how the grammar represents variation. Second, the evidence is consistent with the hypothesis that phonological opacity has only one source: the interleaving of phonology and morphology.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Sansò, Andrea. "Variation in language use is different from variation in genes". Biological Evolution 3, n.º 1 (2 de agosto de 2021): 83–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/elt.00027.san.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract This commentary discusses some aspects of Haider’s model of grammar change that are problematic from the perspective of usage-based approaches to language change. These aspects include (i) the postulated equivalence between intentionality and teleology, (ii) the metaphorical nature of Darwinism when applied to other domains, and (iii) the nature of explanations of language change. With respect to (i), it is argued that equating intentionality with teleology disregards the fact that innovation in grammar is not unprincipled like in genes. With respect to (ii), the question is whether a comparison between as different concepts as human behaviors/brains and genes/populations can be considered as more than a metaphor (however powerful). Finally, with respect to (iii), a number of diachronic-typological studies are discussed that concur to suggest that variation in speakers’ verbal productions is largely adaptive, and therefore selection operates on a skewed pool of variants in which non-adaptive/dysfunctional variants are a minority (if any).
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Shepherd, Colin. "Typological variation in pre-modern settlement morphology in the Clashindarroch Forest, Aberdeenshire". Landscape History 33, n.º 2 (octubre de 2012): 49–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01433768.2012.739396.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

Guerrero, Lilián y Rebeca Gerardo-Tavira. "La posición de las cláusulas temporales con cuando". Anuario de Letras. Lingüística y Filología 9, n.º 1 (13 de enero de 2021): 7–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.19130/iifl.adel.2021.1.00281.

Texto completo
Resumen
Previous typological studies have shown that temporal clauses, unlike other adverbial clauses, can occur before or after the main clause, and this order variation has been observed across languages and within the same language. In the case of Spanish, some studies have found that temporal clauses tend to occur at the beginning of the clause. In this paper, we extend the assumptions of typological studies into the analysis of temporal clauses introduced by cuando ‘when’. Based in used data, we found that the initial position is preferred in oral data, while both positions are equally common in writing data. We examine some semantic, syntactic, and pragmatic motivations that, together, may explain this order variation: the semantic nature of cuando, sequential iconicity, length, and syntactic complexity, as well as pragmatic order.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Levinson, Stephen C. "Advancing our grasp of constrained variation in a crucial cognitive domain". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, n.º 5 (octubre de 2010): 391–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1000141x.

Texto completo
Resumen
AbstractJones's system of constraints promises interesting insights into the typology of kin term systems. Three problems arise: (1) the conflation of categories with algorithms that assign them threatens to weaken the typological predictions; (2) OT-type constraints have little psychological plausibility; (3) the conflation of kin-term systems and kinship systems may underplay the “utility function” character of real kinship in action.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

ÖZÇALIŞKAN, ŞEYDA. "Ways of crossing a spatial boundary in typologically distinct languages". Applied Psycholinguistics 36, n.º 2 (25 de julio de 2013): 485–508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716413000325.

Texto completo
Resumen
ABSTRACTExpression of spatial motion shows wide variation as well as patterned regularities across the world's languages (Talmy, 2000), and events involving the traversal of a spatial boundary impose the tightest typological constraints in the lexicalization of motion, providing a true test of cross-linguistic differences. Speakers of verb-framed languages are required by their language not to use manner verbs in marking the change of location across boundaries (Aske, 1989). Here we test the strength of the boundary-crossing constraint and ask how speakers convey motion events when the constraints imposed by the experimental task are at odds with the constraints imposed by their native language. We address this question by comparing adult speakers’ description of motion scenes that involve the traversal of a spatial boundary in two typologically distinct languages: English and Turkish. Using an experimental paradigm that imposes competing demands with the semantic structure of Turkish, we compare Turkish speakers’ description of boundary-crossing scenes to that of English speakers. We find strong cross-linguistic differences in speakers’ verb choice (manner vs. path) and event segmentation (one vs. many), suggesting that boundary-crossing constraint can serve as a reliable test to detect the typological class of a language.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Bochnak, M. Ryan, Vera Hohaus y Anne Mucha. "Variation in Tense and Aspect, and the Temporal Interpretation of Complement Clauses". Journal of Semantics 36, n.º 3 (23 de abril de 2019): 407–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jos/ffz008.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract In this paper, we investigate the temporal interpretation of propositional attitude complement clauses in four typologically unrelated languages: Washo (language isolate), Medumba (Niger-Congo), Hausa (Afro-Asiatic), and Samoan (Austronesian). Of these languages, Washo and Medumba are optional-tense languages, while Hausa and Samoan are tenseless. Just like in obligatory-tense languages, we observe variation among these languages when it comes to the availability of so-called simultaneous and backward-shifted readings of complement clauses. For our optional-tense languages, we argue that a Sequence of Tense parameter is active in these languages, just as in obligatory-tense languages. However, for completely tenseless clauses, we need something more. We argue that there is variation in the degree to which languages make recourse to res-movement, or a similar mechanism that manipulates LF structures to derive backward-shifted readings in tenseless complement clauses. We additionally appeal to cross-linguistic variation in the lexical semantics of perfective aspect to derive or block certain readings. The result is that the typological classification of a language as tensed, optionally tensed, or tenseless, does not alone determine the temporal interpretation possibilities for complement clauses. Rather, structural parameters of variation cross-cut these broad classes of languages to deliver the observed cross-linguistic picture.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Norvik, Miina, Yingqi Jing, Michael Dunn, Robert Forkel, Terhi Honkola, Gerson Klumpp, Richard Kowalik et al. "Uralic typology in the light of a new comprehensive dataset". Journal of Uralic Linguistics 1, n.º 1 (13 de junio de 2022): 4–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/jul.00002.nor.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract This paper presents the Uralic Areal Typology Online (UraTyp 1.0), a typological dataset of 35 Uralic languages and a total of 360 features, mainly covering the levels of morphology, syntax, and phonology. The features belong to two different datasets: 195 features’ definitions originate from the Grambank (GB) database, developed for comparison of world language typology, whereas 165 features (UT) have been designed specifically to describe the typological variation within the Uralic language family. We present a series of analyses of the dataset demonstrating its scope and possibilities. The complete data set correctly identifies the main Uralic subgroups in a Principal Components Analysis, whereas GB data alone is insufficiently granular to detect this family-internal structure. Similar analyses limited to various typological subdomains also give variable results. A model-based admixture analysis identifies four distinct areas of historical interaction: Saami, Finnic, the Volga area and Ob-Ugric.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Moriarty, Erin. "Variation in Indonesian Sign Language: A Typological and Sociolinguistic Analysis by Nick Palfreyman". Sign Language Studies 20, n.º 2 (2020): 355–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sls.2020.0006.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Poletto, Cecilia. "Contrastive linguistics and micro-variation". Languages in Contrast 12, n.º 1 (12 de enero de 2012): 47–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/lic.12.1.04pol.

Texto completo
Resumen
This article deals with a very general problem, namely the origin of the well-known distinction between dialectal and typological variation. It is argued that the fact that the possible grammatical choices are more restricted within a dialectal domain is not due to a supposed principled difference in the parameters that rule variation. Rather, they are a function of the originally unitary lexicon dialects share. If language variation is essentially located in the functional items, and they are derived from the same lexicon, then they will share some core properties that make dialectal variation so restricted. I propose that the fact that the lexicon is similar can give us clues about the internal structure of syntactically complex elements which are represented by a single word, like quantifiers, wh-items, modal verbs, etc. Within a homogenous domain, structural complexity correlates with a higher number of lexical roots: the higher the number of the lexical roots found, the more complex internal structure the functional item will display.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Sompolska-Rzechuła, Agnieszka. "Spatial variation in the quality of life in Poland". Wiadomości Statystyczne. The Polish Statistician 62, n.º 6 (28 de junio de 2017): 38–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.5604/01.3001.0014.0958.

Texto completo
Resumen
The subject of the article is to assess the similarity of voivodships in Poland in terms of the quality of life of inhabitants and to define typological classes of voivodships concerning the analysed phenomenon. The study was conducted by means of fuzzy classification — C-means. The choice of the method resulted from the nature of the researched phenomenon, which is difficult to identify unequivocally. Data from the CSO studies and the Social Diagnosis 2015 relating to 2014 were used in the research. The analysis proved that Polish voivodships are very different in terms of the quality of life.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Oomen, Marloes y Roland Pfau. "Signing not (or not): A typological perspective on standard negation in Sign Language of the Netherlands". Linguistic Typology 21, n.º 1 (1 de enero de 2017): 1–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2017-0001.

Texto completo
Resumen
AbstractThe expression of standard negation by means of manual and/or non-manual markers has been described for a considerable number of sign languages. Typological comparisons have revealed an intriguing dichotomy: while some sign languages require a manual negative element in negative clauses (manual-dominant sign languages), in others negation can be realized by a non-manual marker alone (in particular a headshake; non-manual-dominant sign languages). We are here adding data from Sign Language of the Netherlands (NGT) to the picture, and we demonstrate that NGT belongs to the latter group. Still, detailed comparison suggests that NGT patterns differently from other non-manual-dominant sign languages, thereby improving our understanding of the typological variation in this domain. A novel contribution of the present study is that it is based on naturalistic corpus data, showing more variation than often found in elicitation and grammaticality judgment studies of sign languages, but also presenting new problems of interpretation.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

De Clercq, Karen. "The internal syntax of Q-words". Linguistics in the Netherlands 34 (23 de noviembre de 2017): 31–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/avt.34.03dec.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract This paper aims at describing Q(uantity)-words, i.e. many/much and few/little, from a typological perspective, and presenting typological generalisations based on it. The typological sample provides support for a mass-count and positive-negative dimension in the domain of Q-words. Both dimensions also intersect. Along the negative dimension, it seems that languages fall into two groups: those having an opaque strategy for few/little and those having only an analytic strategy (not-much/many). Four patterns can be discerned on the basis of the sample, which are each exemplified by means of one language, i.e. English, Dutch, Wolof and Western Armenian. In addition, I make an attempt at developing a nanosyntactic analysis of the data, which aims to show how language variation in the domain of Q-words can be accounted for in terms of varying the size of lexically stored trees (Starke 2014). Finally, I show how one missing type of pattern is underivable on the basis of the analysis proposed.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Börjars, Kersti. "Swedish Double Determination in a European Typological Perspective". Nordic Journal of Linguistics 17, n.º 2 (diciembre de 1994): 219–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0332586500003024.

Texto completo
Resumen
This article discusses a phenomenon which has been referred to as ‘double determination’, ‘double definiteness’, or in the Scandinavian tradition ‘over-definiteness’. In this article, I define double determination and double definiteness, so that a distinction is made between the two terms. I use ‘double determination’ when both elements can function independently as semantic determiners. ‘Double definiteness’, on the other hand, is a form of agreement. A number of Swedish constructions are then examined which are plausible candidates for double determination. It is shown that only some of these are genuine cases of double determination, the others are more accurately described as double definiteness. In the cases of double determination, the determination is represented once as a syntactic element and once as a morphological element. The second part of this article focuses on this ‘morphological determiner’, referred to as def. The Swedish morphological determiner is compared with those of the other Scandinavian languages and the languages of the Balkans. It is shown that in languages which have an element like the Swedish def there is considerable variation in how this element functions within the language and in its status with respect to double determination and double definiteness.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Lewandowski, Wojciech y Şeyda Özçalışkan. "How language type influences patterns of motion expression in bilingual speakers". Second Language Research 37, n.º 1 (7 de octubre de 2019): 27–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0267658319877214.

Texto completo
Resumen
Expression of motion shows systematic inter-typological variability between language types, particularly with respect to manner and path components of motion: speakers of satellite-framed languages (S-language; e.g. German) frequently conflate manner and path into a single clause, while verb-framed language speakers (V-language; e.g. Spanish) typically express manner and path in separate clauses, a pattern that also becomes evident in bilinguals’ expression of motion events in each language type. However, less is known about intra-typological variability within each language type, particularly for the expression of motion events among bilingual speakers. In this study, we examine motion descriptions produced by two groups of bilinguals – with Polish as first language – learning a second language that belongs to the same (Polish–German) or a different language type (Polish–Spanish), in comparison to monolinguals in each language (German, Spanish, Polish). Our results, based on written descriptions of animated motion scenes, showed evidence for both inter-typological and intra-typological variation in the expression of motion, with greater attunement to first-language (L1) patterns in learning a language of the same type, and closer alignment to second-language (L2) patterns in learning a language that belongs to a different language type.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Delle Macchie, Sara, Simone Secchi y Gianfranco Cellai. "Acoustic Issues in Open Plan Offices: A Typological Analysis". Buildings 8, n.º 11 (14 de noviembre de 2018): 161. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/buildings8110161.

Texto completo
Resumen
This paper reports the acoustic issues of open plan office environments. According to a detailed research based on the scientific literature of the most suitable acoustic descriptors recommended for the open plan offices analysis, the main typological-functional configurations of these environments have been analyzed in order to identify six spatial typologies. The variation of acoustic parameters of these typologies has been evaluated by using a sound pyramid tracing software. The analysis procedure was calibrated in a case study of an office environment, where a measurement campaign was carried out. Results point out that the acoustic improvement of open plan offices can usually be achieved by introducing a sound absorbing false ceiling and dividing panels between working positions, but there are different issues depending on spatial geometries of the office. Better results are referred to office typologies characterized by reduced height and equal plan dimensions.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Suzuki, Seiichi. "Metrical Positions and their Linguistic Realisations in Old Germanic Metres: A Typological Overview". Studia Metrica et Poetica 1, n.º 2 (31 de diciembre de 2014): 9–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.12697/smp.2014.1.2.02.

Texto completo
Resumen
This paper provides a typological account of Old Germanic metre by investigating its parametric variations that largely determine the metrical identities of the Old English Beowulf, the Old Saxon Heliand, and Old Norse eddic poetry (composed in fornyrðislag, málaháttr, or ljóðaháttr). The primary parameters to be explored here are the principle of four metrical positions per verse and the differing ways in which these constituent positions are aligned to linguistic material. On the one hand, the four-position principle works with a maximal strictness in Beowulf, and to a slightly lesser extent in fornyrðislag, whereas it allows for a wider range of deviations in verse size in the Heliand and ljóðaháttr. In málaháttr, however, the principle in itself gives way to the five-position counterpart. On the other hand, the variation in the metrical– linguistic alignment in the three close cognate metres may be generalised by positing the common scale, Heliand > Beowulf > fornyrðislag, for the decreasing likelihood of resolution, the increasing likelihood of suspending resolution, and the decreasing size of the drop.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Powell, Jeffrey. "Genetic Variation in Insect Vectors: Death of Typology?" Insects 9, n.º 4 (11 de octubre de 2018): 139. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/insects9040139.

Texto completo
Resumen
The issue of typological versus population thinking in biology is briefly introduced and defined. It is then emphasized how population thinking is most relevant and useful in vector biology. Three points are made: (1) Vectors, as they exist in nature, are genetically very heterogeneous. (2) Four examples of how this is relevant in vector biology research are presented: Understanding variation in vector competence, GWAS, identifying the origin of new introductions of invasive species, and resistance to inbreeding. (3) The existence of high levels of vector genetic heterogeneity can lead to failure of some approaches to vector control, e.g., use of insecticides and release of sterile males (SIT). On the other hand, vector genetic heterogeneity can be harnessed in a vector control program based on selection for refractoriness.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Mengozzi, Alessandro y Emanuele Miola. "Paronomastic Infinitives in North-Eastern Neo-Aramaic: A Typological Approach". Aramaic Studies 16, n.º 2 (19 de noviembre de 2018): 270–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/17455227-01602006.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract In the present article we aim to describe the distribution and functions of preposed and postposed paronomastic infinitives in literary and spoken varieties of North-Eastern Neo-Aramaic (NENA). In the first part, the syntax and the function(s) of constructions involving a paronomastic infinitive will be described from a typological point of view. Syntactic and functional variation of NENA paronomastic infinitives largely corresponds to what is found in other Semitic languages, as well as in many languages belonging to other families. In the second part of the article we will address the rendering of Biblical Hebrew and Classical Syriac paronomastic infinitives in NENA Bible translations and offer a survey of various constructions found in spoken varieties and in the language of early Christian Neo-Aramaic poetry.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Lucas, Christopher y Mohammed Alluhaybi. "The typology of negation across varieties of Arabic". STUF - Language Typology and Universals 75, n.º 4 (1 de noviembre de 2022): 613–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/stuf-2022-1065.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract Drawing primarily on the data collated by Alluhaybi (2019. Negation in modern Arabic varieties from a typological point of view. London: SOAS University of London PhD thesis), this article first situates Arabic within the crosslinguistic typology of negative strategies put forward, among others, by Miestamo (2005. Standard negation: The negation of declarative verbal main clauses in a typological perspective. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter). It then surveys the main parameters of variation among different varieties of Arabic in the expression of standard negation, non-verbal negation, and negative imperatives, with a particular focus on the issue of the single versus bipartite expression of negation. The article finishes by looking at some recent debates concerning the diachronic evolution of the observed patterns.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Kampen, Jacqueline van. "Deriving verb-cluster variation in Dutch and German". Linguistics in the Netherlands 33 (14 de diciembre de 2016): 54–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/avt.33.05kam.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract The difference in West-Germanic V(erb)-clusters, right-branching (Dutch) and left-branching (German), follows from a difference in the acquisition of V-second. The decisive factor is a rightward selection of the <+Aux> verbs in Dutch main clauses. That decisive factor had already been acquired before any V-cluster appeared in the child’s speech. Longitudinal Dutch child data show that modals and aspectuals develop a rightward selection that carries over into the V-cluster. The German child data do not show such a development. Automatic phrasal formation by the acquisition procedure allows a V-cluster without assuming V-to-V-movement from an underlying structure. The general perspective is that (i) the acquisition procedure is a discovery procedure, and that (ii) typological effects are the outcome of early local string-determined licensing/selection.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Blaich, Charles F. y Humberto Barreto. "Typological thinking, statistical significance, and the methodological divergence of experimental psychology and economics". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, n.º 3 (junio de 2001): 405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01244148.

Texto completo
Resumen
While correctly describing the differences in current practices between experimental psychologists and economists, Hertwig and Ortmann do not provide a compelling explanation for these differences. Our explanation focuses on the fact that psychologists view the world as composed of categories and types. This discrete organizational scheme results in merely testing nulls and wider variation in observed practices in experimental psychology.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Mikulskas, Rolandas. "Aspectual variation in Lithuanian copular constructions". Lietuvių kalba, n.º 9 (18 de diciembre de 2015): 1–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/lk.2015.22627.

Texto completo
Resumen
In this article an attempt is made to pinpoint all possibilities of expressing aspectual meanings in Lithuanian copular constructions. The author departs from the tradition of distinguishing only perfective vs. imperfective aspect in Lithuanian. Instead, in testing various possibilities of expressing aspect in the constructions under discussion, the relevant meanings are chosen from a wider range of aspectual grams established in recent typological work on aspect.Until now the aspect of copular constructions had remained an understudied and underdescribed topic not only in Lithuanian. This is because the main concern of linguists has been with general problems of predication (or identification) in equative structures. Researchers (mainly of the formal persuasion) have based their accounts on copular sentences where the copula “be”, more specifically its present tense form, is taken to be the default case. Of course such ‘default’ copular constructions deliver an unproblematic aspectual meaning, that of imperfective state. Empirically, however, present tense (or zero) copulas are the default instance only in the case of identificational equatives, for identity statements typically assert general truths that are ‘timeless’ or ‘omnitemporal’. As this research has shown, the remaining types of copular constructions have a much wider potential for choosing the desired aspectual meaning. First of all, the functional−structural properties of these constructions and the contexts in which they are used provide much more space for aspect−tense variation in their copular verbs than in the default cases of identificational equatives. Secondly, the aspectual properties of the copular construction can be changed externally by introducing, at clausal level, specialized adverbials or periphrastic aspectual constructions, such as jau ‘already’, du kartus ‘twice’, buvo be-tampąs, bet… ‘he was about to become, but…’ etc.; in this case the aspectual interpretation of the copula is coerced in line with the overall aspectual profile of the construction. Thirdly, the range of aspectual meanings in the copular constructions becomes even wider if we take into account other copulas than “be”, viz. those of dynamic or locative origin. The typological research of recent decades in this field (Stassen 1997; Pustet 2003) has shown that cross-linguistically it is not unusual for copulas to be grammaticalized from different lexical sources. One may reasonably suppose that one of the motives for including new lexemes in the class of copulas was the need of expressing relevant aspectual meanings in the predication of identity, i.e. in various types of copular constructions. So languages tend to have, in addition to their main copula, a small number of verbs that can qualify as copulas and that mainly serve the needs of aspectual expression in their constructions. In Lithuanian these copular verbs are tapti, pasidaryti / darytis ‘become’, virsti ‘turn into’, likti ‘remain’. Depending on their tense and syntactic environment, ‘dynamic’ copulas can express ingressive, progressive or habitual aspect. The copula likti denotes continuation of the profiled situation, as does the prefixed form tebe-būti (tebe-(nebe-) being the usual marker of (phasal / aspectual) continuative meaning in Lithuanian).In the article due attention is also paid to the aspectual properties of the different types of copular constructions. In his earlier works the author has provided a new classification based on the idea of type instantiation, developed by Ronald Langacker (1991). In the second section of this paper these types are briefly passed in review, but the classification is updated and enriched with new discussion. The main idea behind the new classification is that all types of copular constructions are treated as instantiations of one archetype, that of the identity relation. What distinguishes these constructional types is the syntactic class of their predicative complements (adjective vs. noun) and additionally, in the case of predicative nominals, their referential properties. Before turning, in the fourth section, to a discussion of concrete aspectual meanings as expressed in different types of copular constructions, the author first makes a brief excursion, in the third section, to the typology of strategies for encoding predicatives as defined in Leon Stassen’s comprehensive typological study on intransitive predication (1997). This seemed necessary as one may suspect that variation in this domain of morphosyntactic encoding is to a great extent motivated by the need of expressing additional aspectual meanings in the constructions under discussion.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Krastev, Nikolai, Yoanna Tivcheva, Lina Malinova y Lazar Jelev. "Retroaortic left renal vein: a clinically significant vascular variation with suggestion of a practical typological scheme". Anatomy 16, n.º 1 (20 de abril de 2022): 41–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.2399/ana.21.9143157.

Texto completo
Resumen
The retroaortic left renal vein (RLRV) is a rare vascular variant with a typical position of the vein between the abdominal aorta and the vertebral column. Despite usually asymptomatic, RLRV might be associated with posterior nutcracker syndrome, other vascular pathologies and may cause major surgical complications. An existing but not expected RLRV might hamper aortic surgical dissection and to cause a life threatening bleeding. A case of a RLRV was observed during routine dissection of a formalin fixed 62-year-old female cadaver. The left kidney was found to be ectopic and located lower than usual between the level of L1 and L4 vertebra. The left renal vein was formed in the renal hilum at the level of L2/L3 intervertebral disc, and then passed obliquely downwards behind the abdominal aorta to join in the inferior vena cava at the level of upper border of L4 vertebra. The length of the RLRV was 7.5 cm. The main tributaries were the left suprarenal and left ovarian veins. Measuring the diameters of the renal vein showed slight dilation at its origin. Based on the literature review, the vascular variation reported here can be classified as Type II – RLRV draining at a lower than normal level of the inferior vena cava. An extended classification scheme of the left renal vein variations is presented here as well as an optional typological scheme.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Smith, Adam T. "Late Bronze/Early Iron Age Fortresses of the Ararat and Shirak Plains, Armenia: Typological Considerations1". Ancient Civilizations from Scythia to Siberia 5, n.º 2 (1999): 73–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/157005798x00017.

Texto completo
Resumen
AbstractWalled fortresses set atop rock outcrops and hills are the dominant settlement type documented in archaeological investigations of late second/early first millennium B.C. southern Transcaucasia. These sites arose as centers of the emerging complex polities in the region, marking not only the expansion of social inequalities but the formalization of a governmental apparatus. However, there have been few systematic attempts to understand the morphology of Late Bronze/Early Iron Age fortresses and assess dimensions of formal variation. This article proposes a typology of these early southern Transcaucasian fortresses based upon qualitative dimensions of a corpus of fortress sites from the Ararat and Shirak plains of the Republic of Armenia. Variation in these qualitative dimensions is then assessed in reference to quantitative elements of settlement.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Smith, Adam T. "Late Bronze/Early Iron Age Fortresses of the Ararat and Shirak Plains, Armenia: Typological Gonsiderations". Ancient Civilizations from Scythia to Siberia 5, n.º 3 (1999): 73–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/157005799x00133.

Texto completo
Resumen
AbstractWalled fortresses set atop rock outcrops and hills are the dominant settlement type documented in archaeological investigations of late second/early first millennium B.C. southern Transcaucasia. These sites arose as centers of the emerging complex polities in the region, marking not only the expansion of social inequalities but the formalization of a governmental apparatus. However, there have been few systematic attempts to understand the morphology of Late Bronze/Early Iron Age fortresses and assess dimensions of formal variation. This article proposes a typology of these early southern Transcaucasian fortresses based upon qualitative dimensions of a corpus of fortress sites from the Ararat and Shirak plains of the Republic of Armenia. Variation in these qualitative dimensions is then assessed in reference to quantitative elements of settlement.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Zavyalova, Viktoriya L. "Tracing the roots of phonetic variation in East Asian Englishes through loan phonology". Russian Journal of Linguistics 24, n.º 3 (15 de diciembre de 2020): 569–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.22363/2687-0088-2020-24-3-569-588.

Texto completo
Resumen
One key aspect of Englishes in the Kachruvian Expanding Circle concerns phonetic features as they commonly bear traits of speakers native languages. This article explores language contact phenomena that are likely to cause L1L2 phonological transfer, which underlies the phonetic specificity of English in East Asia. Drawing on the general theory of loan phonology, the author treats phonographic adaptation of English loanwords in East Asian languages compared to Russian, as a reliable source of data that supports research on the nature of phonetic variation in Chinese, Korean, Japanese, and Russian Englishes. The data were obtained through comparative analysis of English loanwords (200 for each language) selected from dictionary sources and speech samples from the Russian-Asian Corpus of English which was collected in earlier research. The findings confirm typological correlation of phonological transfer in loanword phonographic adaptation and in foreign language phonology. In both linguistic contexts, a crucial role is played by syllabic constraints, because being the fundamental unit of any phonological system, a syllable serves a domain of its segmental and suprasegmental features. Consequently, various resyllabification phenomena occur in English borrowings in the languages of East Asia whose phonological typology is distant from that of English; as a demonstration of this same conflict, the syllabic and, hence, rhythmic organization of East Asian Englishes tends to exhibit similar code-copying variation. The greater typological proximity of English and Russian syllable regulations leads to fewer manifestations of syllabic and rhythmic restructuring in both loanword adaptations and English spoken by native speakers of Russian.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Kardelis, Vytautas. "Areal-typological complexity of Lithuanian dialects". Lietuvių kalba, n.º 11 (20 de diciembre de 2017): 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/lk.2017.22550.

Texto completo
Resumen
This article deals with one of the most promising stages in Lithuanian dialectology discussed in the article “Seven stages of Lithuanian dialectology” published in the 2016 issue of the scientific electronic journal “Lietuvių kalba” (‘The Lithuanian Language’) (see Kardelis 2016). I referred to this stage as typological; however, now I think that the best term for this stage is the term areal-typological complexity (arealtypologische Komplexität) which originates from works by Alfred Lameli (2013). The concept of complexity is not associated with the attempt to classify dialects according to different “distinctive features” but rather with an idea, clearly supported by empirical facts that diatopical variation in language is highly complex. A closer look at the context of Lithuanian dialectology research reveals that the concept of complexity is still not discussed thoroughly; while specific studies are practically non-existent. The most general methodological principle which should be applied in carrying out an areal-typological study of the complexity of Lithuanian dialects could be referred to as the principle of offside. This means that studies of Lithuanian dialectology should offside from the conventionally applied research tradition and from: a) the aim to classify dialects typologically; b) all classifications of Lithuanian dialects published up to date; c) distinctive features described in the contemporary classification (as well as earlier classifications); d) the goal to specify the boundaries of dialects and subdialects. The second general theoretical criterion is related to the approach to the linguistic system. Here I rely on the concept of a diasystem introduced into the field of dialectology by Uriel Weinreich (1954; 1974). The whole area of the Lithuanian language together with its diatopical variants may be interpreted as a diasystem of Lithuanian which consists of separate systems. The most suitable, convenient and universal criteria for the analysis of empirical data established by the long-standing theory and practice of research into phonology are the following: 1) the quality of the elements of a vocalism system; 2) the quantity of the elements of a vocalism system; 3) the interrelationship between the quality and quantity of a vocalism system. Since here we are dealing with the Lithuanian language which features a complex prosodic system, we must introduce an additional criterion, i.e. 4) stress. Empirical data for the present study were collected from modern, phonological “grammars of dialects”. This article does not encompass the whole diasystem of the Lithuanian language since it only tackles the area covered by the Aukštaičiai dialect. The main phonological qualities according to which the basic vocalism model of the diasystem of the Aukštaičiai dialect can be described are the following: 1) the system of long vowels in the stressed position; 2) the shift in the level of rise of low vowels; 3) the system of short vowels in the stressed position; 4) automatic qualitative shifts; 5) vowel reduction (three degrees). The basic vocalism model described in accordance with the above criteria rather clearly indicates that the great differentiation of Lithuanian dialects postulated in the works on Lithuanian dialectology only has a phonetic and not a phonological basis and it can only be based on the differences of phonetic features. A phonological approach to the diasystem of the Aukštaičiai dialect of Lithuanian does not reveal any radical or extreme differentiation. In addition, the model also shows another significant regularity. The more features are taken into account, the lower the occurrence of individual, less significant dialectal elements distinguished on the basis of one feature (in comparison to the classification by A. Girdenis and Z. Zinkevičius). This approach thus allows solving the complexity of the puzzle of Lithuanian dialects (or, rather the complexity of the diasystem of the Aukštaičiai dialect of Lithuanian) which is summarised in Figure 10. As the matrix in Figure 9 illustrates, the diasystem of the Aukštaičiai dialect of Lithuanian so far consists of three zones represented in the matrix by three different colours.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Ofrecemos descuentos en todos los planes premium para autores cuyas obras están incluidas en selecciones literarias temáticas. ¡Contáctenos para obtener un código promocional único!

Pasar a la bibliografía