Siga este enlace para ver otros tipos de publicaciones sobre el tema: Contingent capture of attention.

Artículos de revistas sobre el tema "Contingent capture of attention"

Crea una cita precisa en los estilos APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard y otros

Elija tipo de fuente:

Consulte los 50 mejores artículos de revistas para su investigación sobre el tema "Contingent capture of attention".

Junto a cada fuente en la lista de referencias hay un botón "Agregar a la bibliografía". Pulsa este botón, y generaremos automáticamente la referencia bibliográfica para la obra elegida en el estilo de cita que necesites: APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

También puede descargar el texto completo de la publicación académica en formato pdf y leer en línea su resumen siempre que esté disponible en los metadatos.

Explore artículos de revistas sobre una amplia variedad de disciplinas y organice su bibliografía correctamente.

1

Remington, Roger W., Charles L. Folk y John P. Mclean. "Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention?" Perception & Psychophysics 63, n.º 2 (febrero de 2001): 298–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03194470.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Leblanc, Émilie, David J. Prime y Pierre Jolicoeur. "Tracking the Location of Visuospatial Attention in a Contingent Capture Paradigm". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20, n.º 4 (abril de 2008): 657–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20051.

Texto completo
Resumen
Currently, there is considerable controversy regarding the degree to which top-down control can affect attentional capture by salient events. According to the contingent capture hypothesis, attentional capture by a salient stimulus is contingent on a match between the properties of the stimulus and top-down attentional control settings. In contrast, bottom-up saliency accounts argue that the initial capture of attention is determined solely by the relative salience of the stimulus, and the effect of top-down attentional control is limited to effects on the duration of attentional engagement on the capturing stimulus. In the present study, we tested these competing accounts by utilizing the N2pc event-related potential component to track the locus of attention during an attentional capture task. The results were completely consistent with the contingent capture hypothesis: An N2pc wave was elicited only by distractors that possessed the target-defining attribute. In a second experiment, we expanded upon this finding by exploring the effect of target-distractor similarity on the duration that attention dwells at the distractor location. In this experiment, only distractors possessing the target-defining attribute (color) captured visuospatial attention to their location and the N2pc increased in duration and in magnitude when the capture distractor also shared a second target attribute (category membership). Finally, in three additional control experiments, we replicated the finding of an N2pc generated by distractors, only if they shared the target-defining attribute. Thus, our results demonstrate that attentional control settings influence both which stimuli attract attention and to what extent they are processed.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Chua, F. "Non-contingent attention capture by an onset". Journal of Vision 10, n.º 7 (2 de agosto de 2010): 112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/10.7.112.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Zivony, Alon y Dominique Lamy. "Contingent Attentional Engagement: Stimulus- and Goal-Driven Capture Have Qualitatively Different Consequences". Psychological Science 29, n.º 12 (4 de octubre de 2018): 1930–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797618799302.

Texto completo
Resumen
We examined whether shifting attention to a location necessarily entails extracting the features at that location, a process referred to as attentional engagement. In three spatial-cuing experiments ( N = 60), we found that an onset cue captured attention both when it shared the target’s color and when it did not. Yet the effects of the match between the response associated with the cued object’s identity and the response associated with the target (compatibility effects), which are diagnostic of attentional engagement, were observed only with relevant-color onset cues. These findings demonstrate that stimulus- and goal-driven capture have qualitatively different consequences: Before attention is reoriented to the target, it is engaged to the location of the critical distractor following goal-driven capture but not stimulus-driven capture. The reported dissociation between attentional shifts and attentional engagement suggests that attention is best described as a camera: One can align its zoom lens without pressing the shutter button.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Huang, Wanyi, Yuling Su, Yanfen Zhen y Zhe Qu. "The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture". Psychophysiology 53, n.º 5 (16 de febrero de 2016): 650–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12615.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Reeck, Crystal, Kevin S. LaBar y Tobias Egner. "Neural Mechanisms Mediating Contingent Capture of Attention by Affective Stimuli". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24, n.º 5 (mayo de 2012): 1113–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00211.

Texto completo
Resumen
Attention is attracted exogenously by physically salient stimuli, but this effect can be dampened by endogenous attention settings, a phenomenon called “contingent capture.” Emotionally salient stimuli are also thought to exert a strong exogenous influence on attention, especially in anxious individuals, but whether and how top–down attention can ameliorate bottom–up capture by affective stimuli is currently unknown. Here, we paired a novel spatial cueing task with fMRI to investigate contingent capture as a function of the affective salience of bottom–up cues (face stimuli) and individual differences in trait anxiety. In the absence of top–down cues, exogenous stimuli validly cueing targets facilitated attention in low-anxious participants, regardless of affective salience. However, although high-anxious participants exhibited similar facilitation following neutral exogenous cues, this facilitation was completely absent following affectively negative exogenous cues. Critically, these effects were contingent on endogenous attentional settings, such that explicit top–down cues presented before the appearance of exogenous stimuli removed anxious individuals' sensitivity to affectively salient stimuli. fMRI analyses revealed a network of brain regions underlying this variability in affective contingent capture across individuals, including the fusiform face area (FFA), posterior ventrolateral frontal cortex, and SMA. Importantly, activation in the posterior ventrolateral frontal cortex and the SMA fully mediated the effects observed in FFA, demonstrating a critical role for these frontal regions in mediating attentional orienting and interference resolution processes when engaged by affectively salient stimuli.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Du, Feng, Yue Yin, Yue Qi y Kan Zhang. "Contingent orienting or contingent capture: A size singleton matching the target–distractor size relation cannot capture attention". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21, n.º 4 (20 de diciembre de 2013): 1011–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0567-0.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Wang, L. y S. B. Most. "Is contingent attentional capture not contingent on working memory?" Journal of Vision 8, n.º 6 (20 de marzo de 2010): 1121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/8.6.1121.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Serences, John T., Sarah Shomstein, Andrew B. Leber, Xavier Golay, Howard E. Egeth y Steven Yantis. "Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex". Psychological Science 16, n.º 2 (febrero de 2005): 114–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00791.x.

Texto completo
Resumen
Visual attention may be voluntarily directed to particular locations or features (voluntary control), or it may be captured by salient stimuli, such as the abrupt appearance of a new perceptual object (stimulus-driven control). Most often, however, the deployment of attention is the result of a dynamic interplay between voluntary attentional control settings (e.g., based on prior knowledge about a target's location or color) and the degree to which stimuli in the visual scene match these voluntary control settings. Consequently, nontarget items in the scene that share a defining feature with the target of visual search can capture attention, a phenomenon termed contingent attentional capture. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to show that attentional capture by target-colored distractors is accompanied by increased cortical activity in corresponding regions of retinotopically organized visual cortex. Concurrent activation in the temporo-parietal junction and ventral frontal cortex suggests that these regions coordinate voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control settings to determine which stimuli effectively compete for attention.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Schönhammer, Josef G. y Dirk Kerzel. "Detection costs and contingent attentional capture". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 79, n.º 2 (28 de noviembre de 2016): 429–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1248-7.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Wirth, Benedikt Emanuel y Dirk Wentura. "Attentional bias to threat in the general population is contingent on target competition, not on attentional control settings". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 71, n.º 4 (1 de enero de 2018): 975–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1307864.

Texto completo
Resumen
Dot-probe studies usually find an attentional bias towards threatening stimuli only in anxious participants. Here, we investigated under what conditions such a bias occurs in unselected samples. According to contingent-capture theory, an irrelevant cue only captures attention if it matches an attentional control setting. Therefore, we first tested the hypothesis that an attentional control setting tuned to threat must be activated in (non-anxious) individuals. In Experiment 1, we used a dot-probe task with a manipulation of attentional control settings (‘threat’ – set vs. control set). Surprisingly, we found an (anxiety-independent) attentional bias to angry faces that was not moderated by attentional control settings. Since we presented two stimuli (i.e., a target and a distractor) on the target screen in Experiment 1 (a necessity to realise the test of contingent capture), but most dot-probe studies only employ a single target, we conducted Experiment 2 to test the hypothesis that attentional bias in the general population is contingent on target competition. Participants performed a dot-probe task, involving presentation of a stand-alone target or a target competing with a distractor. We found an (anxiety-independent) attentional bias towards angry faces in the latter but not the former condition. This suggests that attentional bias towards angry faces in unselected samples is not contingent on attentional control settings but on target competition.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Hilchey, Matthew D., Blaire J. Weidler y Jay Pratt. "Statistical learning can modulate contingent attentional capture". Journal of Vision 19, n.º 10 (6 de septiembre de 2019): 139c. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/19.10.139c.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Liao, H. I. y S. L. Yeh. "Contingent attentional capture depends on stimulus properties". Journal of Vision 11, n.º 11 (23 de septiembre de 2011): 85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/11.11.85.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Wyble, Brad, Charles Folk y Mary C. Potter. "Contingent attentional capture by conceptually relevant images." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 39, n.º 3 (2013): 861–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030517.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

Livingstone, Ashley C., Gregory J. Christie, Richard D. Wright y John J. McDonald. "Signal enhancement, not active suppression, follows the contingent capture of visual attention." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 43, n.º 2 (2017): 219–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000339.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

Dubbelde, Dick y Adam Greenberg. "Proportional Context of Distracters alters Top-Down Sets during Contingent Attention Capture". Journal of Vision 17, n.º 10 (31 de agosto de 2017): 947. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/17.10.947.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

ARIGA, A. y K. YOKOSAWA. "Contingent attentional capture occurs by activated target congruence". Perception & Psychophysics 70, n.º 4 (1 de mayo de 2008): 680–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pp.70.4.680.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

Folk, Charles L., Roger W. Remington y James C. Johnston. "Contingent attentional capture: A reply to Yantis (1993)." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 19, n.º 3 (1993): 682–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.19.3.682.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Barras, Caroline y Dirk Kerzel. "ERP correlates of contingent attentional capture and suppression". Journal of Vision 15, n.º 12 (1 de septiembre de 2015): 318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/15.12.318.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Schmidt, James R. y Daniel H. Weissman. "Contingent attentional capture triggers the congruency sequence effect". Acta Psychologica 159 (julio de 2015): 61–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.05.007.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Prasad y Mishra. "The Nature of Unconscious Attention to Subliminal Cues". Vision 3, n.º 3 (1 de agosto de 2019): 38. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/vision3030038.

Texto completo
Resumen
Attentional selection in humans is mostly determined by what is important to them or by the saliency of the objects around them. How our visual and attentional system manage these various sources of attentional capture is one of the most intensely debated issues in cognitive psychology. Along with the traditional dichotomy of goal-driven and stimulus-driven theories, newer frameworks such as reward learning and selection history have been proposed as well to understand how a stimulus captures attention. However, surprisingly little is known about the different forms of attentional control by information that is not consciously accessible to us. In this article, we will review several studies that have examined attentional capture by subliminal cues. We will specifically focus on spatial cuing studies that have shown through response times and eye movements that subliminal cues can affect attentional selection. A majority of these studies have argued that attentional capture by subliminal cues is entirely automatic and stimulus-driven. We will evaluate their claims of automaticity and contrast them with a few other studies that have suggested that orienting to unconscious cues proceeds in a manner that is contingent with the top-down goals of the individual. Resolving this debate has consequences for understanding the depths and the limits of unconscious processing. It has implications for general theories of attentional selection as well. In this review, we aim to provide the current status of research in this domain and point out open questions and future directions.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Folk, C., A. Berenato y B. Wyble. "Semantic Priming Produces Contingent Attentional Capture by Conceptual Content". Journal of Vision 14, n.º 10 (22 de agosto de 2014): 318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/14.10.318.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

Munneke, Jaap, Johannes Jacobus Fahrenfort, David Sutterer, Jan Theeuwes y Edward Awh. "Multivariate analysis of EEG activity indexes contingent attentional capture". NeuroImage 226 (febrero de 2021): 117562. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117562.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Liao, Hsin-I. y Su-Ling Yeh. "Capturing attention is not that simple: Different mechanisms for stimulus-driven and contingent capture". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 75, n.º 8 (14 de septiembre de 2013): 1703–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0537-7.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Moore, Katherine Sledge y Daniel H. Weissman. "Involuntary transfer of a top-down attentional set into the focus of attention: Evidence from a contingent attentional capture paradigm". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 72, n.º 6 (agosto de 2010): 1495–509. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/app.72.6.1495.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

Barratt, Daniel y Claus Bundesen. "Attentional capture by emotional faces is contingent on attentional control settings". Cognition & Emotion 26, n.º 7 (noviembre de 2012): 1223–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.645279.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Anderson, Brian A. y Charles L. Folk. "Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 74, n.º 6 (7 de junio de 2012): 1183–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0325-9.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

Winther, Gesche N. y Michael Niedeggen. "Distractor-Induced Blindness: A Special Case of Contingent Attentional Capture?" Advances in Cognitive Psychology 13, n.º 1 (31 de marzo de 2017): 52–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.5709/acp-0206-5.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Folk, Charles L., Andrew B. Leber y Howard E. Egeth. "Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink". Perception & Psychophysics 64, n.º 5 (julio de 2002): 741–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03194741.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Huffman, Greg, Victoria M. Antinucci y Jay Pratt. "The illusion of control: Sequential dependencies underlie contingent attentional capture". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25, n.º 6 (12 de enero de 2018): 2238–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1422-5.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Ansorge, Ulrich, Gernot Horstmann y Ingrid Scharlau. "Top–down contingent attentional capture during feed-forward visual processing". Acta Psychologica 135, n.º 2 (octubre de 2010): 123–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.008.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Úlfsson, Tryggvi Örn. "Possibility of Science, Impossibility of Miracles: Léon Brunschvicg against Quentin Meillassoux". Praktyka Teoretyczna 28, n.º 2 (15 de octubre de 2018): 124–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.14746/prt.2018.2.6.

Texto completo
Resumen
The article argues that, while Quentin Meillassoux‘s project, undertaken in After Finitude, merits attention, since the French philosopher is right that faith in sciences‘ capacity to open up new domains to thought must be restored, the solutions he offers have two serious shortcomings. 1) His depiction of science as the producer of ancestral statements does not capture satisfactorily the essence of scientific creativity. 2) The claim that everything is necessarily contingent is fundamentally incompatible with scientific knowledge. The article, then, contrasts Meillassoux‘s principle of the necessity of contingency with a principle that is extracted from the historical epistemology of Léon Brunschvicg and Antoine-Augustin Cournot. Instead of a principle of unreason, the article defends a principle of a metamorphosing reason founded on the practical impossibility of irreducible contingency.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Blakely, D., T. Wright, W. Boot y J. Brockmole. "On The Precision of Attention Sets: Effects of Distractor Probability and Temporal Expectations on Contingent Capture". Journal of Vision 11, n.º 11 (23 de septiembre de 2011): 81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/11.11.81.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

WANG, Hui-Yuan, Ai-Rui CHEN y Ming ZHANG. "Meaningful contingent attentional orienting effects: Spatial location-based inhibition and capture". Acta Psychologica Sinica 53, n.º 2 (2021): 113. http://dx.doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00113.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

Van der Lubbe, Rob H. J. y Jurjen Van der Helden. "Failure of the extended contingent attentional capture account in multimodal settings". Advances in Cognitive Psychology 2, n.º 4 (1 de enero de 2006): 255–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0060-x.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Atchley, Paul, Arthur F. Kramer y Anne P. Hillstrom. "Contingent capture for onsets and offsets: Attentional set for perceptual transients." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26, n.º 2 (2000): 594–606. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.594.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Al-Aidroos, N., M. Giammarco, A. Paoletti y E. Guild. "Contingent attentional capture by stimuli that match long-term memory representations". Journal of Vision 14, n.º 10 (22 de agosto de 2014): 645. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/14.10.645.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Moore, Katherine, Greg Ramos y Kathleen Trencheny. "Set-specific contingent attentional capture costs are modulated by color similarity". Journal of Vision 15, n.º 12 (1 de septiembre de 2015): 311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/15.12.311.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Mayer, Jutta S., Keisuke Fukuda, Edward K. Vogel y Sohee Park. "Impaired Contingent Attentional Capture Predicts Reduced Working Memory Capacity in Schizophrenia". PLoS ONE 7, n.º 11 (12 de noviembre de 2012): e48586. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048586.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Moore, K., E. Wiemers, S. Lee y C. Santos. "Dual-target contingent attentional capture effects are modulated by associative learning". Journal of Vision 13, n.º 9 (25 de julio de 2013): 87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/13.9.87.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Glickman, Moshe y Dominique Lamy. "Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces is contingent on implicit attentional settings". Cognition and Emotion 32, n.º 2 (10 de marzo de 2017): 303–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1301883.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Gibson, Bradley S. y Erin M. Kelsey. "Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for displaywide visual features." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24, n.º 3 (1998): 699–706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.699.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Blakely, D. P., R. S. Landbeck y W. R. Boot. "On the Precision of Attention Sets: The Effects of Spatial Context and Distractor Multiplicity on Contingent Capture". Journal of Vision 12, n.º 9 (10 de agosto de 2012): 1345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/12.9.1345.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Reuss, Heiko, Carsten Pohl, Andrea Kiesel y Wilfried Kunde. "Follow the sign! Top-down contingent attentional capture of masked arrow cues". Advances in Cognitive Psychology 7, n.º -1 (1 de enero de 2011): 82–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0091-3.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Du, Feng, Jiaoyan Yang, Yue Yin, Kan Zhang y Richard A. Abrams. "On the automaticity of contingent capture: disruption caused by the attentional blink". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20, n.º 5 (27 de febrero de 2013): 944–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0410-7.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Ito, Motohiro y Jun I. Kawahara. "Contingent attentional capture across multiple feature dimensions in a temporal search task". Acta Psychologica 163 (enero de 2016): 107–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.11.009.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Lu, Shena y Shihui Han. "Attentional capture is contingent on the interaction between task demand and stimulus salience". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 71, n.º 5 (julio de 2009): 1015–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/app.71.5.1015.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Ghorashi, S. M. Shahab, Samantha M. Zuvic, Troy A. W. Visser y Vincent Di Lollo. "Focal distraction: Spatial shifts of attentional focus are not required for contingent capture." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29, n.º 1 (2003): 78–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.78.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Lamy, D., L. Alon, N. Shalev y T. Carmel. "The role of conscious perception in contingent attentional capture and working memory updating". Journal of Vision 14, n.º 10 (22 de agosto de 2014): 643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/14.10.643.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Wu, C. t., D. H. Weissman y M. G. Woldorff. "Contingent attentional capture occurs only for irrelevant stimuli that can be consciously perceived". Journal of Vision 6, n.º 6 (24 de marzo de 2010): 602. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/6.6.602.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Ofrecemos descuentos en todos los planes premium para autores cuyas obras están incluidas en selecciones literarias temáticas. ¡Contáctenos para obtener un código promocional único!

Pasar a la bibliografía