Artículos de revistas sobre el tema "Action"

Siga este enlace para ver otros tipos de publicaciones sobre el tema: Action.

Crea una cita precisa en los estilos APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard y otros

Elija tipo de fuente:

Consulte los 50 mejores artículos de revistas para su investigación sobre el tema "Action".

Junto a cada fuente en la lista de referencias hay un botón "Agregar a la bibliografía". Pulsa este botón, y generaremos automáticamente la referencia bibliográfica para la obra elegida en el estilo de cita que necesites: APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

También puede descargar el texto completo de la publicación académica en formato pdf y leer en línea su resumen siempre que esté disponible en los metadatos.

Explore artículos de revistas sobre una amplia variedad de disciplinas y organice su bibliografía correctamente.

1

Marx, Vivien. "Actin in action". Nature Methods 20, n.º 2 (febrero de 2023): 178–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41592-022-01762-2.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Hurtley, Stella M. "Parasite actin in action". Science 366, n.º 6465 (31 de octubre de 2019): 584.6–585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.366.6465.584-f.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Hurtley, S. M. "Nuclear Actin in Action". Science Signaling 6, n.º 276 (21 de mayo de 2013): ec116-ec116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/scisignal.2004335.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Abedin Tanvir, Fakhrul. "Assessing the Validity of Human Intention for Action: Exploring Unintentional Actions". Public Health Open Access 8, n.º 1 (2024): 1–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.23880/phoa-16000274.

Texto completo
Resumen
This paper examines the validity of human intention for action, specifically focusing on unintentional actions that are unaffected by bias. Through the observation of a substantial number of individuals, estimated to be over 100, we investigate the power of human actions and their corresponding intentions. Given the underlying similarities in general thought processes and intentions among humans, it becomes possible to establish common patterns by observing a significant sample size. While this research provides observational results indicating a one-second validity of human intentions, it is important to note that these findings have not been scientifically proven. Nevertheless, this study contributes to the ongoing discourse by shedding light on participant expressions and experiences, furthering our understanding of human intentionality and action.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Vahidnia, Hamid y Ronald Mitchell. "Employing Control in Entrepreneurial Actions: An Action-to-Action Model". Academy of Management Proceedings 2014, n.º 1 (enero de 2014): 15046. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/ambpp.2014.15046abstract.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Gudehus, Christian. "Action Action Action". Journal of Perpetrator Research 3, n.º 1 (5 de mayo de 2020): 188. http://dx.doi.org/10.21039/jpr.2.1.35.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Fagioli, Sabrina, Fabio Ferlazzo y Bernhard Hommel. "Controlling attention through action: Observing actions primes action-related stimulus dimensions". Neuropsychologia 45, n.º 14 (enero de 2007): 3351–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.012.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

Melin, Ulf y Karin Axelsson. "Action in action research". Journal of Systems and Information Technology 18, n.º 2 (9 de mayo de 2016): 118–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/jsit-10-2015-0074.

Texto completo
Resumen
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to elaborate on the concept of action by addressing actions and roles in the practice of action research, illustrated by dilemmas in an action research project on information systems development in public sector. The main ambition with action research is being able to solve organisational problems through intervention and to contribute to scientific knowledge. The main emphasis has so far been on the “research part”. Here the authors focus on the “action part” of action research to generate rigorous research, to solve local problems and to deal with evident dilemmas in action research. Design/methodology/approach This is a qualitative case study. The empirical illustrations of this paper originate from an action research project that focused the two e-service development initiatives analysed below. The analysis is structured using key aspects and phases proposed by Avison et al. (2001). As a result of the analysis, the concept of action is elaborated. The action elements action, actor, motive, space and time are analysed together with different roles. This goes beyond the existing action research literature. Findings The conclusions show that there is a need to understand actions and roles within action research projects – not separating action from research. Research is also seen as action. The practice of action research is also discussed as context-bounded interactive social action: action research as a recurrent, interactive and dynamic activity. It is also identified that the understanding of roles, actions and interaction can help handle dilemmas in action research. Research Limitations/implications The authors contribute to the body of knowledge concerning action research in the information systems research field and in general by exploring the need to study the concept of action (e.g. situations and elements), to be explicit concerning the different phases, roles and responsibilities and management of different dilemmas in action research. A limitation of this study is that the inter-organisational development character in this study adds an extra dimension into the practice of actions research only partially highlighted. Another limitation is focus on public agencies. However, this is not critical for the results on action elements and the action research dilemmas that are studied. Practical Implications The understanding of roles, actions and interaction can solve the dilemmas and challenges linked to the practice of action research in the information systems field, but such understanding can help discover and handle dilemmas in action research. Originality/value The originality in this research is an illustration of and a perspective of action research as a context-bounded interactive social action: action research as a recurrent, interactive and dynamic activity. The value is that this knowledge can help handle dilemmas in action research.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Peruzzi, Alberto. "Action of structures, structure of actions". Axiomathes 6, n.º 3 (diciembre de 1995): 325–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02228983.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Nanay, Bence. "Comment: Every Action Is an Emotional Action". Emotion Review 9, n.º 4 (8 de agosto de 2017): 350–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073916684556.

Texto completo
Resumen
In action theory, emotional actions are standardly treated as exceptions—cases where the “normal” springs of action are not functioning properly. My aim here is to argue that this is not so. We have plenty of evidence—beautifully brought together in the present special issue—that emotions play a crucial and often constitutive role in all the important phases of action preparation and initiation. Most of our actions are less stupid than, say, Zidane’s head-butt (a paradigmatic emotional action), but all of our actions have emotional components. Actions can be more or less emotional, but they are never completely nonemotional.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Silva Pereira, Fernando. "Ação de impugnação pauliana contra devedor insolvente – efeitos insolvenciais da procedência da ação". Revista Electrónica de Direito 27, n.º 1 (2022): 194–222. http://dx.doi.org/10.24840/2182-9845_2022-0001_0006.

Texto completo
Resumen
This article discusses the problem of knowing whether, if an actio pauliana is filed to contest an act performed by an insolvent debtor (or one that has been declared insolvent in the meantime), the effects of the granting of this action will benefit only the claimant creditor (who has the right to the execution of the asset directly in the third party acquirer's legal sphere - Article 616(1) CC - competing with the other creditors of the third party acquirer) or whether, on the contrary, should the object of the contested transaction return to the debtor's legal sphere, entering the insolvent estate and thus benefiting all of the insolvent debtor's creditors (principle of par conditio creditorum). In the previous legal regime, the CPEREF, there was a figure, the collective actio pauliana, which disappeared in the current CIRE, being absorbed by the regime of "unconditional resolution". In addition to a problem of articulation of the action of actio pauliana (singular) with the regime of resolution for the benefit of the insolvent mass (see Article 127 CIRE), there is the problem of the insolvency effects of the granting of such action. Article 127 CIRE, in particular paragraph 3 of this article, leaves doubts on this matter, and there is no total uniformity in doctrine and jurisprudence.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Shulga, Anatoliy. "Remote action as a type of legally significant action". 33, n.º 33 (28 de junio de 2022): 16–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.26565/2075-1834-2022-33-02.

Texto completo
Resumen
Introduction. In the article, the system-forming subject of research is remote action as a type of lawful actions of legal entities, which is understood as the direct opposite of contact action, the criterion for distinguishing which is their method of implementation in space. The concepts, signs of contact and remote actions, as well as the classification of the main types of remote actions are represented. The purpose of the article involves, first of all, the characterization of remote action from the standpoint of general theoretical jurisprudence, the definition of its concept with the justification of its terminological form and internal logic. Summary of the main research results. According to the method of implementation in space, it is necessary to distinguish between contact actions and remote (non-contact) actions, which become lawful actions in the case when the hypothesis of the rule of law provides for the relevant action as a legal fact, as a result its "legal consequences" (positive, negative). The main features of remote action are "instrumental" feature (a special way of implementing the action) and "spatial" feature (remoteness in the physical space of the subject of action in relation to a particular object, subject). The basic concepts of the author's definition of remote action: "mode of action", "remoteness (significant, insignificant) in space", "subject of action", "object (subject) of influence (interaction)". According to the method of implementation there are remote actions: verbal, physical (body-moving): insignificant, symbolic; using: natural (modified, unmodified) means; artificial means ("technical", "robotic" - non-autonomous, autonomous). By types of technical means there are remote actions with the use of technical means: non-electronic, electronic; the latest (using technology with artificial intelligence technology), historical and traditional. Conclusions. The term "remote action" is ambiguous, the phrase "remote action" may be related to such legal categories as "subjects of law (legal relations)", "objects of law (legal relations)". Legally significant "remote action" should be defined as a rule provided by the rules (principles) of law, which is carried out in a way that allows a certain significant or insignificant distance in the space of the subject of action and its object (subject) of influence (interaction). Today, first of all, there is a need to distinguish between remote actions using artificial means (technical, robotic - non-autonomous, autonomous), as well as - physical, using the Internet, electronic remote actions.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Raposo, Ana, Helen E. Moss, Emmanuel A. Stamatakis y Lorraine K. Tyler. "Modulation of motor and premotor cortices by actions, action words and action sentences". Neuropsychologia 47, n.º 2 (enero de 2009): 388–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.017.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Dubey, Bidhyut Kumar, Mohini Chaurasia y Jyoti Yadav. "ACTIVE CHEMICAL CONSTITUENTS FROM MEDICINAL PLANTS AND MECHANISM OF ACTION AS ANTIPARKINSONIAN". Era's Journal of Medical Research 7, n.º 1 (junio de 2020): 120–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.24041/ejmr2019.120.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

Dubey, Bidhyut Kumar, Mohini Chaurasia y Jyoti Yadav. "ACTIVE CHEMICAL CONSTITUENTS FROM MEDICINAL PLANTS AND MECHANISM OF ACTION AS ANTIPARKINSONIAN". Era's Journal of Medical Research 7, n.º 1 (junio de 2020): 120–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.24041/ejmr2020.20.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

Press, Clare, Elena Gherri, Cecilia Heyes y Martin Eimer. "Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, n.º 10 (octubre de 2010): 2198–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21409.

Texto completo
Resumen
Several theories of the mechanisms linking perception and action require that the links are bidirectional, but there is a lack of consensus on the effects that action has on perception. We investigated this by measuring visual event-related brain potentials to observed hand actions while participants prepared responses that were spatially compatible (e.g., both were on the left side of the body) or incompatible and action type compatible (e.g., both were finger taps) or incompatible, with observed actions. An early enhanced processing of spatially compatible stimuli was observed, which is likely due to spatial attention. This was followed by an attenuation of processing for both spatially and action type compatible stimuli, likely to be driven by efference copy signals that attenuate processing of predicted sensory consequences of actions. Attenuation was not response-modality specific; it was found for manual stimuli when participants prepared manual and vocal responses, in line with the hypothesis that action control is hierarchically organized. These results indicate that spatial attention and forward model prediction mechanisms have opposite, but temporally distinct, effects on perception. This hypothesis can explain the inconsistency of recent findings on action–perception links and thereby supports the view that sensorimotor links are bidirectional. Such effects of action on perception are likely to be crucial, not only for the control of our own actions but also in sociocultural interaction, allowing us to predict the reactions of others to our own actions.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

Touati, Hakim y Zbigniew W. Ras. "Mining Meta-Actions for Action Rules Reduction". Fundamenta Informaticae 127, n.º 1-4 (2013): 225–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/fi-2013-906.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

Khan, Fahad Shahbaz, Jiaolong Xu, Joost van de Weijer, Andrew D. Bagdanov, Rao Muhammad Anwer y Antonio M. Lopez. "Recognizing Actions Through Action-Specific Person Detection". IEEE Transactions on Image Processing 24, n.º 11 (noviembre de 2015): 4422–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/tip.2015.2465147.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Ogura, Tamiko, Junko Hirai y Naoko Hamabe. "Children’s Actions Responding to Mother’s Action Words". Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association 86 (2022): 1PM—070—PO—1PM—070—PO. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/pacjpa.86.0_1pm-070-po.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Babakhanian, Armand. "Naive Action Theory and Essentially Intentional Actions". Southwest Philosophy Review 40, n.º 1 (2024): 229–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/swphilreview202440125.

Texto completo
Resumen
In their recent paper, “Practical Knowledge without Luminosity,” Bob Beddor and Carlotta Pavese (2022) claim that the doctrine of essentially intentional actions, or “essentialism,” is false. Essentialism states that some actions are essentially intentional, such that, “whenever they are performed, they are performed intentionally” (2022, p. 926). Beddor and Pavese work to reject essentialism, which figures as a key premise in Juan Piñeros Glasscock’s anti-luminosity argument against the knowledge condition for intentional action (Piñeros Glasscock, p. 1240). Historically, essentialism has received little attention from philosophers since its inception in Elizabeth Anscombe’s Intention (2000, §47). However, I believe that essentially intentional actions can play an important role in an ontology of action. In my paper, I develop and argue for a variety of essentialism in the context of naive action theory, which I call naive essentialism. Naive essentialism is a two-fold thesis, which claims that (1) essentially intentional actions exist, and (2) that essentially intentional actions ground accidentally intentional actions. My paper has four parts. In the first part, I distinguish between essentially and accidentally intentional actions, and unpack the relevant principles of naive action theory. Second, I present the grounding thesis that accidentally intentional actions are grounded in essentially intentional actions. Next, I provide an argument for the existence of essentially intentional actions. Lastly, I briefly respond to a possible objection to my argument. The upshot of my arguments is that essentially intentional actions form the metaphysical and explanatory bedrock in a naive ontology of action, and that there are good reasons for accepting a key premise in Piñeros Glasscock’s anti-luminosity argument.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Blessing, Carolyn A., Gergana T. Ugrinova y Holly V. Goodson. "Actin and ARPs: action in the nucleus". Trends in Cell Biology 14, n.º 8 (agosto de 2004): 435–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tcb.2004.07.009.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Royall, Dawna. "Collective action / Action collective". Canadian Journal of Dietetic Practice and Research 75, n.º 03 (1 de septiembre de 2014): 110. http://dx.doi.org/10.3148/cjdpr-2014-016.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

Townsley, Jill. "Repetitive action repetitive action". Radar 1, n.º 1 (marzo de 2010): 30–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.5920/radar.2010.1130.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Shepherd, Joshua. "Conscious Action/Zombie Action". Noûs 50, n.º 2 (9 de enero de 2015): 419–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/nous.12086.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Grzybowski, Anne. "Action learning in action". Perspectives: Policy and Practice in Higher Education 12, n.º 4 (octubre de 2008): 110–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13603100802376592.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

Pealtie, Ken. "Action Learning in Action". Management Learning 27, n.º 1 (marzo de 1996): 87–112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1350507696271006.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Carter, Philip D. "Building purposeful action: action methods and action research". Educational Action Research 10, n.º 2 (junio de 2002): 207–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09650790200200180.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

Suárez-Hernández, Alejandro, Javier Segovia-Aguas, Carme Torras y Guillem Alenyà. "Online Action Recognition". Proceedings of the AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence 35, n.º 13 (18 de mayo de 2021): 11981–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1609/aaai.v35i13.17423.

Texto completo
Resumen
Recognition in planning seeks to find agent intentions, goals or activities given a set of observations and a knowledge library (e.g. goal states, plans or domain theories). In this work we introduce the problem of Online Action Recognition. It consists in recognizing, in an open world, the planning action that best explains a partially observable state transition from a knowledge library of first-order STRIPS actions, which is initially empty. We frame this as an optimization problem, and propose two algorithms to address it: Action Unification (AU) and Online Action Recognition through Unification (OARU). The former builds on logic unification and generalizes two input actions using weighted partial MaxSAT. The latter looks for an action within the library that explains an observed transition. If there is such action, it generalizes it making use of AU, building in this way an AU hierarchy. Otherwise, OARU inserts a Trivial Grounded Action (TGA) in the library that explains just that transition. We report results on benchmarks from the International Planning Competition and PDDLGym, where OARU recognizes actions accurately with respect to expert knowledge, and shows real-time performance.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Lim, Junghwan, Chenglong Luo, Seunghun Lee, Young Eun Song y Hoeryong Jung. "Action Recognition of Taekwondo Unit Actions Using Action Images Constructed with Time-Warped Motion Profiles". Sensors 24, n.º 8 (18 de abril de 2024): 2595. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s24082595.

Texto completo
Resumen
Taekwondo has evolved from a traditional martial art into an official Olympic sport. This study introduces a novel action recognition model tailored for Taekwondo unit actions, utilizing joint-motion data acquired via wearable inertial measurement unit (IMU) sensors. The utilization of IMU sensor-measured motion data facilitates the capture of the intricate and rapid movements characteristic of Taekwondo techniques. The model, underpinned by a conventional convolutional neural network (CNN)-based image classification framework, synthesizes action images to represent individual Taekwondo unit actions. These action images are generated by mapping joint-motion profiles onto the RGB color space, thus encapsulating the motion dynamics of a single unit action within a solitary image. To further refine the representation of rapid movements within these images, a time-warping technique was applied, adjusting motion profiles in relation to the velocity of the action. The effectiveness of the proposed model was assessed using a dataset compiled from 40 Taekwondo experts, yielding remarkable outcomes: an accuracy of 0.998, a precision of 0.983, a recall of 0.982, and an F1 score of 0.982. These results underscore this time-warping technique’s contribution to enhancing feature representation, as well as the proposed method’s scalability and effectiveness in recognizing Taekwondo unit actions.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Coughlan de Perez, Erin, Bart van den Hurk, Maarten K. van Aalst, Irene Amuron, Deus Bamanya, Tristan Hauser, Brenden Jongma et al. "Action-based flood forecasting for triggering humanitarian action". Hydrology and Earth System Sciences 20, n.º 9 (5 de septiembre de 2016): 3549–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/hess-20-3549-2016.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract. Too often, credible scientific early warning information of increased disaster risk does not result in humanitarian action. With financial resources tilted heavily towards response after a disaster, disaster managers have limited incentive and ability to process complex scientific data, including uncertainties. These incentives are beginning to change, with the advent of several new forecast-based financing systems that provide funding based on a forecast of an extreme event. Given the changing landscape, here we demonstrate a method to select and use appropriate forecasts for specific humanitarian disaster prevention actions, even in a data-scarce location. This action-based forecasting methodology takes into account the parameters of each action, such as action lifetime, when verifying a forecast. Forecasts are linked with action based on an understanding of (1) the magnitude of previous flooding events and (2) the willingness to act "in vain" for specific actions. This is applied in the context of the Uganda Red Cross Society forecast-based financing pilot project, with forecasts from the Global Flood Awareness System (GloFAS). Using this method, we define the "danger level" of flooding, and we select the probabilistic forecast triggers that are appropriate for specific actions. Results from this methodology can be applied globally across hazards and fed into a financing system that ensures that automatic, pre-funded early action will be triggered by forecasts.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Stulp, F. y M. Beetz. "Refining the Execution of Abstract Actions with Learned Action Models". Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research 32 (27 de junio de 2008): 487–523. http://dx.doi.org/10.1613/jair.2479.

Texto completo
Resumen
Robots reason about abstract actions, such as "go to position `l'", in order to decide what to do or to generate plans for their intended course of action. The use of abstract actions enables robots to employ small action libraries, which reduces the search space for decision making. When executing the actions, however, the robot must tailor the abstract actions to the specific task and situation context at hand. In this article we propose a novel robot action execution system that learns success and performance models for possible specializations of abstract actions. At execution time, the robot uses these models to optimize the execution of abstract actions to the respective task contexts. The robot can so use abstract actions for efficient reasoning, without compromising the performance of action execution. We show the impact of our action execution model in three robotic domains and on two kinds of action execution problems: (1) the instantiation of free action parameters to optimize the expected performance of action sequences; (2) the automatic introduction of additional subgoals to make action sequences more reliable.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Sebanz, Natalie y Günther Knoblich. "Progress in Joint-Action Research". Current Directions in Psychological Science 30, n.º 2 (29 de enero de 2021): 138–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721420984425.

Texto completo
Resumen
Humans have a striking ability to coordinate their actions with each other to achieve joint goals. The tight interpersonal coordination that characterizes joint actions is achieved through processes that help with preparing for joint action as well as processes that are active while joint actions are being performed. To prepare for joint action, partners form representations of each other’s actions and tasks and the relation between them. This enables them to predict each other’s upcoming actions, which, in turn, facilitates coordination. While performing joint actions, partners’ coordination is maintained by (a) monitoring whether individual and joint outcomes correspond to what was planned, (b) predicting partners’ action parameters on the basis of familiarity with their individual actions, (c) communicating task-relevant information unknown to partners in an action-based fashion, and (d) relying on coupling of predictions through dense perceptual-information flow between coactors. The next challenge for the field of joint action is to generate an integrated perspective that links coordination mechanisms to normative, evolutionary, and communicative frameworks.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Lonkar, Bhavesh. "Criminal Action Surveillance System". Paripex - Indian Journal Of Research 2, n.º 3 (15 de enero de 2012): 105–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.15373/22501991/mar2013/39.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Dezfouli, Amir, Nura W. Lingawi y Bernard W. Balleine. "Habits as action sequences: hierarchical action control and changes in outcome value". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369, n.º 1655 (5 de noviembre de 2014): 20130482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0482.

Texto completo
Resumen
Goal-directed action involves making high-level choices that are implemented using previously acquired action sequences to attain desired goals. Such a hierarchical schema is necessary for goal-directed actions to be scalable to real-life situations, but results in decision-making that is less flexible than when action sequences are unfolded and the decision-maker deliberates step-by-step over the outcome of each individual action. In particular, from this perspective, the offline revaluation of any outcomes that fall within action sequence boundaries will be invisible to the high-level planner resulting in decisions that are insensitive to such changes. Here, within the context of a two-stage decision-making task, we demonstrate that this property can explain the emergence of habits. Next, we show how this hierarchical account explains the insensitivity of over-trained actions to changes in outcome value. Finally, we provide new data that show that, under extended extinction conditions, habitual behaviour can revert to goal-directed control, presumably as a consequence of decomposing action sequences into single actions. This hierarchical view suggests that the development of action sequences and the insensitivity of actions to changes in outcome value are essentially two sides of the same coin, explaining why these two aspects of automatic behaviour involve a shared neural structure.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

DESMARAIS, GENEVIÈVE, MARIA CRISTINA PENSA, MIKE J. DIXON y ERIC A. ROY. "The importance of object similarity in the production and identification of actions associated with objects". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 13, n.º 6 (18 de octubre de 2007): 1021–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1355617707071287.

Texto completo
Resumen
Past research suggests that the similarity between the objects associated with actions impacts visual action identification and action production. Indeed, people often confuse actions that are visually similar, as well as actions that are associated with visually similar objects. However, because the action errors often involve actions that are visually similar and are associated with visually similar objects, it is difficult to disambiguate between the influences of object similarity and action similarity. In our experiments, healthy participants were asked to learn to associate nonword names and actions with novel objects. Participants were first shown each object and its action and were then asked to visually identify each object. In Experiment 1, participants were then asked to produce the action associated with each object, and in Experiment 2, they were asked to visually identify the action associated with each object. Actions were confused more often when they were associated with similar objects than when they were associated with dissimilar objects. Furthermore, following an object naming error, participants were more likely to produce the action associated with the erroneous name than any other erroneous action. The results suggest that the visual characteristics of the objects influenced action production and action identification. (JINS, 2007, 13, 1021–1034.)
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Gerber, Doris. "The Concept of Action and the Relevance of Intentional Collective Action in History". Journal of the Philosophy of History 10, n.º 2 (17 de junio de 2016): 235–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/18722636-12341323.

Texto completo
Resumen
The article starts with the theses that it is the very concept of action that is at stake in many debates between philosophers and historians. Whereas in philosophy actions are conceptualized by reference to their beginning, namely their motives or intentions, in historiography the consequences of actions are much more in the focus of interest. Especially the debate about the dualism of structure and agency is characterized by different concepts of action. In the article it is argued that this dualism can be overcome by stressing the intentionality of collective actions. First, a concept of action is developed against the background of a theory of intentionality. Second, intentional collective action is distinguished from individual and social actions and an analysis of a collective we-intention is proposed. Third, it is argued that the development of social structures has to be understood by reference to intentional collective actions.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Vogt, Stefan. "Imagery and perception-action mediation in imitative actions". Cognitive Brain Research 3, n.º 2 (marzo de 1996): 79–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0926-6410(95)00032-1.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Le Runigo, Cyrille, Nicolas Benguigui y Benoit G. Bardy. "Perception–action coupling and expertise in interceptive actions". Human Movement Science 24, n.º 3 (junio de 2005): 429–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.008.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

Ménoret, Mathilde, Aurore Curie, Vincent des Portes, Tatjana A. Nazir y Yves Paulignan. "Simultaneous action execution and observation optimise grasping actions". Experimental Brain Research 227, n.º 3 (25 de abril de 2013): 407–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3523-3.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Kim, Si-Cheon. "Between ‘Non-action’ to ‘Action’". Journal of Korean Sundo Culture 9 (31 de agosto de 2010): 485–512. http://dx.doi.org/10.35573/jksc.9.13.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Schiavio, Andrea. "Action, Enaction, Inter(en)action". Empirical Musicology Review 9, n.º 3-4 (5 de enero de 2015): 254. http://dx.doi.org/10.18061/emr.v9i3-4.4440.

Texto completo
Resumen
<p>Leman and Maes offer a comprehensive review of the main theoretical and empirical themes covered by the research on <em>music</em> and <em>embodied cognition</em>. Their article provides an insight into the work being carried at the Institute for Psychoacoustic and Electronic Music (IPEM) of Ghent University, Belgium - in which they work - and presents a theory of the main implications of embodiment for music perception. The present paper is divided into three parts. In the first one, I will explore the conceptual topography of embodied music cognition as maintained by the authors, to see whether the empirical research proposed fits the aims of this standpoint. In the second I will argue that while Leman and Maes are right to move towards a more dynamically implemented stance, the arguments used to justify this shift seem to be inconsistent with the framework they account for. In the third and final part of this commentary I will claim that if the authors wish to dedicate their work to develop a truly embodied, sensorimotor, and dynamic account to music cognition, they would need to abandon some of the assumptions defended in their work, searching for further empirical corroboration in the concrete dynamics of interactive, or <em>participatory</em>, musical sense-making.</p>
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Pedler, Mike y Sonja Antell. "Action learning for social action". Action Learning: Research and Practice 16, n.º 1 (2 de enero de 2019): 3–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14767333.2019.1563326.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Heckhausen, Heinz y Jürgen Beckmann. "Intentional action and action slips." Psychological Review 97, n.º 1 (1990): 36–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.97.1.36.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Hussey, Laura S. "Political Action Versus Personal Action". American Politics Research 42, n.º 3 (9 de septiembre de 2013): 409–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1532673x13498620.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Heck, A. "In action with action potentials". Journal of Physics: Conference Series 1286 (agosto de 2019): 012054. http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/1742-6596/1286/1/012054.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Smith, Nicholas Ryan. "Right Action as Virtuous Action". Australasian Journal of Philosophy 96, n.º 2 (24 de julio de 2017): 241–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00048402.2017.1352611.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Springer, Anne y Wolfgang Prinz. "Action Semantics Modulate Action Prediction". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63, n.º 11 (noviembre de 2010): 2141–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470211003721659.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Bailey, Heather R., Christopher A. Kurby, Tania Giovannetti y Jeffrey M. Zacks. "Action perception predicts action performance". Neuropsychologia 51, n.º 11 (septiembre de 2013): 2294–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.022.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

De Maeght, Sara y Wolfgang Prinz. "Action induction through action observation". Psychological Research 68, n.º 2-3 (3 de febrero de 2004): 97–114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0148-3.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Wenke, Dorit, Florian Waszak y Patrick Haggard. "Action selection and action awareness". Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung 73, n.º 4 (8 de abril de 2009): 602–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-009-0240-4.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Ofrecemos descuentos en todos los planes premium para autores cuyas obras están incluidas en selecciones literarias temáticas. ¡Contáctenos para obtener un código promocional único!

Pasar a la bibliografía