Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Worked examples'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Worked examples.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Llord-Ratcliffe, Kiera. "Worked Examples in Video Lessons to Reduce Cognitive Load." ScholarWorks, 2014. https://scholarworks.waldenu.edu/dissertations/91.
Full textShareghi, Najar Amir. "Evaluating the benefits of worked examples in a constraint-based tutor." Thesis, University of Canterbury. Computer Science and Software Engineering, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10092/9683.
Full textMcCann, Nicholas Francis. "Using Error Anticipation Exercises as an Instructional Intervention in the Algebra Classroom." Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2019. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/591666.
Full textPh.D.
Researchers and instructors have only recently embraced the role of errors as vehicles for learning in the algebra classroom. Studying a mixture of correct and incorrect worked examples has been shown to be beneficial relative to correct worked examples alone. This study examines the effectiveness of having students generate, or anticipate, errors another student might make. Five Algebra 1 sections at a suburban mid-Atlantic public high school participated amid an early equation-solving unit. During teacher-led instruction, all five sections examined 2-3 correct worked examples. The final example varied across conditions. One section received an additional correct worked example. Two sections examined an incorrect worked example. The remaining two sections engaged in an error anticipation exercise where the teacher wrote an equation on the board and asked the students to predict errors another student might make in solving. The study measured conceptual and procedural knowledge, encoding ability, and student-generated errors. Although no meaningful significant differences were found, students in the error anticipation condition saw no difference in performance in conceptual and procedural items versus those who examined incorrect worked examples. Analysis that combined the error anticipation and incorrect worked examples conditions showed that those students trended toward outperforming those who examined correct examples only on procedural items. These results support further examination of error anticipation as a worthwhile instructional activity.
Temple University--Theses
Kick-Samy, Mary. "Worked examples in teaching queries for searching academic databases." Thesis, Old Dominion University, 2013. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3571174.
Full textThe worked-example effect, an application of cognitive load theory, is a well-supported method of instruction for well-structured problems (Chandler and Sweller, 1991; Cooper and Sweller, 1987; Sweller and Cooper, 1985; Tuovinen & Sweller, 1999; Ward and Sweller, 1990). One limitation is expertise-reversal effect, where advanced students perform less well when exposed to worked examples than when exposed to traditional problem solving (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003; Kalyuga, Chandler, & Sweller, 1998; Kalyuga, Chandler, Tuovinen, & Sweller, 2001). A possible alternative to the worked-example approach is the fading example, designed to transition intermediate students to solving well-structured problems without assistance (Renkl, Atkinson & Grobe, 2004). This study showed that studying worked examples was more effect than solving problems or completing fading examples when learning to form search queries for library databases, an ill-structured problem-solving environment. In addition, participants within the worked-example group with low, intermediate and high levels of domain-specific knowledge achieved parity. Within the traditional problem-solving group, those with low domain-specific knowledge performed less well than those with high domain-specific knowledge.
Keywords: cognitive load theory, worked-example effect, fading examples, expertise-reversal effect, information literacy.
Song, Yulun. "An authoring and presentation environment for interactive worked examples." Thesis, University of Glasgow, 2015. http://theses.gla.ac.uk/6152/.
Full textMargulieux, Lauren Elizabeth. "Subgoal labeled instructional text and worked examples in STEM education." Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/51782.
Full textSianez, David M. "An Analysis of Successful and Unsuccessful Example Solutions to Enhance Open-Ended Technological Problem-Solving Efficiency Among Middle School Students." Diss., Virginia Tech, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/27792.
Full textPh. D.
Nagel, Karin Lynne. "Training visual pattern recognition : using worked examples to aid schema acquisition." Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 1993. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/28851.
Full textJohnson, Karen Jo. "THE EFFECTS OF A SIMULATION WITH WORKED EXAMPLES ON EPISODIC MEMORIES AND TROUBLESHOOTING IN MAINTENANCE TECHNICIAN STUDENTS." OpenSIUC, 2020. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/dissertations/1870.
Full textMargulieux, Lauren Elizabeth. "Using subgoal learning and self-explanation to improve programming education." Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/54985.
Full textAbdul, Rahman Siti Soraya. "Learning programming via worked-examples : the effects of cognitive load and learning styles." Thesis, University of Sussex, 2012. http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/38517/.
Full textMiller-Cotto, Dana. "The role of prior knowledge, executive function, and perceived cognitive load on the effectiveness of faded worked examples in geometry." Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2017. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/439545.
Full textPh.D.
Mathematics remains a subject many students fail to become competent in by the time they graduate from high school. Most students often require one on one, individualized tutoring to help them reach competence. That remains a challenge since most classrooms are understaffed and underfunded, frequently having only one teacher in a overpopulated classroom. One strategy that has been used to alleviate some of this over reliance on the teacher has been faded worked examples, or fading. Fading is the successive removal of the last steps in a series of problems until the student is solving problems completely on their own. The current study aimed to determine whether fading improves learning, and for whom. The goal was to compare fading with business as usual (control), worked examples with self-explanations, and fading with self-explanations. Specifically, I was interested in the following research questions: (1) Do the three experimental conditions differ in promoting posttest scores on surface area and volume? (2) Do the three experimental conditions differ in promoting conceptual knowledge and procedural knowledge of surface area and volume at posttest? and (3) When interaction terms are created between student profiles and conditions within regression analyses, which profiles explain significant variance in posttest scores? Repeated measures analysis of variance, principle axis factor analysis, and simple linear regressions were used to examine the differences between conditions at posttest, to create propensity scores, and to determine whether there were any interactions between propensity scores and conditions. Results indicated a significant effect of fading on posttest scores. A regression with propensity factors indicated that the fading conditions appeared to benefit low propensity students moreso than high propensity students. Findings are discussed in terms of educational implications and future research that can complement these findings to contribute to future research.
Temple University--Theses
Zirn, Lena [Verfasser], and Frank [Akademischer Betreuer] Fischer. "Facilitating skill acquisition with video-based modeling worked examples / Lena Zirn. Betreuer: Frank Fischer." München : Universitätsbibliothek der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, 2015. http://d-nb.info/1079140255/34.
Full textOyer, Melissa Heidi. "Investigating Gender Differences in Achievement Goal Orientation in Example-Based Algebra Learning." Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2014. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/253596.
Full textPh.D.
This study was designed to compare the effects of the use of worked examples and self-explanation on motivation for male and female students. More specifically, the present study examines whether there are differences between males and females with regards to their achievement goals and if gender plays a role in how students respond to questions about their motivation in the presence of other male or female students. Comparisons of student responses on Achievement Goal Questionnaire-Revised (AGQ-R) and the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scales (PALS) were also conducted. Participants were 147 seventh-, eighth- and ninth-grade non-honors Algebra I students (82 girls and 65 boys) from three schools and eight classrooms within the same school district on the east cost of the United States of America. Results replicated the finding that females have more mastery goals than males, however no gender differences were found for either performance. In addition, it appears that students respond differently to some questions about their motivation in the presence of other male or female students. Finally, the AGQ-R and the PALS appear to be consistent representations of students' achievement goals.
Temple University--Theses
Ginns, Paul William Education Faculty of Arts & Social Sciences UNSW. "When imagining instructions is effective." Awarded by:University of New South Wales. School of Education, 2002. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/18634.
Full textHsiao, E.-Ling. "The Effectiveness of Worked Examples Associated with Presentation Format and Prior Knowledge: A Web-based Experiment." Ohio : Ohio University, 2010. http://www.ohiolink.edu/etd/view.cgi?ohiou1267576255.
Full textDoherty, Christina Barbieri. "THE EFFECTS OF ERROR REFLECTION AND PERCEIVED FUNCTIONALITY OF ERRORS ON MIDDLE SCHOOL STUDENTS’ ALGEBRA LEARNING AND SENSE OF BELONGING TO MATHEMATICS." Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2015. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/335141.
Full textPh.D.
The current study assessed an error reflection intervention on Algebra I students’ conceptual and procedural knowledge and sense of belonging to mathematics. Also of interest was whether perceptions of the functionality of errors mediated the effect of condition on learning and sense of belonging to mathematics. Middle school students (N = 207) were randomly assigned within classroom to one of four conditions: 1) a Problem-Solving Control group, 2) a Correct Examples Control group, 3) a Correct Examples Error Reflection condition that promoted reflection on hypothetical errors through self-explanation prompts, or 4) an Incorrect Examples Error Reflection condition that promoted reflection on displayed errors within the example through self-explanation prompts. Conceptual and procedural knowledge, sense of belonging to mathematics and perceived functionality of errors were measured pre- and post-intervention. After controlling for unanticipated clustering effects, results suggest that reflecting on and explaining errors within a worked examples intervention is just as effective at promoting learning as traditional problem solving alone or working with traditional correct worked examples and written self-explanation prompts. Students’ sense of belonging to mathematics or perceived functionality of errors for learning were high at the start of the study and remained so throughout the intervention. Perceptions of the functionality of errors were unrelated to learning and sense of belonging to mathematics. The limited size of the minority population in the sample did not allow for exploration of differential effects of condition for underrepresented minority (URM) students. However, these students reported lower feelings of belonging to mathematics than non-URM students. Implications for theory and practice are discussed.
Temple University--Theses
Brooks, Christopher Darren. "Effects of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples and prior knowledge on learner problem solving and attitude a study in the domain of microeconomics /." Tallahassee, Fla. : Florida State University, 2009. http://etd.lib.fsu.edu/theses/available/etd-10222009-202302/.
Full textAdvisor: A. Aubteen Darabi, Florida State University, College of Education, Dept. of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems. Title and description from dissertation home page (viewed May 7, 2010). Document formatted into pages; contains xi, 183 pages. Includes bibliographical references.
Wylie, Ruth. "Examining the Generality of Self-Explanation." Research Showcase @ CMU, 2011. http://repository.cmu.edu/dissertations/99.
Full textFriefeldt, William, and Philip Gullberg. "Nätcoaching som ett komplement till lösningsförslag : En undersökning av en ny digital lärmiljö genom undersökande relation." Thesis, KTH, Skolan för industriell teknik och management (ITM), 2019. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:kth:diva-263949.
Full textThe digital and technical development of the world have been large-scale and people’s day-today life has changed immensely the last century. In contrast, the Swedish school system has not been seeing the same rapid technical development. As students’ results in mathematics are declining an approach to battle this trend can be to apply this technological development on students’ studies in mathematics. Therefore, this thesis sought to ease students’ mathematics studies by creating a digital learning environment. By combining workedexamples with online coaching the environment was created through the platforms Mathleaks and TalkMath. Then examine how online one-to-one coaching works in this environment. It also sought to analyse how students perceive this teaching environment and if it can be used as a complement to worked-examples. The environment was created based on pedagogical theories such as Vygotsky's sociocultural perspective, coach theories, synchronous communication as well as theory around one-to-one online coaching. Two methods were used to gather data. First, the conversations were automatically stored in a database. Secondly, a survey was used to gather the students’ opinion of the teaching environment. The framework Relationship of Inquiry was used to analyse all conversations between students and coaches. Students were chosen from an upper secondary school in the ages of 15-18 where one of the authors worked. Results indicated that the teaching environment that was created was a good complement to worked-examples. The environment was shown to help student’s knowledge progression. This shows that digital teaching environments can be a valuable addition for students when studying mathematics.
Friefeldt, William, and Philip Gullberg. "Nätcoaching som ett komplement till lösningsförslag : En undersökning av en ny digital lärmiljö genomundersökande relation." Thesis, KTH, Lärande, 2019. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:kth:diva-262220.
Full textThe digital and technical development of the world have been large-scale and people’s day-to day life has changed immensely the last century. In contrast, the Swedish school system has not been seeing the same rapid technical development. As students’ results in mathematic sare declining an approach to battle this trend can be to apply this technological development on students’ studies in mathematics. Therefore, this thesis sought to ease students’ mathematics studies by creating a digital learning environment. By combining worked examples with online coaching the environment was created through the platforms Mathleaks and TalkMath. Then examine how online one-to-one coaching works in this environment. It also sought to analyse how students perceive this teaching environment and if it can be usedas a complement to worked-examples. The environment was created based on pedagogical theories such as Vygotsky's sociocultural perspective, coach theories, synchronous communication as well as theory around one-to-one online coaching. Two methods were used to gather data. First, the conversations were automatically stored in a database. Secondly, a survey was used to gather the students’ opinion of the teaching environment. The framework Relationship of Inquiry was used to analyse all conversations between students and coaches. Students were chosen from an upper secondary school in the ages of 15-18 where one of the authors worked. Results indicated that the teaching environment that was created was a good complement to worked-examples. The environment was shown to help student’s knowledge progression. This shows that digital teaching environments can be a valuable addition for students when studying mathematics.
Maxwell, Elizabeth Anne Education Faculty of Arts & Social Sciences UNSW. "An investigation of appropriate instructional design to match the ability of the learner." Publisher:University of New South Wales. Education, 2008. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/40887.
Full textSolé, Llussà Anna. "Disseny i avaluació d’una estratègia didàctica basada en l’ús dels exemples en vídeo per promoure la indagació científica a educació primària." Doctoral thesis, Universitat de Lleida, 2020. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/668715.
Full textLa indagación científica es una metodología ampliamente aceptada para promover las habilidades científicas de los estudiantes. Sin embargo, aunque su implementación ha aportado buenos resultados, los soportes son necesarios para desarrollar actividades indagadoras en estudiantes noveles. En el presente trabajo se diseña, se implementa y se evalúa la introducción de una estrategia didáctica basada en el uso de los ejemplos en vídeo como soporte del proceso de indagación en las aulas de educación primaria. La implementación de esta estrategia se ha abordado a partir de dos estudios de caso y un estudio cuasi experimental y se ha evaluado a través de un método cuantitativo. Los resultados confirman que la estrategia didáctica diseñada contribuye a estructurar y sistematizar el proceso de indagación de los estudiantes. Esta tesis aporta una primera aproximación en la importancia de usar estas guías en el aula de educación primaria, dando soporte a docentes, investigadores y otros profesionales del ámbito educativo en el diseño, implementación y evaluación de actividades indagadoras.
Scientific inquiry is a methodology widely accepted to promote students’ science process skills. Although it provides good results, guides are needed to support the development of inquiry activities in novel students. In the present work, a didactic strategy based on video worked examples for guiding the inquiry processes is design, implement and evaluate in elementary education classroom. The implementation of this methodology has been addressed through two case studies and a quasi-experimental study, and has been evaluated using a quantitative method. The results indicate that the didactic strategy provides students a structure and systematization of the inquiry process. This thesis provides a first approach to the importance of using these guides in the elementary classrooms, supporting teachers, researchers and other educational professionals in the design, implementation and evaluation of inquiry activities.
Aleahmad, Turadg. "Improving Students’ Study Practices Through the Principled Design of Research Probes." Research Showcase @ CMU, 2012. http://repository.cmu.edu/dissertations/129.
Full textRetnowati, Endah Education Faculty of Arts & Social Sciences UNSW. "The effectiveness of a worked example approach in group work settings during mathematics learning." Awarded by:University of New South Wales. Education, 2008. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/40827.
Full textNasah, Angelique. "EXAMINING INSTANT MESSAGING IMPACT ON LEARNING USING AN INTEGRATED WORKED-EXAMPLE FORMAT." Doctoral diss., University of Central Florida, 2008. http://digital.library.ucf.edu/cdm/ref/collection/ETD/id/3094.
Full textPh.D.
Department of Educational Research, Technology and Leadership
Education
Education PhD
Nehring, Wendy M. "Educating Nurses – Examples of Change at ETSU." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2010. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/6731.
Full textFerraz, Susana Carla de Souza. "A função dos exemplos na antropologia de um ponto de vista pragmático." Universidade de São Paulo, 2015. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/8/8133/tde-09102015-131102/.
Full textThe works often regarded as the most relevant to the study of Kant\'s philosophy are the three critical presenting the rigor of philosophical thought and mark a change of perspective in modern thought, pointing limits and new horizons to human knowledge. However, concurrently with the development of these works find others that are also important for understanding the Kantian philosophical project as a whole. Stands out in this research project Anthropology from a pragmatic point of view and its relevance to the study of the formulation of the moral sense from the observation of human actions and the possibility of identifying and training the \"world citizen\" to Kant.
Hoover-Thompson, Alysia, Catherine Jones-Hazledine, and Jodi Polaha. "Overcoming Rural Service Delivery Barriers: Three Examples in Integrated Care." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2013. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/6593.
Full textHacking, Damian. "Setting priorities in health research using the World Health Organisation model: Development of a quantitative methodology using Tuberculosis in South Africa as a worked example." Master's thesis, University of Cape Town, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/11427/22635.
Full textBeinhoff, Andreas. "Developer usability testing : A real world example." Thesis, Uppsala universitet, Institutionen för informatik och media, 2010. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-132227.
Full textTollison, Christopher Scott. "The effect of a worked example on online debate quality in an information systems course." Diss., Mississippi State : Mississippi State University, 2009. http://library.msstate.edu/etd/show.asp?etd=etd-07012009-171819.
Full textLewis, David. "The Acquisition of Procedural Skills: An Analysis of the Worked-Example Effect Using Animated Demonstrations." [Tampa, Fla] : University of South Florida, 2008. http://purl.fcla.edu/usf/dc/et/SFE0002661.
Full textNiemeyer, Erik. "Lösningsförslag i den svenska skolan : Påverkar lösningsförslag elevers lärande?" Thesis, KTH, Skolan för teknikvetenskaplig kommunikation och lärande (ECE), 2016. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:kth:diva-194505.
Full textTo solve mathematical tasks with the help of example solutions can be seen as a way for learners to apply less effort and therefore learn less or perhaps learn in undesirable ways. However, existing international research has indicated that classrooms where pupils mainly study worked examples has had a significant effect on their ability to solve tasks. This method is called "the worked example effect" and has gained increasing support over time. It is based on Sweller’s cognitive load theory. According to his and others’ research the working memory can be divided into three different types. These three, intrinsic cognitive load, germane cognitive load and extraneous cognitive load, are additive and support learning differently. Using worked examples has been shown to reduce the amount of extraneous cognitive load and instead increase the amount of germane cognitive load. Germane cognitive load has in turn been shown to correlate to how well pupils are able to solve mathematical tasks. This thesis aims to study the worked example effect in relation to learning outcome in Swedish secondary school. Three schools took part in the study, of which one only participated in a pilot study. A total of 93 pupils participated, all of them in the science program. Results of the national test for the four latest courses were used in this thesis. The pupils were also asked to fill out a survey with the aim to gather information as to how they used the worked examples when studying. The results of the study are ambiguous. More than 75% of the pupils claim to have changed how they study, but whether their learning has improved, measured in grades, is unclear. The study showed a negative result for one course but a positive for another. These different results originated from different schools. The results of the study may indicate that the worked example effect differs depending on which type of math is being taught. Another important factor is how the worked examples are used by the pupils, something this study has not examined.
Canavan, Caroline. "Using real world data to generate health economic models : a worked example assessing the cost-effectiveness of referral to gastroenterology for irritable bowel syndrome in the UK." Thesis, University of Nottingham, 2016. http://eprints.nottingham.ac.uk/32666/.
Full textWärmedal, Björn. "Explaining kernel space with a real world example." Thesis, Umeå universitet, Institutionen för datavetenskap, 2012. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-58069.
Full textBlevins, Leia, James J. Fox, and R. Leppert. "Primary Level PBS: Two Examples of Successful Implementation and Sustainability in Rural Schools." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2013. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/154.
Full textBower, M., and A. Lynn Williams. "The Three E's of Treatment Efficacy: A Phonological Case Example." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 1998. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/2107.
Full textBlevins, Leia, and James J. Fox. "Primary Level-School-Wide Positive Behavior Support: Two Examples of Successful Implementation and Sustainability in Rural Schools." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2014. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/150.
Full textHung, Chun-Ling, and 洪君羚. "Integrating Problem Posing and Worked Examples as a Means for Improving Learning." Thesis, 2013. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/49891599579363560806.
Full text國立彰化師範大學
工業教育與技術學系
101
Both problem posing and worked examples have developed research lines on different ways to improve learning. This dissertation reviewed the effects of both problem posing and worked examples and found that it was possible to find ways to combine worked examples and problem posing to improve learning. The dissertation has developed two studies: The first study was designed to investigate the effects of worked examples underpinning problem posing. The second study was designed to explore the effects of problem posing as a supplement to self-explanation for studying worked examples. The first study hypothesized that worked examples may have benefits for supporting problem posing. A quasi-experiment design was conducted. The results showed that for problem posing skills, those in the experimental group, who generated problems with the additional supports of worked examples, performed better than those in the control group, who only generated problems without support. The significant effects were revealed particularly on posing more non-digressive and complex problems, particularly for analytical problems referring to only a learning concept or a formula involved in a problem. In the second study, problem posing was suggested to induce learners’ self-explanations as a result of the process of self-questioning and self-answering. A quasi-experiment design was conducted to investigate the effects of problem posing for studying worked examples on problem-solving skills, self-efficacy, and mental effort. The results revealed that students who conducted self-explanation and problem posing performed significantly better on problem solving skills in median-, and far-transfer problems, and higher self-efficacy than students who only conducted self-explanation in mind. The results also identified a significant mediation effect for self-efficacy that mediated between problem posing and problem solving ability. We called this the problem posing supplement self-explanation effect referring to the additional benefits of problem posing for studying worked examples. To sum up, this dissertation showed that integrating worked examples into problem posing developed better problem posing skills. Also, incorporating problem posing as a supplement to self-explanation for studying worked examples revealed better problem solving abilities and self-efficacy. Combined with the above results, problem posing and worked examples were mutually benefited from each other.
Wu, I.-Chin, and 吳易親. "The Effect of Adaptive Worked Examples on Students’ Learning Altitude and Cognitive Load." Thesis, 2014. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/9h2dx8.
Full text國立臺北教育大學
數學暨資訊教育學系(含數學教育碩士班)
102
Abstract According to cognitive load theory, learning with worked examples can result in better and more efficient learning than learning through problem solving, resulting in the worked example effect. Studies on worked example effect also suggest that worked examples may be redundant and hinder performance when learners have acquired sufficient schema to solve the problem, resulting in the expertise reversal effect (Sweller, Ayres, & Kalyuga,2011).The purpose of this study was to investigate the consequences of “learning performance”, “learning timeand efficiency”and “cognitive load and motivation” caused by different “instructional mode”, based on the perspective of cognitive load. The instructional mode used here include “Adaptive worked examples”, “problem solving-worked example(P-W)”and “worked example-problem solving (W-P)”. The participants were eighty-eight Grade 4 students in an elementary school in Taipei, Taiwan. This content was new to them. They were randomly assigned to one of the experimental conditions. The material was presented as multimedia material and divided into two lessons. The participants learned the material in a computer room with one lesson a week.At the end of the lesson, there was an immediate test. A delayed posttest was conducted one week after the participants had completed the two lessons.Performance was measured in terms of test performance (combined immediate posttest and delayed posttest) and time (combined learning time and test time).The questionnaires were given after each learning activity lesson and test. The major results were as follows. 1. Learning performance:Instructional mode did not affect the results in the post- test, nor was the delayed posttest. 2. Learning time and efficiency:There was a significant difference between P-W group and the adaptivegroup in lesson 1.The P-W group spent more 80.1seconds than the the adaptive group.Others did not lead to significant difference in the learning time. In learning efficiency,there was also significant differencesbetween P-W and the adaptivegroup in lesson 1. The adaptive group more than the P-W group. 3. Cognitive load and motivation:There was a significant difference between groups in effort invested.The adaptive group was significantly higher than P-W groupin lesson 2.In mental efficiency, the adaptive group lessthan the PW group in lesson 2.
YEO, LIAN-MING, and 楊凌梅. "The Cognitive Effect of Tracing Gesture in the Learning from Mathematics Worked Examples." Thesis, 2019. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/7beng9.
Full text國立中正大學
認知科學博士學位學程
107
Previous studies have suggested that embodied pedagogy through the use of tracing gestures in the worked example-based instruction may enhance the learning by reducing cognitive load. The present study attempted to replicate the previous results and further examined the boundary condition of tracing gesture whether its cognitive effect is merely comparable with other attention-guiding means, i.e., textual attention cueing, in two different learning tasks in nature (Experiment 1 and Experiment 2). In addition, the present study also investigated the individual differences in tracing effect in relation to the learners’ working-memory capacity (Experiment 3). Across three experiments, Year 6 students aged between 11 and 13 years old were recruited. In the first two experiments, the participants were asked to study worked examples on angles relationships involving parallel lines (Experiment 1) and laws of indices (Experiment 2) either without tracing instruction, with textual attention cueing instruction, or tracing instruction. The tracing group outperformed the other two groups on a subsequent test and reported lower levels of test difficulty (i.e. the reduced of intrinsic cognitive load) in Experiment 1 but not in Experiment 2, suggesting that the facilitation effect of tracing gesture was beyond guiding attention in the learning of highly visuo-spatial worked examples but not in the materials with fewer visuo-spatial components. Experiment 3 used the materials on angle relationships involving parallel lines to test whether participants with low working-memory capacity would benefit more than participants with high working-memory capacity when using tracing gesture in the learning of visuospatial-based worked examples. The results showed comparable learning outcomes (i.e. the test performance and ratings of test item difficulty) between the low- and high-capacity individuals in the tracing condition and also in the non-tracing condition, suggesting that the facilitation effect of tracing gesture was not affected by the individuals’ working-memory capacity. Taken together, the present findings suggested that tracing gesture offers a simple yet effective embodied technique that may further enhances the worked example-based learning through its cognitive effect by reducing working memory load specifically on the task involved with spatial information. The limitations and suggestions for future research are discussed.
Lei, Kin Hang, and 李健恆. "The Effects of Worked-out Examples with Different Strategies on Comprehending Geometry Proof." Thesis, 2011. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/07662968348828079292.
Full text國立臺灣師範大學
數學系
100
Geometry proof is an important tool for the development of the mathematical thinking and the learning deductive reasoning; nevertheless, geometry proof is also one of the learning difficulties for students. Worked-out example is a fundamental approach to demonstrate mathematical thinking. Thus, the topic of finding suitable learning strategies in order to understand geometry proof is worth to discuss. The effects of comprehending geometry proof are detected under using different reading learning modes. Proofs are showed with a computer setting. The reading learning modes are formed by worked-out examples with practices or metacognition questions. The intercept theorem (or Thales' theorem) is used as the presenting content of worked-out examples. 254 eighth grade students who have not learned deductive proof are chosen for this research. Reading comprehension test is used to examine students’ understanding and conservation of learning effects. Students also need to fill out the rating-scale measurement of cognitive load. Furthermore, we investigate the relationship between learning process and comprehending geometry proof from students’ writing files of responding questions. The results show that practice with similar structure of worked-out example is helpful for the instant understanding for students; however, students are affected by the prototype of worked-out examples which tend to use copy-and-adapt strategy for doing practices. On the other hand, learning task combined with metacognition questions are more difficult than practices; however, metacognition questions reflect the students’ level of understanding and provide a better conservation. Hence, metacognition question is an ideal tool which is helpful for the reflection in student learning. It is suggested that proper pair of the metacognition question with the advantage of practices may support students to understand the content of deductive proof.
Liu, Chen Yun, and 劉禎芸. "Application of Worked-out Examples in the Fraction Division Classroom for 5th Graders." Thesis, 2011. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/46039108492301157708.
Full text國立臺灣師範大學
教育心理與輔導學系
100
In this study, three different prototypes of “common denominator”, “reciprocal multiplication” , and “integration” as the worked-out examples were applied to explore the learning effect on conceptual knowledge and procedural knowledge of fraction division, especially that on the students with low prior knowledge. The pilot study pre-examined 115 5th graders and 108 6th graders via the examination of prior knowledge and conceptual knowledge. The pilot study result shows lower difficulties in accordance with better discrimination in most of the items. The analysis of internal consistency reliability fitted the goodness; criterion-related validity was qualified for the significant correlation with the mathematics-achievement scores. Therefore, the study only revised minor part of the items. Other items were expected to be adopted in the formal examination. The valid samples in the formal examination were 148, including 5th graders sampling from elementary schools in Taipei and Taoyuan. The participants divided by classes as a unit were designed to three conditions with different worked-out-example teaching instruction. The examination of prior knowledge was proceeded before the instruction. The worked-out-example instruction was then proceeded in the second (integrated group) or third (the other groups). The students were divided into heterogeneous group and they could learn via discussing the examples as well as solving the questions. The examination of fraction division with procedural knowledge and conceptual knowledge were provided after the worked-out examples. The results exhibited the anticipated learning effects on the students with 80% and 90% correct rate via learning conceptual knowledge and procedural knowledge of fraction division respectively. The difference between three groups was not significant according to ANCOVA analysis since the performance of three groups were fine simultaneously. The effect resulting from different groups occurred in the students with low prior knowledge. The effects of learning conceptual knowledge in integrated group were better than those in the reciprocal multiplication group. The effects of learning procedural knowledge in integrate group or reciprocal multiplication group were better than those in the common denominator group. In conclusion, the prototype of worked-out examples in integrated group demonstrated the best efficiency and benefit for the students with low prior knowledge. Additionally, the acts of the students in class, the restriction of the research, and the advice of instruction were all described and discussed in this article.
Fang, Ryh-Sheng, and 方日升. "A Study of the Effects of Worked Examples on Triangle Geometry for Junior High Students." Thesis, 2009. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/94122239828756738462.
Full text淡江大學
教育科技學系碩士在職專班
97
The main purpose of this study, is his second grades of the second half of the triangular geometric module in order to worked examples of improved version (referred to as the modified group) and traditional-style version-based Taipei-Taipei-Keelung (referred to as the traditional group) of two different teaching methods , in the implementation of five lessons, each lesson 45 minutes after the students achievements and attitudes of their advantages and disadvantages compared. By researchers in Taipei County to teach in his second grades of a total of six classes of 201 students, the experimental design adopted pre-test & post-test quasi-experimental design study. And Mathematics at the end of the first semester results, respectively, will improve the group and traditional group is divided into well-differentiated, and ill-differentiated groups. To explore the improved group and the traditional group of students learning through two different units in the triangular geometry to study the effectiveness of changes to 2 × 2 ANCOVA students in each group the difference. The results of the data processing by statistical analysis, to obtain the following major findings: 1.Geometric modules for math, geometry math learning achievement of students: 1-1Improved group and traditional group learning achievement of all students, no significant differences between the two groups. 1-2Improved group and the traditional group of students of low academic achievement group, improvement was superior to the traditional group. 1-3Improved group and the traditional group of students of high academic achievement group, improvement was superior to the traditional group. 2.Geometric modules for math, students attitude towards learning mathematics geometry: 2-1Improved group and the traditional group of students learning attitude, improvement was superior to the traditional group. 2-2Improved group and the traditional group of the low group of students learning attitude, there was no significant difference between the two groups. 2-3Improved group and the traditional group of well-differentiated group of students learning attitude, there was no significant difference between the two groups.
(6996329), Garrett M. O'Day. "Improving Problem Solving with Retrieval-Based Learning." Thesis, 2019.
Find full textRecent research asserts that the mnemonic benefits gained from retrieval-based learning vanish for complex materials. Subsequently, it is recommended that students study worked examples when learning about complex, problem-centered tasks. The experiments that have evaluated the effectiveness of studying worked examples tend to overlook the mental processing that students engage in when completing retrieval-based learning activities. In contrast, theories of transfer-appropriate processing emphasize the importance of compatibility between the cognitive processing required by the test and the cognitive processing that is activated during learning. For learners to achieve optimal test performance, according to transfer-appropriate processing, they need to study in such a way that they are engaging in the same mental processing that will be required of them when tested. This idea was used to generate testable predictions that compete against the claim that the retrieval practice effect disappears for complex materials, and these competing predictions were evaluated in three experiments that required students to learn about the Poisson probability distribution.
In Experiment 1, students learned the general procedure for how to solve these problems by either repeatedly recalling the procedural steps or by simply studying them. The retrieval practice condition produced better memory for the procedure on an immediate test compared to the study only condition. In Experiment 2, students engaged in the same learning activities as Experiment 1, but the test focused on their problem- solving ability. Students who practiced retrieval of the procedural steps experienced no benefit on the problem-solving test compared to the study only condition. In Experiment 3, students learned to solve Poisson probability problems by studying four worked examples, by studying one worked example and solving three practice problems, or by studying one worked example and solving three practice problems with feedback. Students were tested on their problem-solving ability one week later. The problem- solving learning activities outperformed the worked example condition on the final problem-solving test. Taken together, the results demonstrate a pronounced retrieval practice effect but only when the retrieval-based learning activities necessitated the same mental processing that was required during the final assessment, providing support for the transfer-appropriate processing account.
Hsu, Wen-Chin, and 許文清. "The Effect of Instructional Orders of Worked Examples On Students’ Learning Area Covering and Cognitive Load." Thesis, 2013. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/evfwue.
Full text國立臺北教育大學
數學暨資訊教育學系(含數學教育碩士班)
101
The purpose of this study was to investigate the consequences of “learning achievement” and “cognitive load” caused by different “instructional order”, based on the perspective of cognitive load. The instructional orders used here including “worked example-practice (W-P)” and “problem solving-worked example(P-W)”. This study adopted experimental design, focusing on the “area covering” in the geometry of elementary schools. The teaching material in this study was designed by the instructional principles of cognitive load, especially the principles of worked-examples. The participants were 104 four graders of a elementary school. They were randomly devided into “worked example-practice (W-P)” and “problem solving-worked example(P-W)” group. Research instruments included two multimedia lesssons and scales of cognitive load. A pretest was given before deviding them into such two groups. Then two multimedia lesssons were given, followed by a post-test a week later, and a delay post-test after another week. Questionnaires were given after each learning activity lesson and test. The results were: 1. The learning achievement of P-W group was much better than W-P group in both the post-test and delay post-test. 2. In the aspect of cognitive load, group P-W had a significant difference with group W-P in terms of the first lesson of multimedia lessons and delay post-test, and group P-W worked harder than group W-P during the learning process. 3. In the area covering questions, the students did better in squares than in triangles; they did best in 4 cm2 squares; worst in 0.5 cm2 triangles; the highest average passing rate of unit was 1/4; the type of questions with the best passing rate was 4 cm2 squares covered by triangles. 4. According to the questionnaires, most students were interested in area covering conducted by multimedia lessons, it promoted their learing motives.
陳美秀. "Presentation and Explanation Modes in Worked-Examples: Examining their Effects on Third Graders’ Learning of Area Concepts." Thesis, 2011. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/43654483898571213494.
Full text佛光大學
學習與數位科技學系
99
The purpose of this research was to investigate the effects of using different presentation and explanation modes in multimedia worked examples on students’ learning of mathematic area concepts. Sixty-five third graders from a public school in Yilan County were randomly assigned to one of the four groups in this experiment that used a 2 by 2 factorial design. The independent variables in the study were presentation (i.e., dynamic vs. static) and explanation (i.e., self vs. instructional) modes. The dependent variables in the study involved posttest scores, practice scores, learning time, as well as cognitive load and motivation. The results of the study were as follows. 1. Different presentation and explanation modes would not affect students’ posttest scores. 2. Different presentation and explanation modes would not affect students’ practice performance. 3. Different presentation modes would not affect the amount of time students spent on learning the materials. Comparing to instructional explanation devices, using self explanation devices in multimedia would increase the amount of time students spent on learning the materials. 4. Different presentation modes would not affect students’ cognitive load and their motivation levels. In addition, different explanation modes would not affect students’ perceived effort needed, the amount of effort they devoted to the materials, their willingness to read, and their confidence levels in understanding the materials. Comparing with instructional explanation devices, self explanation devices, however, would increase students’ perceived task difficulty. 5. Perceived task difficulty and effort needed were positively correlated. Reading willingness and confidence level were positively correlated. Additionally, perceived task difficulty correlated negatively with reading willingness and with confidence level. Effort needed correlated negatively with reading willingness and with confidence level. Finally, effort devoted and reading willingness were positively correlated. Recommendations regarding ways to improve the design of this study as well as ways to use presentation (i.e., dynamic vs. static) and explanation (i.e., self vs. instructional) modes in multimedia worked examples were given. Future research directions were also proposed.
Huang, Xiaoxia. "The effect of different types of worked examples on student learning and transfer of a problem-solving task." 2007. http://etd.lib.fsu.edu/theses/available/etd-11132007-001928.
Full textAdvisor: Robert Reiser, Florida State University, College of Education, Dept. of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems. Title and description from dissertation home page (viewed Mar. 19, 2008). Document formatted into pages; contains x, 147 pages. Includes bibliographical references.
黃俊瑋. "Natural Language Worked-Examples for Elementary Area Concept Learning:Examining Their Impacts on Learning Effectiveness, Cognitive Load and Motivation." Thesis, 2012. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/50487596223461714588.
Full text佛光大學
學習與數位科技學系
100
The purpose of this study was to investigate how problem statement types in the learning materials affected elementary school children’s performance in learning area concept and measurement, their perception of cognitive load and motivation. Two types of problem statements were examined: mathematic language and natural language (which refers to the use of daily life words or words that are related to daily life items instead of mathematic terms). A between-subject experimental study was used in this study. The content was area measurement (covering an area using different units). The design of the material was based on the instructional design principles proposed by the cognitive load theory, particularly the worked-example principle. The participants were eighty-three third graders from a public school in Yilan County. They were randomly assigned to one of the two groups ("natural language" and "mathematic language"). The participants first took a pretest, followed by two learning and immediate posttest sessions, and a one-week delayed posttest. The participants’ time-on-task and test performance was measured. Their perception of cognitive load and motivation after each learning session and the delayed posttest were also measured. Instructional efficiency, including training efficiency and mental efficiency, was calculated using the formula proposed by van Gog and Paas (2008). The results showed that the types of problem statements did not have a significant influence on the effectiveness of learning, but it had a significant influence on the time needed to complete the task. The "natural language" group spent more time to complete the learning task than the "mathematic language" group. In the aspect of cognitive load and motivation, the result showed that "natural language" group reported a lower level of confidence in the immediate posttest , but a higher level of interest in the delayed posttest than the "mathematic language" group. When instructional efficiency was calculated, the results showed that the two types of problem statements did not lead to a significant difference in either training efficiency or mental efficiency in this study.