Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Vocalization'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Vocalization.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Geest, Trijntje Teixeira de Lima van den. "Vocalization in saxophone class." Master's thesis, Universidade de Aveiro, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/10773/15343.
Full textInitiating saxophone students often demonstrate difficulties in playing with a consistent sound. In addition, many initiating music students demonstrate difficulties with imagining the music before they play it. Consequently, their performances lack musical interpretation. The aim of this educational project is to develop a vocalization method in order to help initiating saxophone students to improve their tone quality and the use of articulation and dynamics, by singing and playing with vowels and consonants.
Alunos iniciantes de saxofone demonstram por vezes dificuldade na aquisição de um timbre consistente. Associada a esta dificuldade observa-se também uma falta de imaginação musical o que pode ter uma influência direta na performance. O objetivo deste projeto educacional é o desenvolvimento de um método que pretende ajudar alunos de saxofone a enriquecer o seu timbre e a melhorar o seu uso da articulação e das dinâmicas através de exercícios cantados com vogais e consoantes.
Hazlett, Emily G. "Vocalization processing across amygdalar populations." Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1563885605313261.
Full textFerreira, Marna. "Evolutionary implications of variation in the calling song of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus De Geer (Orthoptera : Gryllidae)." Diss., University of Pretoria, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/2263/23394.
Full textSoendjoto, Mochamad Arief. "Vocalization behavior of captive loggerhead shrikes (Lanius ludovicianus excubitorides)." Thesis, McGill University, 1995. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=23427.
Full textCoveyduck, Susan. "Vocalization and its effect on the intonation of a beginning instrumentalist." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1998. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/tape15/PQDD_0004/MQ34934.pdf.
Full textBoisseau, Oliver, and n/a. "The acoustic behaviour of resident bottlenose dolphins in Fiordland, New Zealand." University of Otago. Department of Marine Science, 2005. http://adt.otago.ac.nz./public/adt-NZDU20060728.140213.
Full textMaiwald, Patrick [Verfasser]. "The vocalization of semivowels in medieval English : a quantitative study / Patrick Maiwald." Gießen : Universitätsbibliothek, 2017. http://d-nb.info/1140734989/34.
Full textRodrigues, Vanessa Filipa Santos. "Vocalization repertoires variation of Red-billed Chough (Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax) population in Portugal." Master's thesis, Universidade de Aveiro, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/10773/21457.
Full textEcological changes in the habitats, induced by climate change or the new paradigms of land use, have had enormous effects in the distribution of species, from the fragmentation of the populations to changes on the genome and/or their behavior. In the case of birds, these constraints may cause changes in vocal repertoires among isolated populations. It was recently confirmed that the two last populations of Red-billed Chough (Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax) resident in Portugal (populations of the Serra d'Aire e Candeeiros and Sagres) have an apparent gene-flow restriction, suggesting an isolation that is not explained only by the geographical distance that separates them. In fact, the species has a high dispersion capacity which can not itself justify this restriction of genes between these two populations. Being the species a corvid with a high cognitive capacity and gregarious species, the communication between individuals becomes a vital point to its survival. This work intends to investigate if the gene-flow restriction is due to a social segregation that can be reflected in the variations of the vocal repertoire between the two populations. The aim of this study was to describe and try to understand the main differences between the two populations, as well as possible variations among individuals of the same population (in this case, only in Serra d'Aire e Candeeiros). The results showed that the two studied populations have different repertoires, were described five call types from the population in the Serra d'Aire e Candeeiros and only four in the population of Sagres, thus not existing an evident common vocal repertoire between both populations. It was verified that among the populations the greatest differences are in the duration, maximum frequency and final frequency, where the population from Sagres has smaller durations and maximum frequencies and a higher final frequency than the population from Serra d'Aire e Candeeiros. In the case of the differences between individuals analysed at the intra-population level, these showed individual variations, namely peak frequency and ascending time. Overall, the results seem to corroborate the thesis that the genetic isolation of these populations can be attributed to communication and social barriers.
As mudanças no estado ecológico dos habitats, induzidas por alterações climáticas e/ou por novos paradigmas dos usos do solo, têm tido inúmeras consequências na distribuição das espécies, desde a fragmentação das populações, passando por mudanças no genoma e no seu comportamento. No caso das aves, estes constrangimentos poderão traduzir-se em mudanças nos repertórios vocais entre populações isoladas. Recentemente, confirmou-se que as duas últimas populações de Gralha de Bico Vermelho (Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax) residentes em Portugal (populações da Serra d’Aire e Candeeiros e de Sagres) apresentam uma aparente restrição de fluxo genético entre si, sugerindo um isolamento que não se explica apenas pela distância geográfica que as separa. De facto, a espécie apresenta uma elevada capacidade de dispersão o que não pode, só por si, justificar esta restrição de genes. Sendo a espécie um corvídeo com uma elevada capacidade cognitiva e gregária a comunicação entre indivíduos torna-se vital à sua sobrevivência. Este trabalho pretende investigar se a restrição do fluxo genético se deve a uma segregação social que pode refletir-se nas variações do repertório vocal entre as duas populações. Com este intuito, o presente estudo teve como objetivo descrever e tentar compreender quais as principais diferenças entre as duas populações, bem como possíveis variações entre indivíduos de uma mesma população (neste último caso apenas na Serra d’Aire e Candeeiros). Os resultados demonstraram que as duas populações estudadas exibiram repertórios diferentes, com a existência de cinco tipos de vocalizações para a população da Serra na Serra d’Aire e Candeeiros e apenas quatro na população de Sagres, não havendo uma partilha evidente de entre tipos de vocalização comuns a ambas as populações. Verificou-se que entre as populações as maiores diferenças estão na duração, frequência máxima e frequência final, sendo que na população de Sagres as vocalizações têm durações e frequências máximas menores e a frequência final maior que as da população de Serra d’Aire e Candeeiros. No caso das diferenças entre indivíduos analisados ao nível intra-populacional, estes mostraram variações individuais nomeadamente na frequência de pico e do tempo de ascensão. Globalmente, os resultados parecem corroborar a tese de que o isolamento genético destas populações pode ser imputável também a barreiras comunicacionais e sociais.
Scherer, Nancy J., A. Lynn Williams, and Kerry Proctor-Williams. "Early and Later Vocalization Skills in Children with and Without Cleft Palate." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2008. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/1774.
Full textBurgdorf, Jeffrey. "THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF 50-KHZ VOCALIZATIONS IN RATS." Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2005. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1131386335.
Full textGrašytė, Gintarė. "Žvirblinės pelėdos (Glaucidium passerinum L.) buveinių pasirinkimas ir vokalizacijos sąlygos." Master's thesis, Lithuanian Academic Libraries Network (LABT), 2014. http://vddb.library.lt/obj/LT-eLABa-0001:E.02~2012~D_20140704_171934-73661.
Full textThe Eurasian Pygmy Owl (Glaucidium passerinum L.) is a widely distributed and protected species all over its distribution range. There are special protected areas in EU countries, where species’ monitoring is carried out under EU Birds directive. However, there is a lack in quantitative data on species macrohabitat, habitat preferences, and vocalization conditions. This thesis addresses these issues. The Eurasian Pygmy Owl macrohabitats – squares of 200 ha, where territorial owls were located in 2005 - 2011 – were compared with randomly selected 200 ha squares. It was ascertained that the Pygmy Owl prefers forest (82 % ± 18 % SD, n = 178) with spruce (21 % ± 14 % SD, n = 160) and mature stands (33 % ± 14 % SD, n = 160) in its macrohabitats. It avoids deciduous trees like the Grey Alder, poplar and willow (2 % ± 4 % SD, n = 160). On the other hand, the Eurasian Pygmy Owl inhabits various types of forests in distinct sub-regions of Lithuania. There was no single variable, which was more important than others for the species habitat use in three analysed sub-regions, however, different preferred/avoided variables emerged. Natural Eurasian Pygmy Owl vocalization was registered 2001 - 2011, during two seasons per year. The date of vocalization varied between years, however, the average date of owl vocalization was 20th of March in winter - spring season (n = 117) and 12th of October in autumn - winter season (n = 42). It was found, that the key factors affecting the Eurasian Pygmy... [to full text]
Hosemann, Aimee Jean. "EFFECTS OF HABITAT DENSITY AND OTHER VARIABLES ON THE FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCY OF THE NONHUMAN PRIMATE LONG CALL." OpenSIUC, 2008. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/theses/414.
Full textXia, Lu. "FATHERS' LANGUAGE INFLUENCE ON THEIR SIX-MONTH-OLD INFANTS' VOCALIZATION DURING FREE-PLAY." Master's thesis, University of Central Florida, 2010. http://digital.library.ucf.edu/cdm/ref/collection/ETD/id/2142.
Full textM.A.
Department of Child, Family and Community Sciences
Education
Early Childhood Development and Education MS
Scherer, Nancy J., Sarah Boyce, and Gerri Martin. "Pre-Linguistic Children with Cleft Palate: Growth of Gesture, Vocalization, and Word Use." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2013. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/1534.
Full textChehayeb, Diala. "Factors affecting amphetamine-induced 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in adult rats." Thesis, McGill University, 2007. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=112388.
Full textKulander, Olivia Clare. "Whence and Whither: Acoustic Variability and Biogeography of Tarsiers in North Sulawesi." PDXScholar, 2018. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/4360.
Full textIrlandini, Isabella Azevedo. "A voz no teatro de animação : artificialidade e síntese vocal." Universidade do Estado de Santa Catarina, 2013. http://tede.udesc.br/handle/handle/1276.
Full textCoordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior
This research focuses on the voice in Puppet Theater a term that embraces different kinds of performances, characterized by hybridity and the dynamics of the animate/inanimate present in Puppet Theater, Mask Theater, Shadow Puppetry, Visual Theater, etc. The voice is approached, in Puppet Theater, as a creator of presence and meaning, according to Hans Ulrich Gumbrecht s theory of presence production. I investigate how the voice can generate presence in relation to specific Puppet Theater poetics. The voice, in singular, refers to its essence as an oscillation between presence and meaning. However, since the voice unfolds itself in different cultural, poetic and historical contexts, actually the voice is plural in its several manifestations. By setting out the following problem, when voice is in Puppet Theater? , I seek to identify poetics that are also defined by its voice presence. Once identified such poetics, I seek to distinguish the voice s generating and constituting elements that establish it as presence and meaning, in relation to its poetics. During the investigation, I contemplate authors and artists that reflect about the use of voice in Puppet Theater and research their procedures, as well as those who treat it as a background to or even outlaw it from the scene. I survey the bibliography on the subject, bring examples from national and international performances seen in the last three years in Brazil, and use material drawn from interviews, seminars and courses realized in this period. Along this research, some recurring vocal procedures in Puppet Theater are individuated, and in some cases, these revealed themselves as specific to an art form characterized by the separation between the puppet-object and its sound source. During the investigation I came to realize that, although the Brazilian Puppet Theater quite often uses speech in its performances, there is significant absence of voice as I have defined above: voice as an oscillation between presence and meaning. Based on Adriana Cavarero s philosophical studies, which describe the de- vocalization process of a civilization centered in the logos (understood as reason), I realized that this process is mirrored by the Puppet Theater scene. The twentieth century emerges as a century in which performances are based on the sense of vision, promoted by a civilization centered on the image. The sense of vision has taken over all the other senses, promoting the de-vocalization of the logos, leading to a de-vocalization of the scene. The aim with this research is to - identify the vocal havens and to investigate the procedures that establish this oscillation between presence and meaning in Puppet Theater s voice, in relation to its poetics. This purpose is key to the re-vocalization in contemporary Brazilian Puppet Theater.
Esta pesquisa trata a voz no Teatro de Animação - termo que abraça diversas formas espetaculares híbridas centradas na dinâmica do animado/inanimado, como o teatro de bonecos, de máscaras, de sombras, visual, etc. A voz é abordada, no Teatro de Animação, como produtora de presença e sentido, conforme a teoria de produção de presença de Hans Ulrich Gumbrecht. Investigo como a voz gera presença numa relação com determinada poética no âmbito do Teatro de Animação. A voz no singular refere-se à sua essência como oscilação entre presença e sentido. Todavia, como a voz surge em diferentes contextos culturais e poéticos, ou seja, históricos, a voz na realidade é plural nas suas diversas manifestações. A partir da questão quando a voz é no Teatro de Animação? , procuro identificar poéticas que engendram o princípio pelo qual a voz é. Uma vez identificada a poética, busco distinguir os elementos que geram e constituem a voz, instaurando-a como presença e sentido, numa relação com a sua poética. Na investigação, contemplo autores e artistas que refletem sobre o uso da voz no teatro de animação e exploram seus procedimentos, assim como aqueles que a relegam a segundo plano ou mesmo a banem da cena. Faço um levantamento bibliográfico do assunto, recorro a exemplos de espetáculos nacionais e internacionais assistidos nos últimos três anos no Brasil e utilizo material colhido em entrevistas, seminários e cursos realizados nesse período. Durante a exposição desta pesquisa são individuados procedimentos recorrentes de uso da voz no Teatro de Animação que, em alguns casos, revelaram-se como específicos da linguagem cuja característica é a separação entre boneco-objeto e fonte sonora. Durante a investigação, constato que, embora a voz seja muito utilizada em cena no Teatro de Animação no Brasil, há uma significante ausência da voz como a defino acima: voz como oscilação entre presença e sentido. A partir dos estudos filosóficos de Adriana Cavarero, que descreve um processo de desvocalização numa civilização centrada no logos entendido como razão, chego à conclusão de que esse processo se espelha na cena do teatro de animação. O século XX desponta como um século cuja espetacularidade teatral é calcada na visão, reflexo de uma civilização centrada na imagem, em que o sentido da visão tem primazia sobre os outros sentidos, promovendo a desvocalização do logos, levando a uma desvocalização na cena. Tal conclusão faz com que o propósito da pesquisa - identificação dos redutos vocais e investigação dos procedimentos que instauram uma oscilação de presença e sentido na voz no Teatro de Animação, numa relação com as poéticas - seja fundamental para a revocalização da cena no Teatro de Animação contemporâneo no Brasil.
Lea, Brian N. "Ultrasonic vocalization in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) : evidence for begging behavior in infant mammals? /." Electronic version (PDF), 2006. http://dl.uncw.edu/etd/2006/leab/brianlea.pdf.
Full textCarrion, Deva P. "Examining the utility of implementing stimulus-stimulus pairing as the first step to build and echoic repertoire." Diss., University of Iowa, 2018. https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/6386.
Full textLe, Roux Aliza. "The alarm vocalizations of Brants' whistling rat, Parotomys Brantsii." Thesis, Stellenbosch : Stellenbosch University, 2001. http://hdl.handle.net/10019.1/52481.
Full textENGLISH ABSTRACT: The alarm calls of a colonial rodent, Brants' whistling rat (Parotomys brantsii), were investigated at Goegap Nature Reserve in the Northern Cape, South Africa. At this site P. brantsii occurs in sympatry with the congeneric P. littledalei, and the alarm calls in response to humans was compared between the two species. Parotomys brantsii's typical call in reaction to a human is a simple whistle, characterized by an initial upward frequency sweep and high frequency plateau, whereas the alarm whistle of P. littledalei has three overlapping components. Parotomys littledalei's calls are significantly shorter and lower in dominant frequency than those of P. brantsii, and have a significantly wider frequency bandwidth. These distinctions were attributed to species discrimination and habitat preferences. The acoustic adaptation hypothesis is supported in part, as P. littledalei, which inhabits a closed habitat in comparison with P. brantsii, has calls which are lower in frequency than P. brantsii calls, but contrary to the hypothesis, P. brantsii calls show less frequency modulation than those of P. littledalei. The high pitch of both species' vocalizations may be a convergent feature making the callers difficult for predators to locate. The alarm call repertoire of P. brantsii was investigated by presenting members of the P. brantsii colony with models of some typical predators, a raptor and puff adder, as well as a human observer. Virtually no variation was detected in the structure of alarm whistles elicited by different predators, indicating that P. brantsii did not identify different predator types by means of vocalizations, even though behavioural responses showed that P. brantsii do distinguish between different predators and non-predators. Note duration varied in conjunction with the physical reaction of the caller. When the caller bolted towards safety, the whistle was significantly shorter than when it remained aboveground. During an encounter with a snake or far-off human the caller would utter relatively long duration calls, signifying a low-risk situation, while highrisk situations such as a nearby human or raptor provoked short calls, emitted just before the whistling rat bolted underground. I regard P. brantsii's alarm call repertoire as a graded "urgency-based" system, indicating threat level perceived rather than predator type. This system is widespread among ground squirrels and is usually associated with animals inhabiting a two-dimensional environment. Playback experiments were conducted to determine the meaning of alarm calls to conspecifics, focusing specifically on the effect of variation in single call duration and function of alarm-calling bouts. All alarm calls induced heightened vigilance in receivers, but the behavioural response did not differ between short and long single calls. Multiple calls lead to significantly longer periods of high vigilance than single calls even after calling bouts have ended, showing that one of the functions of multiple calls is to maintain high vigilance in conspecifics for relatively long periods. The non-iconic nature of their alarm calls and the undiscriminating response of receivers to different calls emphasizes the importance of contextual clues in the communication system of animals living in an open habitat like that of P. brantsii.
AFRIKAANSE OPSOMMING: Die alarm-roepgeluide van 'n koloniale knaagdier, Brants se fluitrot (Parotomys brantsii), is ondersoek by Goegap Natuurreservaat in the Noordkaap, Suid-Afrika, waar hulle simpatries voorkom met die naverwante P. littledalei. Die species se alarmroepe in reaksie op mense is met mekaar vergelyk. Die tipiese alarmroep van P. brantsii is 'n eenvoudige fluit, gekenmerk deur 'n aanvanklike skerp frekwensiestyging en hoë-frekwensie plato. Parotomys littledalei se fluitroep het drie komponente wat mekaar gedeeltelike oordek. Parotomys littledalei se roepgeluide is beduidend korter en laer in heersende toonhoogte as dié van P. brantsii, en hul frekwensie-wydte is ook beduidend wyer. Hierdie verskille word toegeskryf aan species-verskille en habitat-voorkeure. Die akoestiese aanpassingshipotese word gedeeltelik deur die studie ondersteun, aangesien P. littledalei, wat 'n geslote habitat verkies in vergelyking met P. brantsii, alarmroepe het met 'n laer dominante toonhoogte as P. brantsii. In teenstelling met die hipotese, toon P. brantsii se fluitgeluide minder frekwensie-modulasie as P. littledalei se geluide. Die hoë toonhoogte van beide species se alarmroepe kan 'n konvergente verskynsel wees wat dit vir roofdiere moeilik maak om fluitende individue te lokaliseer. Die alarm-repertoire van P. brantsii is ondersoek deur individue bloot te stel aan 'n menslike waarnemer en modelle van tipiese predatore, naamlik 'n jakkalsvoël en pofadder. Daar was omtrent geen variasie in die klank-struktuur van alarmroepe in reaksie op verskillende roofdiere nie. Dit wys daarop dat P. brantsii nie roofdiere deur middel van roepgeluide klassifiseer nie, hoewel gedragsresponse getoon het dat individue wel tussen roofdiere kan onderskei. Die duur van alarmroepe varieer na gelang van die gedrag van die fluitende individue - 'n fluitrot wat vlug, se roep is beduidend korter as die geluid van een wat bogronds bly. 'n Pofadder en 'n veraf mens ontlok lankdurende geluide, wat dui op 'n lae-risiko situasie, terwyl rotte op vlug slaan in reaksie op 'n hoë-risiko situasie, soos met 'n nabygeleë mens of jakkalsvoël, wanneer kort alarmroepe geuiter word. Ek beskou P. brantsii se alarmrepertoire as 'n gegradeerde "nood-gebaseerde" stelsel, wat die mate van bedreiging aandui, eerder as die identiteit van die spesifieke roofdier. Hierdie sisteem is tipies van grondeekhorings en word gewoonlik verbind met diere wat 'n twee-dimensionele omgewing bewoon. Parotomys brantsii se reaksie op die terugspeel van die species se geluide is hierna ondersoek. Ek het gefokus op die effek van 'n verandering in die duur van alleenstaande fluitnote, asook hulle reaksie op fluitreekse. Alle alarmroepe het gelei tot hoër vlakke van waaksaamheid in luisterende individue, maar reaksies op kort en lang enkelnote het nie van mekaar verskil nie. Fluitreekse het beduidend langer periodes van hoë waaksaamheid veroorsaak as enkele geluide, en waaksaamheid het hoog gebly selfs nadat reekse geëindig het. Dit beteken dat fluitreekse onder andere die funksie vervul om waaksaamheid in kolonielede vir relatief lang tye te onderhou. Parotomys brantsii se eenvoudige alarm-repertoire en nie-onderskeidende reaksie op verskillene roepgeluide, beklemtoon die belangrikheid van aanvullende inligting uit die omgewing vir diere in 'n twee-dimensionele habitat wanneer die akoestiese kommunikasiestelse slegs 'n algemene waarskuwingsfunksie vervul.
Scherer, Nancy J., Sarah Boyce, and G. Martin. "Longitudinal Analysis of Rates of Gesture, Vocalization and Word use in Toddlers with Cleft Palate." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2011. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/1537.
Full textau, turtle111@aapt net, and Jacqueline Giles. "The underwater acoustic repertoire of the long-necked, freshwater turtle Chelodina oblonga." Murdoch University, 2005. http://wwwlib.murdoch.edu.au/adt/browse/view/adt-MU20061121.103729.
Full textCouroux, Christina. "Neighbor-stranger discrimination and individual recognition by voice in the American redstart (Setophaga ruticilla)." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1997. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/tape16/PQDD_0027/MQ37111.pdf.
Full textGrünloh, Thomas, and Ulf Liszkowski. "Prelinguistic vocalizations distinguish pointing acts." Cambridge University Press, 2015. https://tud.qucosa.de/id/qucosa%3A39052.
Full textWatson, Peter J. "Effects of abdominal trussing on breathing, vocalization, and speech in persons with cervical spinal cord injury." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/282834.
Full textBurke, da Silva Karen. "Antipredator calling by the eastern chipmunk, Tamius striatus." Thesis, McGill University, 1993. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=68157.
Full textExperiments were carried out to determine the function of the trill and chipping. Demographic and contextual effects indicate that the trill is in part a call which functions to warn kin but may also indicate to conspecifics that the caller has escaped into a refuge. An experiment with a tethered cat concluded that chipping is likely to function to deter predators from hunting in the area. This is done through vocal mobbing by several individuals whose home ranges overlap.
Gridley, Teresa. "Geographic and species variation in bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops spp.) signature whistle types." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/1910.
Full textEldridge, Jacob Douglas. "A Comparison of Current Anuran Monitoring Methods with Emphasis on the Accuracy of Automatic Vocalization Detection Software." TopSCHOLAR®, 2011. http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses/1122.
Full textDencev, Aurelia. "An ascending cholinergic projection to the basal forebrain involved in the initiation of ultrasonic vocalization in the rat." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1999. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape11/PQDD_0002/MQ42238.pdf.
Full textLagorio, Amy D. "AMPHIBIAN VOCALIZATION: IMPLICATIONS OF A NOVEL LARYNGEAL MUSCLE IN THE CALLING MECHANISMS OF THE TÚNGARA FROG ENGYSTOMOPS PUSTULOSUS." Scholarly Commons, 2020. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/3683.
Full textAwasthi, Smita. "Emergence of vocalization in non vocal children with a diagnosis of autism : building an evidence base for interventions." Thesis, Queen's University Belfast, 2017. https://pure.qub.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/emergence-of-vocalization-in-non-vocal-children-with-a-diagnosis-of-autism-building-an-evidence-base-for-interventions(e0005530-9415-4229-88fd-6dce404962a1).html.
Full textMizuguchi, Daisuke. "Underwater vocal repertoire and their function in three ice-breeding seals in the Arctic." 京都大学 (Kyoto University), 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/2433/215373.
Full textHay, Timothy D. "Variation in advertisement call structure of whistling frogs." Thesis, University of Canterbury. Zoology, 1994. http://hdl.handle.net/10092/1517.
Full textDigweed, Shannon Marie, and University of Lethbridge Faculty of Arts and Science. "The functions and mechanisms of predator-associated vocalisations in North American red squirrels (Tamiasciurus Hudsonicus)." Thesis, Lethbridge, Alta. : University of Lethbridge, Dept. of Psychology, c2009, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/10133/2474.
Full textxi, 153 leaves : ill. ; 29 cm
Bernstein, Gabrielle. "Glidevowel alternation in Biblical Hebrew." Thesis, McGill University, 1986. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=65473.
Full textBonenfant, Marjolaine. "Escape and vocal responses of eastern chipmunks (Tamias striatus) to simulated aerial predator attack." Thesis, McGill University, 1996. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=23989.
Full textStuby, Richard George Jr. "A stochastic measure of similarity between dolphin signature whistles." Thesis, Virginia Tech, 1993. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/31408.
Full textMaster of Science
Woolley, Sarah Margaret Nicolay. "Auditory feedback and song behavior in adult Bengalese finches /." Thesis, Connect to this title online; UW restricted, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/1773/10653.
Full textFenzl, Thomas. "Differentiated roles of the periaqueductal gray and the paralemniscal area on vocalization in the new world bat Phyllostomus discolor." Diss., lmu, 2003. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bvb:19-14231.
Full textDazey, Jennifer. "Influence of Daily Electrical Stimulation of Periaqueductal Grey on Vocalization and Depressive-like Behavior during Separation in Guinea Pigs." Wright State University / OhioLINK, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=wright1355011112.
Full textPereira, da Cruz Benetti Lucia. "Infant vocal imitation of music." The Ohio State University, 2017. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1492408461186887.
Full textFreeman, Angela Rose. "Vasopressin and Social Behavior in Richardson's Ground Squirrels." Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1480353729694591.
Full textMelcon, Matthew Clinton 1957. "Effect of age on laryngeal airway resistance during vowel production." Thesis, The University of Arizona, 1988. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/276751.
Full textSamarra, Filipa Isabel Pereira. "Functional design and use of acoustic signals produced by killer whales (Orcinus orca)." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/2564.
Full textCandiotti, Agnès. "Social life and flexibility of vocal behaviour in Diana monkeys and other cercopithecids." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/8171.
Full textDunaway, Mark Allen. "Seasonal Territoriality in the Carolina Wren, Thryothorus ludovicianus, to Visual and Vocal Stimuli." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2006. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/2208.
Full textMonteleone, Stephanie. "Writing A Way Out of the Chamber| Re-vocalization of Myth in the Works of Eudora Welty, Shirley Jackson, and Toni Morrison." Thesis, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, 2016. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10163283.
Full textTale and myth have a long history of reinforcing, commenting on, and often subverting the ideologies at work in the society where the stories are being told. This research explores the ways three American novels, Eudora Welty’s The Robber Bridegroom, Shirley Jackson’s We Have Always Lived in the Castle, and Toni Morrison’s The Bluest Eye, all incorporate variants of the fairytale Bluebeard: a fairytale which centers on domestic trauma. All three novels also re-vocalize the myth of Demeter and Persephone, and this re-vocalization serves to empower the female characters and subvert the dominant patriarchal paradigm. The subversion of white masculine ideology in these novels reflects a changing social structure during the thirty year span in which these three novels were published. Looking at the texts holistically while considering the ways the tale and myth interweave in each offers insight into the way these social changes for women were being narrated and explored. The question of interpretation is central to this research, which explores both feminine and masculine lenses in story. Particularly the ways a woman’s sexual agency, decision not to marry, or even inability to escape are narrated and interpreted by the community around her. These fictional communities and the issues explored in the realm of tale reflect the larger society and ideological currents surrounding novels themselves. All three novels incorporate the Bluebeard tale, reject the masculine reading of women in that tale, and work to subvert not just patriarchal ideology but the flat literary trope and ways of writing and reading women.
Baldwin, Jeffrey W. Jr. "Determination of a Two-Week `Window’ for PCB Influence on Ultrasonic Vocalization and Other Behavioral Measures in Young Sprague-Dawley Rats." Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1404686457.
Full textRicci, Gisele Dela. "Efeitos da climatização ambiental no comportamento e bem-estar de matrizes e leitões em maternidades livres de gaiolas." Universidade de São Paulo, 2018. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/74/74131/tde-13022019-082413/.
Full textPigs adapt to the environmental adversities imposed by confinement from physiological and behavioral changes and the insertion of simple climatizations can improve the comfort and well-being of the animals. The objective of this study was to evaluate the effects of environmental conditioning of cage-free motherhood on the behavior and well-being of sows and piglets. Twenty-six females and 281 piglets of the Large White x Landrace strain, weaned at 21 days, were kept in two types of environment: climated (ventilator and sprinkler on roof) and non-climated (control) Costa, of the USP, in Pirassununga. The internal temperature in the control environment at 7 hours was 23.7 ° C ± 0.64 with humidity of 74% ± 1.64 and at 13 hours of 26.2 ° C ± 1.23 with humidity of 70.4% ± 1.68. In the air-conditioned environment, at 7 o\'clock the ambient temperature was 22.1 ° C ± 0.66C with humidity of 81.9% ± 1.66 and at 13 hours 24.5 ° C ± 0.85 and humidity of 85 , 7% ± 1.34. Respiratory rate and rectal temperature were obtained three times a week, twice a day, morning and afternoon, by counting the flank per minute and digital rectal thermometer, respectively. Saliva harvests for cortisol analysis (matrices only) were performed once a week, twice daily in the morning and afternoon. The evaluation of dominance hierarchy and teat selection records were performed in three analyzes per week from direct focal observations, two per period, morning and afternoon. Classification in dominant, intermediate and subordinate piglets was obtained by sow, according to the presentation of fights among piglets. For statistical analysis of the hierarchy, a binary matrix of dominance based on occurrences of fights was constructed, identifying the individuals of the lines dominating column individuals, analyzed by means of variance with fixed treatment effects, periods, sex and their interactions. Behavior and environmental preference were obtained three times per week, twice a day, two hours per period, morning and afternoon. The sites evaluated were left (side with drinking fountains), right (anti-crushing bar), middle (bed and enrichment), front (concrete floor) and behind (cane bagasse bed). The measures of behavior and environmental preference were evaluated by analysis of variance, with effects of treatment, time and place. Dorsal, ventral surface temperatures of the legs, legs, heads, muzzles, eyepieces and mammary gland (sows) were measured using infrared thermographic camera, as well as roof, bed and floor temperatures. The air temperature, relative humidity and black globe temperature were also obtained and stored in a datalogger and correlations were made between environmental and surface temperature data. The thermographic images were analyzed by the software IRSoft Version 3.6 Testo and analyzed by variance with fixed effects of treatment and season of the year. The results showed that in relation to the hierarchy there was no effect of the fixed factors (P> 0.05). The piglets sought more often the intermediate, upper and lower teats. Sex did not influence the classification of hierarchy. Piglets classified as lightweight presented lower weight gain at weaning in the air-conditioned environment. Regarding the behavior, there was no interaction between periods and sites for sows and piglets. The females preferred the left side, followed by the right, front, back and middle of the bay. Weaning weight and weight loss of the females had no effect of the environments (P> 0.05). Regarding infrared thermography analyzes, the seasons of the year influenced bed, concrete and roof temperatures due to the higher summer temperatures. Positive correlations were found regarding the internal temperature of bed facilities (r = 0.28, P <0.05) and concrete (r = 0.26, P <0.05). For females, the seasons influenced the body temperatures. Positive correlations were found regarding bed and temperatures of the back, belly, mammary gland, thigh, head and eyes. The respiratory rate showed a positive correlation with the back, snout, bed, concrete and roof. Salivary cortisol did not present positive and significant correlations with the facilities. The seasons and the periods of the day influenced the superficial temperatures of the piglets, with higher temperatures in the summer and in the afternoon. The respiratory rate was influenced by the period of the day with higher frequencies presented in the afternoon. It is concluded with this study that the climatization does not influence the choice of the tits in the mammary gland. The ambient climated generated greater thermal comfort to the matrices, however, did not alter the comfort of piglets, since they had escamoteadores with the function of protecting against temperatures below their zone of thermal comfort. The presence of ventilators and water sprinklers on the roof was not sufficient to reduce the body surface temperature of females and piglets inserted in stalls free of maternity cages.
Monteiro, Stefanny Christie Gomes. "Relações entre taxa de vocalização, níveis plasmáticos de corticosterona e imunocompetência em Hypsiboas albopunctatus (Spix, 1824)." Universidade de São Paulo, 2013. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/41/41135/tde-10102013-152930/.
Full textAccording to the model of intersexual selection mediated by parasites, changes in conditions of sexual ornaments or display rates reflect different degrees of resistance to parasitism. In natural choruses of amphibians, calls that are higher, longer, more complex and / or issued at higher rates are probably more easily detected and attract more females. However, the vocal effort is positively related to plasma levels of potentially immunosuppressive steroids, such as testosterone and corticosterone. The aim of this study was to evaluate the relationship between calling rates, immunocompetence and plasma levels of corticosterone in Hypsiboas albopunctatus, a midsize anuran occurring in South America. Such relationships were studied from focal observations of calling behavior, followed by blood collection for measurements of corticosterone plasma levels and evaluation of some immune parameters. The present study showed a positive relationship between the calling rate with plasma levels of corticosterone, and a negative correlation between these two parameters with cell-mediated immunocompetence. These results highlight the importance of the hormone corticosterone as a possible mediator of trade-off between vocal effort and cell-mediated immunity in anurans.