To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Theory of mind.

Journal articles on the topic 'Theory of mind'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Theory of mind.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Smukler, David. "Unauthorized Minds: How “Theory of Mind” Theory Misrepresents Autism." Mental Retardation 43, no. 1 (February 2005): 11–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1352/0047-6765(2005)43<11:umhtom>2.0.co;2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Barrett, Justin L. "From theory of mind to divine minds." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, no. 6 (June 2011): 244–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Chung, Jaesik. "Gregory Bateson’s Aesthetic Circuits of the Mind." Criticism and Theory Society of Korea 23, no. 2 (June 18, 2018): 93–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.19116/theory.2018.23.2.93.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Brosow, Frank. "TRAP-Mind Theory." Journal of Didactics of Philosophy 4, no. 1 (March 1, 2020): 14–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.46586/jdph.2020.9570.

Full text
Abstract:
The TRAP-Mind-Theory introduces a problem-oriented technique of philosophizing, based on the results of empirical research in cognitive psychology. Philosophizing is understood as the mental activity in which philosophical education is applied. In order to learn how to philosophize, students must perform the mental processes that philosophizing is all about. Those processes can be identified by making use of empirical findings of cognitive psychology. The observance of those findings leads to the matrix of contemplation, a model with three areas and four levels. People develop their thoughts to the next level by adding reasons (for themselves, others or all people) to the results of the current one. Reasons and considerations get tested by using the 5C-criteria (clarity, correlation, consistency, completeness, comparison). By breaking down the complex process of philosophizing into these steps, philosophizing with children and grown-ups becomes possible, teachable, and evaluable.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Leudar, Ivan, Alan Costall, and Dave Francis. "Theory of Mind." Theory & Psychology 14, no. 5 (October 2004): 571–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0959354304046173.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Stuart, Jonathan. "Theory of mind." New Scientist 218, no. 2922 (June 2013): 31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0262-4079(13)61556-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Frith, Chris, and Uta Frith. "Theory of mind." Current Biology 15, no. 17 (September 2005): R644—R645. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.041.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Carlson, Stephanie M., Melissa A. Koenig, and Madeline B. Harms. "Theory of mind." Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 4, no. 4 (March 18, 2013): 391–402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1232.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Seo, Young Yi. "Monistic Theory of Human Mind-Moral Mind : Experiential Reading of Noh Susin"s Mind Theory." Journal Of pan-Korean Philosophical Society 91 (December 31, 2018): 75–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.17745/pkps.2018.12.91.75.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Berube, Michael. "A Theory of Theory of Mind." American Scientist 101, no. 2 (2013): 148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1511/2013.101.148.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

FRITH, C. D. "Schizophrenia and theory of mind." Psychological Medicine 34, no. 3 (April 2004): 385–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291703001326.

Full text
Abstract:
We suspect that people have an everyday theory of mind because they explain and frequently talk about the behaviour of others and themselves in terms of beliefs and desires. Having a theory of mind means that we believe that other people have minds like ours and that we understand the behaviour of these others in terms of the contents of their minds: their knowledge, beliefs and desires. But how can we demonstrate experimentally that people are using their theory of mind to predict the behaviour of others. This problem is particularly acute in the case of animals or young human children when they do not have language. Dennett (1978) discussing Premack & Woodruff's (1978) seminal paper ‘Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?’, suggested that the use of false beliefs to explain behaviour would provide convincing evidence. When their belief is true (i.e. corresponds to the actual state of the world) we can explain peoples' behaviour on the basis of the state of the world without needing to know about their beliefs. This ambiguity does not arise when the belief is false. The first experiment to use this approach was published by Wimmer & Perner (1983). They showed that at around 4 years of age a child knows that Maxi will look for his chocolates where Maxi believes them to be, even though the child knows that this belief is false because he has seen Maxi's mother moving the chocolates. In the English-speaking world the task involving Maxi and the chocolates has become the Sally-Anne task (see this issue, Lee et al. 2004).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Rumyantseva, E. E. "Theory of mind and neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenia." Клиническая и специальная психология 4, no. 3 (2015): 77–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.17759/cpse.2015040306.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this work was to study the problem of interrelation between theory of mind and neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Tasks: analysis of the literature on the problem of interrelation of theory of mind and neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Subject of research: interrelation of theory of mind and neurocognitive functioning. Research hypothesis: the state of the mental model correlated with neurocognitive functioning. Registered a decline in the functioning of theory of mind in schizophrenia. It is known that hypofrontality in schizophrenia determines the reduction of social perception. A number of authors allocate structures in the brain, providing mental models: regions of the medial prefrontal cortex and posttemporal areas, including the temporo parietal region. Some studies found relationship between the theory of mind and memory, executive functions. However, there are studies, which has not been found the interrelation between theory of mind and neurocognitive functioning. Nonetheless, some studies concluded that currently there is no consensus about the influence of neurocognitive functioning on the theory of mind in schizophrenia.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Rubia-Vila, Francisco José. "The Theory of Mind." ANALES RANM 135, no. 02 (December 30, 2018): 141–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.32440/ar.2018.135.02.rev05.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Hatfield, Gary. "Descartes’s Theory of Mind." International Philosophical Quarterly 45, no. 1 (2005): 124–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/ipq200545170.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Thompson, Manley. "Kant’s Theory of Mind." International Studies in Philosophy 17, no. 3 (1985): 80–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/intstudphil198517321.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Bernstein, Daniel M. "Lifespan Theory of Mind." Zeitschrift für Psychologie 226, no. 2 (April 2018): 85–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000324.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

ANDERSON, OLIVER D. "Graph Theory Mind-Grinders." Teaching Mathematics and its Applications 10, no. 2 (1991): 74–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/teamat/10.2.74.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Yoshida, Wako, Ray J. Dolan, and Karl J. Friston. "Game Theory of Mind." PLoS Computational Biology 4, no. 12 (December 26, 2008): e1000254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000254.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Liszkowski, Ulf. "Using Theory of Mind." Child Development Perspectives 7, no. 2 (January 29, 2013): 104–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12025.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

de Maré, Patrick, and Roberto Schöllberger. "A Theory of Mind." Group Analysis 39, no. 1 (March 2006): 65–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0533316406062086.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Verschure, Paul F. M. J. "Smolensky's theory of mind." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13, no. 2 (June 1990): 407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00079474.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Allen, Josephine R., and Kristofer Kinsey. "Teaching Theory of Mind." Early Education & Development 24, no. 6 (August 2013): 865–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2013.745182.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Astington, Janet Wilde, and Terri Barriault. "Childrenʼs Theory of Mind." Infants & Young Children 13, no. 3 (January 2001): 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001163-200113030-00005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Yergeau, Melanie, and Bryce Huebner. "Minding Theory of Mind." Journal of Social Philosophy 48, no. 3 (September 2017): 273–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josp.12191.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Müller,, Ulrich, and Willis F. Overton. "Action Theory of Mind and Representational Theory of Mind: Is Dialogue Possible?" Human Development 41, no. 2 (1998): 127–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000022573.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

전현희. "Chutzu's Theory of Human Mind and Moral Mind." JOURNAL OF KOREAN PHILOSOPHICAL HISTORY ll, no. 31 (March 2011): 289–319. http://dx.doi.org/10.35504/kph.2011..31.011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Jolley, N. "Descartes's Concept of Mind; Descartes's Theory of Mind." Philosophical Review 114, no. 1 (January 1, 2005): 118–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/00318108-114-1-118.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Bohl, Vivian, and Nivedita Gangopadhyay. "Theory of mind and the unobservability of other minds." Philosophical Explorations 17, no. 2 (July 30, 2013): 203–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13869795.2013.821515.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Dant, Tim. "In two minds: Theory of Mind, intersubjectivity, and autism." Theory & Psychology 25, no. 1 (November 13, 2014): 45–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0959354314556526.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Jung, Kyunghoon. "A Theoretical Ground of Neuropsychoanalysis I: Visiting the Concepts of Drive with Freud, Lacan, Damasio, Panksepp, and Solms." Criticism and Theory Society of Korea 27, no. 3 (October 31, 2022): 139–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.19116/theory.2022.27.3.139.

Full text
Abstract:
Psychoanalysis and neuroscience are the same in that both study the mind, although their methodologies are different. Neuropsychoanalysis, which makes integrative approaches to them, is needed for the mind studies. The paper attempts to explore the neuropsychoanalytic concept of drive by examining the drive-related theories of Freud, Lacan, Damasio, Panksepp, and Solms. According to Freud, drive arises from somatic sources and operates at the unconscious level. Affects, emotions, and feelings are based on drives, but they are felt and realized at the conscious levels. He focuses only on the psychological due to the intellectual limitations of his times but makes it clear that the sources of drive are somatic. Lacan refuses to attend to the physical source of drive. For Damasio, feelings arise primarily from the physical sources, and the feelings include background/primitive feelings, primary emotion feelings, and secondary emotion feelings. Feelings are fundamental to the mind and accompany emotions. Freud’s drive and Damasio’s feeling concepts are similar in that they indicate the basic energy of the mind arising from the somatic. Panksepp presents four major emotional systems on the ground of the sources of feelings within intrinsic brain functions. Solms follows Panksepp’s ideas. For both Damasio and Panksepp, feelings/emotional systems emerge from somatic sources, and the sources are a variety of neural circuits and neurochemicals. If we can fill with Damasio’s and Panksepp’s similar ideas of neural circuits and neurochemicals the somatic sources that Freud left blank, we can think of the neuropsychoanalytic concept of drive.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Hoogenhout, Michelle, and Susan Malcolm-Smith. "Theory of mind predicts severity level in autism." Autism 21, no. 2 (August 19, 2016): 242–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1362361316636758.

Full text
Abstract:
We investigated whether theory of mind skills can indicate autism spectrum disorder severity. In all, 62 children with autism spectrum disorder completed a developmentally sensitive theory of mind battery. We used intelligence quotient, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.) diagnosis and level of support needed as indicators of severity level. Using hierarchical cluster analysis, we found three distinct clusters of theory of mind ability: early-developing theory of mind (Cluster 1), false-belief reasoning (Cluster 2) and sophisticated theory of mind understanding (Cluster 3). The clusters corresponded to severe, moderate and mild autism spectrum disorder. As an indicator of level of support needed, cluster grouping predicted the type of school children attended. All Cluster 1 children attended autism-specific schools; Cluster 2 was divided between autism-specific and special needs schools and nearly all Cluster 3 children attended general special needs and mainstream schools. Assessing theory of mind skills can reliably discriminate severity levels within autism spectrum disorder.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Kobayashi Frank, Chiyoko. "Reviving pragmatic theory of theory of mind." AIMS Neuroscience 5, no. 2 (2018): 116–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.3934/neuroscience.2018.2.116.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Hobson, R. Peter. "Against the theory of ‘Theory of Mind’." British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9, no. 1 (March 1991): 33–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.1991.tb00860.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Feldman, Carol Fleisher. "The New Theory of Theory of Mind." Human Development 35, no. 2 (1992): 107–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000277138.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Kimbrough, Erik O., Nikolaus Robalino, and Arthur J. Robson. "Applying “theory of mind”: Theory and experiments." Games and Economic Behavior 106 (November 2017): 209–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2017.10.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Tsoukalas, Ioannis. "Theory of Mind: Towards an Evolutionary Theory." Evolutionary Psychological Science 4, no. 1 (August 7, 2017): 38–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40806-017-0112-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

GÄRDENFORS, PETER. "Mind-reading as Control Theory." European Review 15, no. 2 (April 4, 2007): 223–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1062798707000233.

Full text
Abstract:
In contrast to other animals, humans are good at mind-reading in the sense that they can represent the contents of the minds of others. In this article, the competence for inter-subjectivity is divided into representing the emotions, the attention, the intentions and the beliefs and knowledge of others. Recent attempts to exploit control theory for modelling various cognitive functions are discussed and it is outlined how this modelling approach can be combined with the analysis of inter-subjectivity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Tarricone, Pina. "From a copy theory of mind to a quasi-constructivist theory of mind." Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35, no. 1 (January 2014): 58–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2013.08.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Florio, S., and S. Shapiro. "Set Theory, Type Theory, and Absolute Generality." Mind 123, no. 489 (January 1, 2014): 157–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/mind/fzu039.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Buckwalter, Wesley. "Mind-Brain Dichotomy, Mental Disorder, and Theory of Mind." Erkenntnis 85, no. 2 (July 28, 2018): 511–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10670-018-0038-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Bodden, Maren, Dorothee Kübler, Susanne Knake, Katja Menzler, Johannes Heverhagen, Jens Sommer, Elke Kalbe, Sören Krach, and Richard Dodel. "Comparing the neural correlates of affective and cognitive theory of mind using fMRI: Involvement of the basal ganglia in affective theory of mind." Advances in Cognitive Psychology 9, no. 1 (March 31, 2013): 32–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.5709/acp-0129-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Stone, Valerie E., Simon Baron-Cohen, and Robert T. Knight. "Frontal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10, no. 5 (September 1998): 640–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892998562942.

Full text
Abstract:
“Theory of mind,” the ability to make inferences about others' mental states, seems to be a modular cognitive capacity that underlies humans' ability to engage in complex social interaction. It develops in several distinct stages, which can be measured with social reasoning tests of increasing difficulty. Individuals with Asperger's syndrome, a mild form of autism, perform well on simpler theory of mind tests but show deficits on more developmentally advanced theory of mind tests. We tested patients with bilateral damage to orbito-frontal cortex (n = 5) and unilateral damage in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (n = 5) on a series of theory of mind tasks varying in difficulty. Bilateral orbito-frontal lesion patients performed similarly to individuals with Asperger's syndrome, performing well on simpler tests and showing deficits on tasks requiring more subtle social reasoning, such as the ability to recognize a faux pas. In contrast, no specific theory of mind deficits were evident in the unilateral dorsolateral frontal lesion patients. The dorsolateral lesion patients had difficulty only on versions of the tasks that placed demands on working memory.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Sebastián, Miguel Ángel. "Consciousness and Theory of Mind: a Common Theory?" THEORIA. An International Journal for Theory, History and Foundations of Science 31, no. 1 (February 17, 2016): 73–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1387/theoria.14091.

Full text
Abstract:
Many philosophers and scientists have argued that the difference between phenomenally conscious states and other kind of states lies in the implicit self-awareness that conscious states have. Higher-Order Representationalist (HOR) theories, attempt to explain such a self-awareness by means of a higher-order representation. Consciousness relies on our capacity to represent our own mental states, consciousness depends on our Theory of Mind. Such an ability can, at least conceptually, be decomposed into another two: mindreading and metacognition. In this paper I will argue that consciousness cannot depend on mindreading. The tenability of HOR theories depends, therefore, on the relation between mindreading and metacognition. I analyze several views on such a relation and argue that none of them seem to be a plausible option for HOR theories.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Moawad, Ruba Abdelmatloub. "The Development of Theory of Mind in Saudi Children." Journal of Cognition and Culture 22, no. 1-2 (March 11, 2022): 164–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340128.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Theory of Mind is considered a person’s ability to understand his or her own mind and the minds of others, it includes a social-cognitive skill with implications for many aspects of children’s life, such as social competence, peer acceptance and early success in school. The aims of this research were to study the development of Theory of Mind and to investigate differences in the performance of Theory of Mind tasks across age groups and by gender in Saudi children. 264 children, ranging from 3–12 years of age, participated in this study and were divided into three age groups. A Theory of Mind task battery was conducted individually, and the overall results indicated age and gender differences among our participants, as girls perform better than boys, and the older age groups perform better than they younger groups.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Gál, Zita, Katalin Katona, Karolina Janacsek, and Dezső Németh. "Theory of mind in offenders." Pszichológia 34, no. 3 (September 2014): 289–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/pszicho.34.2014.3.5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Owen, David, and Daniel E. Flage. "David Hume's Theory of Mind." Philosophical Review 101, no. 4 (October 1992): 858. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2185937.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

이영의. "Issues of Extended Mind Theory." Sogang Journal of Philosophy 31, no. ll (November 2012): 29–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.17325/sgjp.2012.31..29.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

酒, 艳红. "The Theory of Mind Wandering." Advances in Social Sciences 10, no. 06 (2021): 1454–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/ass.2021.106200.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Petric, Domina. "The Knot Theory of Mind." Open Journal of Medical Psychology 11, no. 01 (2022): 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ojmp.2022.111001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Kashiwagura, Akihiro. "Zhanran’s Theory of Mind Only." Journal of Indian and Buddhist Studies (Indogaku Bukkyogaku Kenkyu) 69, no. 1 (December 25, 2020): 208–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.4259/ibk.69.1_208.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography