To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Stereotypes (Social psychology).

Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Stereotypes (Social psychology)'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Stereotypes (Social psychology).'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Henwood, K. L. "The social psychology of stereotypes : a critical assessment." Thesis, University of Bristol, 1986. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.376484.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Macrae, Colin Neil. "The effects of stereotypes on social judgements." Thesis, University of Aberdeen, 1990. http://digitool.abdn.ac.uk/R?func=search-advanced-go&find_code1=WSN&request1=AAIU027084.

Full text
Abstract:
Research and theorising in the social cognition tradition typically equates stereotypes with cognitive schemata and researchers investigate the extent of schematic effects on social information processing. Within this cognitive perspective, however, the present thesis identified several unresolved issues in the existing stereotype literature. On the basis of these limitations, the present thesis addressed three main empirical issues: (1) stereotypical effects on causal attribution; (2) stereotypical effects on social memory; and (3) stereotypical effects on information processing. Chapters 4 and 5 established the effects of stereotypes on judical decision processes and provided an impetus for the subsequent experimentation. Chapters 6, 7, and 8 investigated the effects of stereotypes on causal attribution and social memory. It was established that stereotype-confirming behaviours are attributed less to external factors than are stereotype-disconfirming behaviours. Also, subjects showed a preferential recall for stereotype-confirming rather than disconfirming information. Chapter 9 investigated the effects of stereotype activation on the speed with which subjects can make a range of attributional inferences. It was demonstrated that when a consistent stereotype is activated subjects have instant access to causal information from which inferences of personal causation can be rapidly computed. Chapter 10 investigated two process models of stereotype use in judgemental tasks. While stereotypical judgemental biases were obtained, the results supported neither of the proposed process models. Consequently, the results were recast into a new theoretical framework implicating both cognitive and motivational factors as determinants of stereotypic biases on judgement and recall. Chapter 11 considered the wider implications of the findings obtained in the present thesis and two process models of stereotype use were presented. The first outlined the effects of stereotypes on judgements of personal causation, and the second the effects of stereotypes on the relationship between judgement and recall in social information processing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Pinel, Elizabeth Claudine. "Stigma-consciousness : the psychological legacy of social stereotypes /." Digital version accessible at:, 1998. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/utexas/main.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Zyzniewski, Linda Elaine. "The Influence of Nouns on Stereotypes." W&M ScholarWorks, 1996. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539626078.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Hall, Leslie. "Facebook and Stereotypes: How Facebook Users Process Stereotype-Consistent and Stereotype-Inconsistent Information with Varying Cognitive Loads." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2013. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/cmc_theses/668.

Full text
Abstract:
This study builds on previous research analyzing the effects of cognitive busyness on recall of stereotype-consistent and stereotype-inconsistent information by examining the real-world context of Facebook profiles. College students (N = 160) were randomly assigned to either a cognitively busy or unbusy condition. They then looked at either the profile of an African-American male or female target. After, they were given a recall test to assess the number of stereotype-consistent and stereotype-inconsistent descriptors correctly recalled. Results were expected to show that participants recalled more stereotype-consistent information when cognitively busy, even more so for male targets. Conversely, participants were expected to recall more stereotype-inconsistent information when cognitively unbusy, even more so for male targets. Neither cognitive busyness nor the interaction between cognitive busyness and target gender affected the type of information participants recalled. Both results were inconsistent with previous research. Future research should find ways to strengthen the construct validity of measures as they operate in real-world contexts such as Facebook.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Kuchynka, Sophie. "System Threats and Gender Differences in Sexism and Gender Stereotypes." Thesis, University of South Florida, 2015. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=1597535.

Full text
Abstract:

In the United States, women’s persistent gains in structural power may cause backlash among those motivated to preserve the status quo. The proposed study examines the conditions that prompt men and women to endorse sexism and promote gender stereotypes. System justification theory proposes that people are motivated to justify the socio-political system that governs them and threats to the stability of their system can increase individual’s motivated defenses. I expect men to show the strongest motivated defenses when the hierarchy is threatened or viewed as unstable, because to protect group-based interests men will reinforce the legitimacy of the system through stronger endorsement of system defenses. In contrast, women will show the strongest system defenses when the hierarchy is viewed as stable, to avoid feeling trapped in an unchanging system that oppresses them. To test these ideas, 430 men and women were exposed to a gender status hierarchy that was portrayed as stable or unstable and then they responded to several measures of sexism and gender stereotypes. Support for the hypothesis was only found on one measure of gender stereotypes. Men reported more system justifying stereotypes of traditional women in the unstable condition, while women showed the opposite pattern. Exploratory results demonstrate that men’s and women’s reports of agentic stereotypes for traditional and nontraditional women depended on whether they were exposed to a stable or unstable gender hierarchy. Future directions and limitations are discussed in consideration of these exploratory findings.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Wade, Martha Leslie. "Behavioral assimilation and nested social categories exploring gender stereotype priming and stereotype threat /." Columbus, Ohio : Ohio State University, 2007. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=osu1181243878.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Klein, Olivier. "Contribution à une approche pragmatique de l'expression des stéréotypes." Doctoral thesis, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/2013/ULB-DIPOT:oai:dipot.ulb.ac.be:2013/211847.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lindburg, Emily R. "Feminist Stereotypes: Communal vs. Agentic." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2014. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/scripps_theses/398.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examined relationships between facial appearance, gender-linked traits, and feminist stereotypes. Naïve college students rated traits based on facial appearance of female CEO's whose companies appeared in the Forbes 1000 list. The photos of each female CEO (n=35) were randomly combined with two descriptive identifiers; an occupation (n=9) and an interest area (n=9), including 'feminist'. Participants then rated the head shots of the CEO's on a 7 point Likert scale of communal (expected feminine) traits like attractiveness, warmth, compassion and cooperativeness, and on agentic (expected masculine) traits like ambition, leadership ability and intelligence. If college students hold negative stereotypes of feminists, feminist identified women are expected to be rated lower on levels of attractiveness, warmth, compassion and cooperativeness, but higher in leadership ability, ambition, and intelligence. Results demonstrated that participants did not hold negative stereotypes of feminists as they rated them similarly to environmentalists, progressives, and liberals. Results demonstrated that participants held negative stereotypes about conservatives and republicans.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

French, Rebecca E. "Interactive Influences of Narcissism and Gender Stereotypes on Insecure Attachment." Thesis, Alliant International University, 2018. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10813990.

Full text
Abstract:

Narcissism is a well-known psychological construct that bears implications for personality, development, adjustment, and relationships. Insecure attachment is also a part of well-developed psychoanalytic theory in psychology. Much research has been conducted on the two constructs, but little has been empirically discovered about how the two relate to each other, particularly for the developing age group of early adolescence. Morf and Rhodewalt [Psychological Inquiry, 12, 4, (2001)] propose that narcissism and insecure attachment are associated, and that whereas narcissistic boys are more likely to report an avoidant attachment, narcissistic girls are more likely to report an anxious attachment. Further, the associations between narcissism and insecure attachment may hinge on the degree to which individuals have internalized prevailing gender stereotypes regarding attachment styles (i.e., that an avoidant style is normative for boys and that an anxious style is normative for girls). A sample of early adolescents (N = 159, 77 boys, 82 girls, Mean age = 12.05 years) responded to measures of narcissism, own-gender stereotypes in attachment styles, and insecure relationship styles to a close friend. Results from hierarchical regression analyses countered what was hypothesized, finding that narcissism negatively predicated the avoidant style, and that gender did not moderate this relationship. Further, results indicated that for the anxious style, narcissistic children who endorsed having an own-gender stereotype of the anxious style were less likely to endorse having an anxious style. Results suggest that there may be something unique about attachment to a close friend and narcissism in early adolescence that warrants further investigation.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Dobish, Heidi B. "Emotion and age-related stereotypes and their social consequences /." Thesis, Connect to Dissertations & Theses @ Tufts University, 2004.

Find full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Tufts University, 2004.
Adviser: Robin Kanarek. Submitted to the Dept. of Experimental Psychology. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 47-53). Access restricted to members of the Tufts University community. Also available via the World Wide Web;
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Debeljak, Anne F. "Confronting Stereotypes: Integrating the Social Issue of Stereotypes Within the Art Curriculum." Ohio : Ohio University, 2009. http://www.ohiolink.edu/etd/view.cgi?ohiou1244121211.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Chan, Xinni. "A Cross-Cultural Investigation of Group Stereotypes." University of Toledo / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=toledo1372072907.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

McFadyen, Ruth Gunn. "Conflicts and consistencies in stereotypes and identities of unemployed people." Thesis, University of Cambridge, 1990. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.239519.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Cicchirillo, Vincent J. "The effects of priming racial stereotypes through violent video games." Columbus, Ohio : Ohio State University, 2009. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view.cgi?acc%5Fnum=osu1243867231.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Haught, Heather M. "Implications of Self for Content and Process Models of Stereotypes." University of Toledo / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=toledo1363792134.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Monte, Erica D. "Sex-role Stereotypes: How Far Have We Come?" PDXScholar, 1995. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/4945.

Full text
Abstract:
Parents are the first source of a child's learning of her or his gender. In fact, sex-role stereotyping of infants by parents may occur within the first 24 hours of birth. This study examined the nature of parental stereotyping on the basis of their infant's sex by obtaining parents' descriptions of their newborn and toy and clothing preferences for their newborn. In 1974, Rubin found that parents responded stereotypically to their infants on the basis of sex. Following Rubin's interview approach, 50 parent pairs from two urban hospitals were asked to participate in a parent-infant study and were subsequently interviewed 24 hours postpartum. Parents were asked open-ended descriptive questions about their newborn, given a semantic differential scale of 18 bi-polar objectives, asked about the importance of others recognizing their baby's sex, and asked a set of questions relating to the preference of clothing and toy choices for their newborn. Findings suggest that parents do stereotype their infants on the basis of biological sex. Sons were more likely to be described as strong, perfect, big or big-featured and energetic,--while daughters received more descriptions that mentioned their eyes, skin, or facial features and were also more likely to be described as small, tiny, or weak. Parents of boys were also more likely to state a preference for gender-specific toys and clothing. Infant sex did not make a notable difference on the importance that parents attributed to others recognizing their baby's sex. Fathers were more likely to perceive and describe their daughters more stereotypically than were mothers of either daughters or sons. Further studies to investigate gender stereotyping and its consequences as well as the interplay between the macro and micro levels of gender relations in society are suggested.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Gill, Michael Joseph. "Rethinking social stereotypes : moving from individual-directed to group-directed thought and behavior /." Digital version accessible at:, 1998. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/utexas/main.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Elizaga, Ronald A. "Eliciting stereotype challenge and stereotype threat effects within the context of women's math performance." Ohio : Ohio University, 2005. http://www.ohiolink.edu/etd/view.cgi?ohiou1132057268.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Ryan, John Patrick. "Thinking, Feeling and Discriminating: The Role of Prejudice as a Mediator between Stereotypes and Discrimination." unrestricted, 2006. http://etd.gsu.edu/theses/available/etd-07212006-164828/.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (M.A.)--Georgia State University, 2006.
Title from title screen. Eric Vanman, committee chair; David Washburn, Tracie Stewart, committee members. Electronic text (64 p.) : digital, PDF file. Description based on contents viewed June 13, 2007. Includes bibliographical references (p. 32-43).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

McMahon, Jean Marie. "Benevolent Sexism and Racial Stereotypes: Targets, Functions, and Consequences." PDXScholar, 2018. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/4227.

Full text
Abstract:
In this dissertation, I present three manuscripts in which I integrate race into an ambivalent sexism framework using experimental, correlational, and cross-sectional methods. The first paper tests whether a female's race acts as a subtype to differentially elicit benevolent sexism (BS). Two experiments demonstrated that BS is more strongly associated with White women than Black women. The second paper explores the relationship between protective paternalism (a subcomponent of BS), anti-minority attitudes, and threat. Threat was associated with stronger endorsement of protective paternalism and a corresponding increase in anti-minority attitudes, particularly for White men, implicating BS in the maintenance of racial inequality. Finally, my third study investigated potential real-world consequences of the differential application of BS to Black and White women in the context of police responses to intimate partner violence (IPV). Officers were more likely to file supplemental paperwork for White victims than Black victims, and were most likely to do so when encountering a White victim and a Black suspect. White victims were also written about with a greater "risk focus", consistent with BS. In sum, chapter II establishes racial differences in who receives BS, chapter III demonstrates how paternalistic protections of White women are racialized, and chapter IV reveals how the intersection of BS with racial stereotypes may impact women seeking help from police. This dissertation is the first investigation in the social psychological literature of how race informs the targets, function, and consequences of BS.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Aktan, Timucin. "Compensatory Nature Of Mixed Stereotypes: An Investigation Of Underlying Mechanisms In The Framework Of Stereotype Content Model." Phd thesis, METU, 2012. http://etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12614349/index.pdf.

Full text
Abstract:
The present dissertation aims to investigate cognitive and motivational underpinnings of stereotype contents in differing contexts. This dissertation consisted of two related sections. In the first section, comparison was suggested as the cognitive process underlying the implicit competence and warmth attributions toward businesswomen and homemakers. Four studies using Go/No Go Association Task were conducted to investigate the comparison process. Findings of the studies indicated that comparison has a significant impact on implicit mixed stereotypes. Implicit mixed stereotypes were not observed when target groups and attributes were presented in non-comparative context (Study 1). However, implicit stereotype contents were obvious in comparative context (Study 2). Finally, implicit stereotype contents of homemakers and businesswomen were shaped in accordance to the part of the context that was comparative (i.e. group comparison in Study 3 and attribute comparison in Study4). In the second section of the dissertation, comparison process was related to individuals&rsquo
compensation tendency. Two studies were conducted to examine the compensation tendency in the framework of System Justification Theory. In the first study (Study 5), presentation order of the target groups was manipulated. By this way, participants were not aware of the second group. Findings indicated that participants tended to compensate their first ratings toward homemakers and businesswomen. Furthermore, ambivalent sexism moderated the compensation tendency. In the second study (Study 6), both groups were presented together. Neither order of presentation nor its interactions were significant. Findings of the studies were discussed in the light of relevant literature.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Araya, Tadesse. "Stereotypes: Suppression, Forgetting, and False Memory." Doctoral thesis, Uppsala University, Department of Psychology, 2003. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-3340.

Full text
Abstract:

This thesis presents four studies investigating (1) whether incidentally primed control-related words can attenuate the impact of activated stereotypes on subsequent evaluation of a target person, (2) the impact of motivated forgetting on the recall of stereotypically congruent and incongruent information, and (3) the impact of a directed forgetting instruction on the false recall and recognition of nonpresented stereotypical information.

In three experiments, Study I showed that participants initially primed with the social category, immigrant, and subsequently primed with words that were evocative of control or self-control made less negative impression of a target displaying ambiguous behaviors than participants not exposed to such words.

Study II, using a directed-forgetting paradigm, demonstrated in two experiments that participants subliminally primed with Swedish facial photographs who later studied stereotypically incongruent words roughly recalled an equal number of items regardless of the forget or remember instructions.

Study III showed that participants primed with the social category, immigrant and then studied a list of stereotypically related and unrelated words falsely recognized more nonpresented stereotypical words when they were furnished with a forget than a remember instruction. Similarly, Study IV (Experiment 2) demonstrated that participants primed with the social category, immigrant, but not with a neutral category, falsely recalled more nonpresented stereotypical words when their cognitive capacity was depleted through a concurrent memory load task.

The thesis presents a review and a discussion of some of the theoretical underpinnings of the extant literature on stereotyping and intergroup relations and of the social implications of the present findings.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Hill, Sara Elizabeth. "Two-Year-Olds' Discrimination of Gender-Stereotyped Activities." Diss., CLICK HERE for online access, 2006. http://contentdm.lib.byu.edu/ETD/image/etd1226.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Mitchell, Erica Rachel. "Instruction type and stereotype threat in analytical reasoning: Can creativity help?" CSUSB ScholarWorks, 2008. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd-project/3362.

Full text
Abstract:
Stereotype threat is the fear that a person's behavior or performance will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which that person identifies. The purpose of this study was to look at the effect of instruction and emphasis on the female performance on an analytical reasoning task. This study tested undergraduate students taking a psychology course from California State University at San Bernardino. In this thesis, the task was framed as either an analytical reasoning task, a creative reasoning task, or there was no framing present. This study found that performance did differ as a result of instruction type, with creative instruction yielding higher scores. Varying instruction type performance can improve performance on an analytical reasoning task.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Cook, Jonathan E. "Social stigma and subjective power in naturalistic social interaction /." view abstract or download file of text, 2007. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1400960581&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=11238&RQT=309&VName=PQD.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Oregon, 2007.
Typescript. Includes vita and abstract. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 100-107). Also available for download via the World Wide Web; free to University of Oregon users.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Zhang, Yunying. "Stereotypes of and discrimination against racial/ethnic minorities can media exposure help change people's racial/ethnic prejudice for the better or for the worse? /." Online access for everyone, 2005. http://www.dissertations.wsu.edu/Thesis/Summer2005/y%5Fzhang%5F062705.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Rosenblum, Ari M. "Gender Nonconformity and the Stereotype Content Model." Oberlin College Honors Theses / OhioLINK, 2018. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=oberlin1528377926660424.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

White, Katherine R. "Attention allocation and the variability of the stereotype priming effect." To access this resource online via ProQuest Dissertations and Theses @ UTEP, 2009. http://0-proquest.umi.com.lib.utep.edu/login?COPT=REJTPTU0YmImSU5UPTAmVkVSPTI=&clientId=2515.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Poore, Abigail G. "The interpersonal consequences of confronting the nonprejudiced self." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1998. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape11/PQDD_0002/MQ44250.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Bhaju, Jeshmin O'Leary Virginia E. Blashfield Roger K. "Stigma based on race and mental illness a diagnostic double whammy /." Auburn, Ala, 2008. http://repo.lib.auburn.edu/EtdRoot/2008/SUMMER/Psychology/Dissertation/Bhaju_Jeshmin_40.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Kuchynka, Sophie Lois. "System Threats and Gender Differences in Sexism and Gender Stereotypes." Scholar Commons, 2015. http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/5720.

Full text
Abstract:
In the United States, women’s persistent gains in structural power may cause backlash among those motivated to preserve the status quo. The proposed study examines the conditions that prompt men and women to endorse sexism and promote gender stereotypes. System justification theory proposes that people are motivated to justify the socio-political system that governs them and threats to the stability of their system can increase individual’s motivated defenses. I expect men to show the strongest motivated defenses when the hierarchy is threatened or viewed as unstable, because to protect group-based interests men will reinforce the legitimacy of the system through stronger endorsement of system defenses. In contrast, women will show the strongest system defenses when the hierarchy is viewed as stable, to avoid feeling trapped in an unchanging system that oppresses them. To test these ideas, 430 men and women were exposed to a gender status hierarchy that was portrayed as stable or unstable and then they responded to several measures of sexism and gender stereotypes. Support for the hypothesis was only found on one measure of gender stereotypes. Men reported more system justifying stereotypes of traditional women in the unstable condition, while women showed the opposite pattern. Exploratory results demonstrate that men’s and women’s reports of agentic stereotypes for traditional and nontraditional women depended on whether they were exposed to a stable or unstable gender hierarchy. Future directions and limitations are discussed in consideration of these exploratory findings.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Wilmot, Gregory Thomas Charlton. "Student-athletes' attitude formation towards sport or other Psychology services /." Thesis, Rhodes University, 2009. http://eprints.ru.ac.za/1569/.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Rush, Ladonna Lewis. "Stereotyping in black and white : differences in stereotype knowledge and stereotype use /." Full-text version available from OU Domain via ProQuest Digital Dissertations, 1997.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Asztalos, Joanne G. "Gender stereotypes in children's television commercials and the effects on consumer purchasing behavior." Morgantown, W. Va. : [West Virginia University Libraries], 2003. http://etd.wvu.edu/templates/showETD.cfm?recnum=3209.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Gyll, Sean Paul. "The automatic activation of ethnic stereotypes in a simple cognitive task." CSUSB ScholarWorks, 1997. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd-project/1303.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Nelson, Andrew A. "Too Pretty for Homework: The Academic Correlates of Sexualized Gender Stereotypes Among Adolescent Girls." UKnowledge, 2017. http://uknowledge.uky.edu/psychology_etds/115.

Full text
Abstract:
Girls grow up in a culture of ubiquitous female sexualization, and this culture propagates stereotypes that could interfere with their academic outcomes. The current study examined the academic correlates of these sexualized gender stereotypes (SGS) among early adolescent girls. Girls (N = 99) aged 11 to 14 (Mage = 12.4 years, SD = .57 years) completed a survey assessing their academic performance, attitudes, and beliefs. The survey also assessed the degree to which girls believed that boys and girls should act in accordance with these sexualized gender stereotypes. Results indicated that higher endorsement of sexualized gender stereotypes was associated with lower academic performance, more negative academic attitudes, and less adaptive approaches to learning. Implications for girls’ academic trajectories are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Ongna, Alison M. "Occupational gender role stereotypes and career choice of young children." Menomonie, WI : University of Wisconsin--Stout, 2007. http://www.uwstout.edu/lib/thesis/2007/2007ongnaa.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Corcoran, Mayia. "Evaluating the negative impact of gender stereotypes on women's advancement in organizations." Online version, 2009. http://www.uwstout.edu/lib/thesis/2009/2009corcoranm.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Mellott, Deborah S. "Measuring implicit attitudes and stereotypes : increasing internal consistency reveals the convergent validity of IAT and priming measures /." Thesis, Connect to this title online; UW restricted, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/1773/9169.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Stone, Mary L. "Do Prospective Ratings Correct Retrospective Distortions Based on Negative Social Stereotypes of Premenstrual Syndrome?" DigitalCommons@USU, 1994. https://digitalcommons.usu.edu/etd/6054.

Full text
Abstract:
The Daily Assessment Form (OAF) retrospectively assessed symptoms of late luteal phase dysphoric disorder (LLPDD), both at the beginning and at the end of a 2-month time interval. Ninety-four women between the ages of 18 and 45 entered the study. Half of the subjects viewed a negative-case, stereotypic presentation of LLPDD prior to the pretest. sixty-eight subjects qualified on the pretest to complete the study. Half of the remaining no-case subjects and half of the remaining negative-case subjects completed 8 weeks' prospective ratings via the OAF. Forty-eight subjects remained to complete the posttest at the end of that time interval. Pretest Total and Pretest Criteria scores revealed significant main effects for case-presentation condition (E[l,63] = 7.08, R = .01) and (E[l,63] = 8.34, R = .01) and completion level (E[l,63] = 6.76, R = .01) and (E[l,63] = 3.76, R .06). Effect sizes equalled 0.48 and 0.45 for case presentation and 1.92 and 1.98 for completion.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Pertiwi, Yopina Galih. "How Does Intergroup Contact Predict Stereotypes in a Complex Social Reality?A Cross-Cultural Study of Intergroup Contact, Stereotypes, and Group Status." University of Toledo / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=toledo1461963876.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Smurda, Julie Dreon. "The influence of possible selves on perceived similarity and the tendency towards stereotyping and projection." Diss., Restricted to subscribing institutions, 2007. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1472128071&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=1564&RQT=309&VName=PQD.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

DuBois, Kate J. "Bidding at the prison auction house an exchange of prisoners' and university students' perceptions of crime and punishment /." Morgantown, W. Va. : [West Virginia University Libraries], 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/10450/10070.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (M.A.)--West Virginia University, 2009.
Title from document title page. Document formatted into pages; contains iii, 60 p. : col. ill. Vita. Includes abstract. Includes bibliographical references (p. 52-54).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

McKee, Stephanie E. "MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF THE HOMELESS." UKnowledge, 2018. https://uknowledge.uky.edu/psychology_etds/132.

Full text
Abstract:
Stereotypes surrounding race and socioeconomic status often have overlapping attributes. That is, we tend to stereotypically associate African Americans and poor individuals with being incompetent. Further, people automatically associate African Americans with the concept of poor. The current research examined people’s mental representations of a homeless person, a poor person, and a person with a home, to see if people’s mental representation of a homeless varied from that of a poor person. Results from Study 1 (N = 524), using a bi-racial base image indicate that people, on average, mentally represent the poor and homeless in a similar manner. The results from Study 2 (N = 496), using a White base image, replicate the findings from Study 1, and indicate that the findings from Study 1 were not the result of idiosyncratic features of the original base image. Future directions are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Rucks, Lana Juliette. "Me, women, and math the role of personal and collective threats in the experience of stereotype threat /." Columbus, Ohio : Ohio State University, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=osu1204661976.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Yeung, Nai Chi Psychology Faculty of Science UNSW. "Stereotype threat behind the wheel." Awarded by:University of New South Wales. School of Psychology, 2006. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/26242.

Full text
Abstract:
Stereotype threat refers to the pressure that individuals feel when they are at risk of confirming a demeaning stereotype about themselves. Research has found that stereotype threat impairs performance on cognitive-based tasks by inducing mental interference (e.g., Schmader & Johns, 2003). This thesis hypothesised that this finding would generalise to driving and that drivers who are better able to inhibit cognitive interference (i.e., with better inhibitory ability) would be less susceptible to the disruptive effect of stereotype threat than drivers who are less able (i.e., with poorer inhibitory ability). A series of three experiments conducted in a driving simulator tested the predictions using the gender stereotype of driving skills and investigated the interpretation of the results. The experiments revealed that stereotype threat exerted both a facilitative and debilitative influence on driving performance, as indicated by different performance measures. The facilitative effect diminished when drivers experienced increased mental demands or when they were assessed by an unexpected performance measure, while the debilitative effect was more likely observed among drivers who received negative feedback than drivers who received positive feedback. Moreover, the results supported the prediction that inhibitory ability would moderate the detrimental impact of stereotype threat as the performance of drivers with poorer inhibitory ability was impeded more than that of drivers with better inhibitory ability. Regarding the processes underlying the present findings, the experiments provided suggestive evidence that stereotype threat elicits cognitive interference and simultaneously motivates drivers to concentrate on particular performance areas in an attempt to refute the stereotype. In combination, these processes appear to be at least partly responsible for the performance deficits and boosts observed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Reyes, Rosanna. "Cultivation theory and stereotypes of latinidad in Desperate housewives." To access this resource online via ProQuest Dissertations and Theses @ UTEP, 2008. http://0-proquest.umi.com.lib.utep.edu/login?COPT=REJTPTU0YmImSU5UPTAmVkVSPTI=&clientId=2515.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Prasad, Ambika Marshall Linda L. "Stereotype threat in India gender and leadership choices /." [Denton, Tex.] : University of North Texas, 2007. http://digital.library.unt.edu/permalink/meta-dc-5128.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Vaughn-Blount, Kelli M. "Psychologist-historians : historying women & benevolent sexism /." Read thesis online Read thesis appendix online, 2008. http://library.uco.edu/UCOthesis/Vaughn-BlountKM2008.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography