Journal articles on the topic 'Spatial perception'

To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Spatial perception.

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Spatial perception.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Amelia, Risky, Ichsan Rauf, Abdul Gaus, Mufti Sultan Amir, and Hernita Pasongli. "Public Perception of Waste Transportation in Ternate City." Jurnal Spatial Wahana Komunikasi dan Informasi Geografi 22, no. 2 (December 8, 2022): 138–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.21009/spatial.222.07.

Full text
Abstract:
The enviroment is something that is important for living things, especially humans. The enviroment is used by humans to meet the needs of life, if it does not take care of the enviroment properly it will cause enviromental problems. There are many environmental problems, one of which is the waste problem. The problem of waste is a problem that often occurs in urban areas, one of which is in the city of Ternate. The Ternate City Government needs tochoose the right solution to overcome the problems that occur due to waste, but before making a solution it is necessary to know how the public’s perception of waste transportation in Ternate City is. The research method is descriptive quantitative using a questionnaire distributed to respondents as a research instrument. The sampling technique used in this research is random sampling with a sample of 400 respondents in four districts in Ternate City. The sample of each sub-district amounted to 100 respondents. Data analysis using interpreted percentages. The results showed that the public’s perception of waste transportation in Ternate City was good.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Frassinetti, Francesca, Barbara Magnani, and Massimiliano Oliveri. "Prismatic Lenses Shift Time Perception." Psychological Science 20, no. 8 (August 2009): 949–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02390.x.

Full text
Abstract:
Previous studies have demonstrated the involvement of spatial codes in the representation of time and numbers. We took advantage of a well-known spatial modulation (prismatic adaptation) to test the hypothesis that the representation of time is spatially oriented from left to right, with smaller time intervals being represented to the left of larger time intervals. Healthy subjects performed a time-reproduction task and a time-bisection task, before and after leftward and rightward prismatic adaptation. Results showed that prismatic adaptation inducing a rightward orientation of spatial attention produced an overestimation of time intervals, whereas prismatic adaptation inducing a leftward shift of spatial attention produced an underestimation of time intervals. These findings not only confirm that temporal intervals are represented as horizontally arranged in space, but also reveal that spatial modulation of time processing most likely occurs via cuing of spatial attention, and that spatial attention can influence the spatial coding of quantity in different dimensions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Green, E. J., and Susanna Schellenberg. "Spatial perception: The perspectival aspect of perception." Philosophy Compass 13, no. 2 (December 11, 2017): e12472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12472.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Jordan, J. Scott, and Günther Knoblich. "Spatial perception and control." Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 11, no. 1 (February 2004): 54–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03206460.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Basso, Demis. "Spatial perception and knowledge." Cognitive Processing 9, no. 2 (April 15, 2008): 81–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-008-0209-z.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Swanston, Michael. "Spatial motion perception requires the perception of distance." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17, no. 2 (June 1994): 334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00034890.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Meng, J. C., and H. A. Sedgwick. "Distance perception across spatial discontinuities." Perception & Psychophysics 64, no. 1 (January 2002): 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03194553.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Kushiro, Keisuke. "Spatial perception and vestibular function." Journal of Physical Fitness and Sports Medicine 1, no. 3 (2012): 547–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.7600/jpfsm.1.547.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Kappers, Astrid M. L., and Jan J. Koenderink. "Haptic Perception of Spatial Relations." Perception 28, no. 6 (June 1999): 781–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p2930.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Lee, T. Y. Y., and D. H. Brainard. "Spatial integration and lightness perception." Journal of Vision 9, no. 14 (December 1, 2009): 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/9.14.62.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Blanke, Marius, Ludwig Harsch, Jonas Knöll, and Frank Bremmer. "Spatial perception during pursuit initiation." Vision Research 50, no. 24 (December 2010): 2714–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.037.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Block, Ned. "Tactile sensation via spatial perception." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, no. 7 (July 2003): 285–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00132-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Antell, Sue E. G., and Albert J. Caron. "Neonatal perception of spatial relationships." Infant Behavior and Development 8, no. 1 (January 1985): 15–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(85)80013-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Wenny, Lili Sudria, and Fanny Nuravianti. "Users’ Perception on Interior Design of Tarumanegara Knowledge Center Library." Buletin Al-Turas 27, no. 1 (January 30, 2021): 105–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.15408/bat.v27i1.15972.

Full text
Abstract:
This study aimed to know the users’ perception of spatial planning in the Tarumanagara Knowledge Center (TKC), Library of Tarumanegara University. It was a descriptive quantitative study that relied on the users’ perception on the spatial planning of the knowledge center as the primary sources. While, the sample involved in the study comprised 90 people who visited the knowledge center taken randomly. Data collection techniques used a Likert scale-based questionnaire distributed to the targeted visitors or users. The collected data were then analyzed using a descriptive statistic. The results showed that the user's perception of the principles in the library layout obtained an average score of 3.07 that meant Good. Perceptions of aspects in library spatial planning get an average score of 3.20 that meant Good. Library users' perceptions of patterns in library layout get an average score of 3.14 that meant Good. Library users' perceptions of the spatial library get an average score of 3.18 that meant Good. Library users' perceptions of library space arrangement get an average score of 2.82 that meant Good. While the final score of the overall average of 3.06 that felt into Good category. It can be concluded that Tarumanegara knowledge Center with its spatial planning was able to provide comfortable rooms and facilities that satisfied the users to access available sources.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Tobimatsu, Shozo. "Visual Gnosis and Face Perception." International Journal of Computational Models and Algorithms in Medicine 3, no. 4 (October 2012): 11–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijcmam.2012100102.

Full text
Abstract:
There are two major parallel pathways in humans: the parvocellular (P) and magnocellular (M) pathways. The former has excellent spatial resolution with color selectivity, while the latter shows excellent temporal resolution with high contrast sensitivity. Visual stimuli should be tailored to answer specific clinical and/or research questions. This chapter examines the neural mechanisms of face perception using event-related potentials (ERPs). Face stimuli of different spatial frequencies were used to investigate how low-spatial-frequency (LSF) and high-spatial-frequency (HSF) components of the face contribute to the identification and recognition of the face and facial expressions. The P100 component in the occipital area (Oz), the N170 in the posterior temporal region (T5/T6) and late components peaking at 270-390 ms (T5/T6) were analyzed. LSF enhanced P100, while N170 was augmented by HSF irrespective of facial expressions. This suggested that LSF is important for global processing of facial expressions, whereas HSF handles featural processing. There were significant amplitude differences between positive and negative LSF facial expressions in the early time windows of 270-310 ms. Subsequently, the amplitudes among negative HSF facial expressions differed significantly in the later time windows of 330–390 ms. Discrimination between positive and negative facial expressions precedes discrimination among different negative expressions in a sequential manner based on parallel visual channels. Interestingly, patients with schizophrenia showed decreased spatial frequency sensitivities for face processing. Taken together, the spatially filtered face images are useful for exploring face perception and recognition.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Kim, Ga-Young. "A Study on Spatial Perceptions and Behaviors through the Perception Phenomenon of the User - The Relationship between Spatial Perception and User Behavior -." Korean Institute of Interior Design Journal 22, no. 5 (October 31, 2013): 143–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.14774/jkiid.2013.22.5.143.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Yuan, Jing, Hang Gao, Yanlong Shen, and Guoqiang Ma. "Spatial Differentiation of Ecotourist Perceptions Based on the Random Forest Model: The Case of the Gansu Section of the Yellow River Basin." Land 13, no. 4 (April 22, 2024): 560. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/land13040560.

Full text
Abstract:
Ecotourism is vital for coordinating regional ecological protection with socio-economic development. The Gansu section of the Yellow River Basin is a typical ecologically fragile area in China, and it holds a distinctive position in ecological protection and high-quality development. This study explores spatial differentiation in ecotourist perceptions and their distinct effects on ecotourist satisfaction, revisitation, and recommendation. It uses four cities (Gannan, Linxia, Lanzhou, and Baiyin) in the Gansu section of the Yellow River (mainstream) as examples, employing a questionnaire survey to collect ecotourists’ perception data and applying a random forest model and one-way ANOVA for analysis. It was found that: (1) rich ecotourism potential exists in the Gansu section of the Yellow River Basin as an ecologically fragile area; (2) there is spatial differentiation in ecotourist perceptions, and among the four regions, Baiyin stands out for its nature and atmosphere perception, and Lanzhou excels in accessibility and service perception; (3) spatial disparities exist in the influencing factors of ecotourist satisfaction, revisitation, and recommendation. Ecotourists in districts with unique natural resources, such as Gannan and Baiyin, prioritize nature perception, whereas districts with abundant natural resources and an established foundation for ecotourism development, such as Linxia and Lanzhou, emphasize service and atmosphere perception. This study constructs a new research framework to explore spatial variations in ecotourists’ perceptions, assisting ecotourism destinations to meet the needs of ecotourists from the supply side, and presents distinctive strategies and recommendations for the development of ecotourism in similar ecologically fragile areas.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Cañal-Bruland, Rouwen, and John van der Kamp. "Embodied Perception: A Proposal to Reconcile Affordance and Spatial Perception." i-Perception 6, no. 2 (January 2015): 63–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0709jc.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Yin, Xiaoyan, Xin Han, and Taeyeol Jung. "Analysis of spatial perception and the influencing factors of attractions in Southwest China’s ethnic minority areas: The case of Dali Bai Autonomous Prefecture." PLOS ONE 18, no. 6 (June 13, 2023): e0285141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0285141.

Full text
Abstract:
As standards of material living continue to improve and urbanization advances, an increasing number of remote ethnic minority areas are becoming tourist destinations. Understanding tourists’ perceptions on a large scale is thus crucial for the development of the regional tourism industry. However, traditional research methods suffer from high costs, small sample sizes, and low efficiency, making it difficult to measure the spatial perception of remote areas on a large scale. This study constructs a research framework for spatial perception measurement of remote ethnic minority areas by collecting reviews data from Ctrip using spatiotemporal data calculation and the Geodetector model. We considered Dali Prefecture as an empirical case and analyzed tourists’ perceptions of the area’s attractions, the spatial distribution of the attractions, and the process of change in the explanatory power of their influencing factors over an eight-year period (2014–2021). The results indicated that the most visited attractions were concentrated in Dali City. The perception of humanistic resources (attractions) with historical value was the highest, followed by natural resources. The high perception of attractions was influenced by the level of tourism development, traffic accessibility and attractiveness, and had an increasing influence on tourists’ perceptions over time. Additionally, changes in the mode of transportation from road to high-speed rail played an important role in the selection of tourist attractions. Conversely, the tourists paid relatively less attention to humanistic resources (e.g., national cultural heritage protection units and traditional villages). Our study provides a basis for the measurement of spatial perception in remote minority areas and can be used as a reference for tourism development planning in Dali Prefecture, thus promoting the sustainable development of tourism in the area.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Brigner, Willard L., and R. W. Berks. "High Spatial Frequencies Inhibit Motion Perception." Perceptual and Motor Skills 61, no. 3 (December 1985): 853–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1985.61.3.853.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Royer, Michael P., and Kevin W. Houser. "Spatial Brightness Perception of Trichromatic Stimuli." LEUKOS 9, no. 2 (October 1, 2012): 89–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1582/leukos.2012.09.02.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Pu, Kyle Z., Zixuan Wang, and David Whitney. "Spatial Heterogeneity of Biological Motion Perception." Journal of Vision 21, no. 9 (September 27, 2021): 2179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.9.2179.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Neale, Dennis C. "Spatial Perception in Desktop Virtual Environments." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 40, no. 22 (October 1996): 1117–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193129604002202.

Full text
Abstract:
This study investigated perceptual and cognitive issues relating to manipulations of geometric field of view (GFOV) in three-dimensional perspective displays and the effects of incorporating virtual environment enhancements in the interface based on visual momentum (VM) techniques. Sixty participants, who were pretested for spatial ability, were required to navigate through a virtual office building while estimating space dimensions and performing spatial orientation tasks. A 3 − 2 − 2 mixed-subjects design compared three levels of GFOV, two levels of VM, and two levels of Difficulty. This study effectively demonstrates that the spatial characteristics of architectural representations in perspective displays are not always accurately perceived. Furthermore, the results indicate that manipulations in GFOV can produce perceptual and cognitive errors for the basic space dimensions in perspective displays; however, VM can be used to compensate for many of the biases shown to occur.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Israel, M., and A. Cohen. "Spatial attention across perception and action." Journal of Vision 14, no. 10 (August 22, 2014): 530. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/14.10.530.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Jennings, Ben, Yinan Yu, and Frederick Kingdom. "Emotional face perception and spatial frequency." Journal of Vision 17, no. 10 (August 31, 2017): 825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/17.10.825.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Risucci, Donald A. "Visual spatial perception and surgical competence." American Journal of Surgery 184, no. 3 (September 2002): 291–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0002-9610(02)00937-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Dakin, Steven C., and Robert F. Hess. "The spatial mechanisms mediating symmetry perception." Vision Research 37, no. 20 (October 1997): 2915–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00031-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Kranjec, A., and G. Lupyan. "Implicit verbal categories modulate spatial perception." Journal of Vision 10, no. 7 (August 17, 2010): 1328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/10.7.1328.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Newport, Roger, Benjamin Rabb, and Stephen R. Jackson. "Noninformative Vision Improves Haptic Spatial Perception." Current Biology 12, no. 19 (October 2002): 1661–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0960-9822(02)01178-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Coleman, Sharon L., and Albert J. Gotch. "Spatial Perception Skills of Chemistry Students." Journal of Chemical Education 75, no. 2 (February 1998): 206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed075p206.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Lacey, Simon, Randall Stilla, Karthik Sreenivasan, Gopikrishna Deshpande, and K. Sathian. "Spatial imagery in haptic shape perception." Neuropsychologia 60 (July 2014): 144–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Nishizawa, Sho, and Carol A. Saslow. "Lateralization of Kinesthetically Guided Spatial Perception." Cortex 23, no. 3 (September 1987): 485–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(87)80009-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Glasauer, Stefan, and Horst Mittelstaedt. "Perception of spatial orientation in microgravity." Brain Research Reviews 28, no. 1-2 (November 1998): 185–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00038-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Bailey, Andrew. "Spatial Perception, Embodiment, and Scientific Realism." Dialogue 46, no. 3 (2007): 553–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0012217300002055.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Gentaz, Edouard, Gabriel Baud-Bovy, and Marion Luyat. "The haptic perception of spatial orientations." Experimental Brain Research 187, no. 3 (April 30, 2008): 331–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1382-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Israel, Moran M., Pierre Jolicoeur, and Asher Cohen. "Spatial attention across perception and action." Psychological Research 82, no. 2 (October 24, 2016): 255–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0820-z.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Samad, Majed, and Ladan Shams. "Visual–somatotopic interactions in spatial perception." NeuroReport 27, no. 3 (February 2016): 180–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0000000000000521.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Keough, Megan, Murray Schellenberg, and Bryan Gick. "Spatial congruence in multimodal speech perception." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 140, no. 4 (October 2016): 3225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4970186.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Brümmer, Ludger. "Composition and Perception in Spatial Audio." Computer Music Journal 41, no. 1 (March 2017): 46–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/comj_a_00402.

Full text
Abstract:
This article discusses the advantages of spatial audio, in general, followed by strategies for applying spatial components to composition. The discussion then looks ahead to questions that may be solved by future implementations of spatial software and hardware. Despite the fact that technical systems for spatial audio have been in use since the 1950s, spatial concepts have not been widely integrated into the compositional process. This is because they involve a complex interaction of several phenomena, all of which play a role in the construction and perception of music. This article presents an analysis of the advantages of spatial audio for perception and provides examples of the decomposition of sonic material with the help of spatial properties, as well as a discussion of limitations in spatial construction and perception. Archival strategies for spatial audio are also briefly discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Humphreys, Glyn, M. Jane Riddoch, Sara Forti, and Katie Ackroyd. "Action influences spatial perception: Neuropsychological evidence." Visual Cognition 11, no. 2-3 (March 2004): 401–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13506280344000310.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Aytekin, Murat, Beatrice Mao, and Cynthia F. Moss. "Spatial perception and adaptive sonar behavior." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 128, no. 6 (December 2010): 3788–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.3504707.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Carrasco, Marisa. "How visual spatial attention alters perception." Cognitive Processing 19, S1 (July 30, 2018): 77–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-018-0883-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Aleci, Carlo, Giulio Piana, Marzia Piccoli, and Marco Bertolini. "Developmental dyslexia and spatial relationship perception." Cortex 48, no. 4 (April 2012): 466–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.10.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

MEERWALDT, J., and H. VANDONGEN. "Disturbances of spatial perception in children." Behavioural Brain Research 31, no. 2 (December 1, 1988): 131–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(88)90016-2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Nakashima, Ryoichi, and Takatsune Kumada. "Peripersonal versus extrapersonal visual scene information for egocentric direction and position perception." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 71, no. 5 (January 1, 2018): 1090–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1310267.

Full text
Abstract:
When perceiving the visual environment, people simultaneously perceive their own direction and position in the environment (i.e., egocentric spatial perception). This study investigated what visual information in a scene is necessary for egocentric spatial perceptions. In two perception tasks (the egocentric direction and position perception tasks), observers viewed two static road images presented sequentially. In Experiment 1, the critical manipulation involved an occluded region in the road image, an extrapersonal region (far-occlusion) and a peripersonal region (near-occlusion). Egocentric direction perception was worse in the far-occlusion condition than in the no-occlusion condition, and egocentric position perceptions were worse in the far- and near-occlusion conditions than in the no-occlusion condition. In Experiment 2, we conducted the same tasks manipulating the observers’ gaze location in a scene—an extrapersonal region (far-gaze), a peripersonal region (near-gaze) and the intermediate region between the former two (middle-gaze). Egocentric direction perception performance was the best in the far-gaze condition, and egocentric position perception performances were not different among gaze location conditions. These results suggest that egocentric direction perception is based on fine visual information about the extrapersonal region in a road landscape, and egocentric position perception is based on information about the entire visual scene.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Amelia, Ria R., and Dhany Arifianto. "Spatial cues on normal hearing and cochlear implant simulation with different coding strategies." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 152, no. 4 (October 2022): A90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/10.0015647.

Full text
Abstract:
Cochlear implant users are known to have limited access to spatial cues. This study investigated the perception of spatial cues in normal-hearing listeners and cochlear implant simulation users. Perception of spatial cues is assessed for performance in determining the direction of the sound and understanding the speech. The results show that cochlear implant simulation users still have access to spatial cues, just like normal- hearing listeners. Normal-hearing listeners and cochlear implant simulation users can perceive spatial cues in ILD and ITD. Both can accurately identify the direction of the sound (slope ≈ 1.00 and of set ≈ 0.00°). Cochlear implant simulation users can understand sentences as well as normal-hearing listeners (PCW = 113.64 rau) by using the coding strategy SPEAK in all channels or CIS with channel above 8. Perception of spatial cues in normal-hearing listeners and cochlear implant users can be improved by listening with two ears and spatially separating the target-masker position. The largest improvement in spatial cue perception was obtained from the head shadow effect (normal-hearing (NH) = 12.96, cochlear implant simulation users (CI) = 59.02), followed by binaural summation (NH = 5.72, CI = 19, 86) and binaural squelch (NH = 3.76, CI = 7.66).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

FUKE, SAWA, MASAKI OGINO, and MINORU ASADA. "BODY IMAGE CONSTRUCTED FROM MOTOR AND TACTILE IMAGES WITH VISUAL INFORMATION." International Journal of Humanoid Robotics 04, no. 02 (June 2007): 347–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s0219843607001096.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper proposes a learning model that enables a robot to acquire a body image for parts of its body that are invisible to itself. The model associates spatial perception based on motor experience and motor image with perception based on the activations of touch sensors and tactile image, both of which are supported by visual information. The tactile image can be acquired with the help of the motor image, which is thought to be the basis for spatial perception, because all spatial perceptions originate in motor experiences. Based on the proposed model, a robot estimates invisible hand positions using the Jacobian between the displacement of the joint angles and the optical flow of the hand. When the hand touches one of the invisible tactile sensor units on the face, the robot associates this sensor unit with the estimated hand position. The simulation results show that the spatial arrangement of tactile sensors is successfully acquired by the proposed model.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Ho, Jeffrey C. F. "Real-World and Virtual-World Practices for Virtual Reality Games: Effects on Spatial Perception and Game Performance." Multimodal Technologies and Interaction 4, no. 1 (January 1, 2020): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/mti4010001.

Full text
Abstract:
Researchers have been investigating ways to improve users’ spatial perception in virtual environments. Very limited studies have focused on the context of virtual reality (VR) games. Tutorials with practices, a common element in games, are good opportunities to implement measures that improve players’ spatial perception. Using an experiment, this paper investigates how two types of practices (real-world and virtual-world practices) influence players’ spatial perception, game performance, and immersion in VR games. Given that spatial perception is viewed as an essential aspect of VR applications, the moderating role of spatial perception on the effect of practices in game performance is also explored. The results demonstrate that virtual-world practice is effective in improving players’ spatial perception of the virtual environment of VR games. Real-world practice is suggested to be effective in enhancing spatial perception when it is averaged over multiple sessions. The results also suggest that spatial perception moderates the effects of practices on game performance. The results imply that practices in game tutorial can be a transitional environment for new players to enter a VR game.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Chen, Kui, Junjie Yang, Takahiro Katano, Mitsuhiro Kamezaki, and Shigeki Sugano. "Analysis of Spatial Perception Ability Based on Human Eyesight for Teleoperators." Abstracts of the international conference on advanced mechatronics : toward evolutionary fusion of IT and mechatronics : ICAM 2015.6 (2015): 351–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1299/jsmeicam.2015.6.351.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Cannon, Mark W. "A Model of Mechanisms Mediating Spatial Pattern Perception." Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting 36, no. 18 (October 1992): 1430–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193129203601815.

Full text
Abstract:
A model consisting of multiple tuned and oriented spatial filters followed by non-linear transducer functions is described. The model was originally derived to account for human perception of contrast while viewing isolated stimuli. The model can also account for human estimates for the image sharpness of spatially filtered real world scenes. The model has several shortcomings uncovered by recent experimental results involving suppression of the apparent contrast of a foveally presented grating patch by a peripheral grating.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography