Academic literature on the topic 'Sensorimotor experience, space, time'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Sensorimotor experience, space, time.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Sensorimotor experience, space, time"

1

Boroditsky, Lera, and Michael Ramscar. "The Roles of Body and Mind in Abstract Thought." Psychological Science 13, no. 2 (March 2002): 185–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00434.

Full text
Abstract:
How are people able to think about things they have never seen or touched? We demonstrate that abstract knowledge can be built analogically from more experience-based knowledge. People's understanding of the abstract domain of time, for example, is so intimately dependent on the more experience-based domain of space that when people make an air journey or wait in a lunch line, they also unwittingly (and dramatically) change their thinking about time. Further, our results suggest that it is not sensorimotor spatial experience per se that influences people's thinking about time, but rather people's representations of and thinking about their spatial experience.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Loeffler, Jonna, Markus Raab, and Rouwen Cañal-Bruland. "Walking Back to the Future." Experimental Psychology 64, no. 5 (September 2017): 346–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000377.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Embodied cognition frameworks suggest a direct link between sensorimotor experience and cognitive representations of concepts ( Shapiro, 2011 ). We examined whether this holds also true for concepts that cannot be directly perceived with the sensorimotor system (i.e., temporal concepts). To test this, participants learned object-space (Exp. 1) or object-time (Exp. 2) associations. Afterwards, participants were asked to assign the objects to their location in space/time meanwhile they walked backward, forward, or stood on a treadmill. We hypothesized that walking backward should facilitate the online processing of “behind”/“past”-related stimuli, but hinder the processing of “ahead”/“future”-related stimuli, and a reversed effect for forward walking. Indeed, “ahead”- and “future”-related stimuli were processed slower during backward walking. During forward walking and standing, stimuli were processed equally fast. The results provide partial evidence for the activation of specific spatial and temporal concepts by whole-body movements and are discussed in the context of movement familiarity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Grasso, Camille L., Johannes C. Ziegler, Jennifer T. Coull, and Marie Montant. "Embodied time: Effect of reading expertise on the spatial representation of past and future." PLOS ONE 17, no. 10 (October 27, 2022): e0276273. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276273.

Full text
Abstract:
How do people grasp the abstract concept of time? It has been argued that abstract concepts, such as future and past, are grounded in sensorimotor experience. When responses to words that refer to the past or the future are either spatially compatible or incompatible with a left-to-right timeline, a space-time congruency effect is observed. In the present study, we investigated whether reading expertise determines the strength of the space-time congruency effect, which would suggest that learning to read and write drives the effect. Using a temporal categorization task, we compared two types of space-time congruency effects, one where spatial incongruency was generated by the location of the stimuli on the screen and one where it was generated by the location of the responses on the keyboard. While the first type of incongruency was visuo-spatial only, the second involved the motor system. Results showed stronger space-time congruency effects for the second type of incongruency (i.e., when the motor system was involved) than for the first type (visuo-spatial). Crucially, reading expertise, as measured by a standardized reading test, predicted the size of the space-time congruency effects. Altogether, these results reinforce the claim that the spatial representation of time is partially mediated by the motor system and partially grounded in spatially-directed movement, such as reading or writing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Deng, Yu, Jixue Yang, Li Wang, and Yaokai Chen. "The Road Less Traveled: How COVID-19 Patients Use Metaphors to Frame Their Lived Experiences." International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 23 (November 30, 2022): 15979. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192315979.

Full text
Abstract:
Metaphor provides an important intellectual tool for communication about intense disease experiences. The present study aimed to investigate how COVID-19-infected persons metaphorically frame their lived experiences of COVID-19, and how the pandemic impacts on their mental health burden. In-depth semi-structured interviews were conducted with 33 patients afflicted with COVID-19. Metaphor analysis of patient narratives demonstrated that: (1) COVID-19 infection impacted patient conceptualization of themselves and the relationship between the “self” and the body, as well as social relationships. (2) Metaphors relating to physical experience, space and time, and integrative behaviors tended to be used by COVID-19 patients in a negative way, whereas war metaphors, family metaphors, temperature metaphors, and light metaphors were likely to express positive attitudes. (3) Patients preferred to employ conventional metaphors grounded on embodied sensorimotor experiences to conceptualize their extreme emotional experiences. This study has important implications with respect to the therapeutic function of metaphors in clinical communication between healthcare professionals and COVID-19 patients.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Tolkacheva, Anastasiya, and Ksenia Belogai. "Sensorimotor and Perceptual Processes in Children of Primary School Age with Multiple Developmental Disorders." Bulletin of Kemerovo State University. Series: Humanities and Social Sciences 2022, no. 3 (October 12, 2022): 163–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.21603/2542-1840-2022-6-3-163-171.

Full text
Abstract:
The article focuses on sensorimotor and perceptual processes in primary school children with multiple developmental disorders. The study involved 40 children aged 9–11 with multiple developmental issues. All the participants studied at the Secondary School of Psychological and Pedagogical Support No. 101 (Kemerovo, Russia). The experiment relied on the method developed by N. I. Ozeretskiy and M. O. Gurevich as the main diagnostic tool. The method combines a set of diagnostic tasks aimed at measuring the level of motor and perceptual development in children. The experimental study also included elements of the neuropsychological approach. The authors designed and conducted a series of practice sessions on the development of sensorimotor and perceptual processes. The neuropsychological exercises developed purposeful voluntary actions with objects and materials, improved available sensory experience, increased visual-motor coordination, sharpened the ability to navigate in various physical environments, and improved body control. They facilitated the compensation of residual reflexes, as well as the development of speech and general motor rhythmization. The exercises involved orthopedic mats, massage balls, tasks on visual-motor perception and integration, Balametrics cerebellar stimulation, etc. Statistic results showed a positive trend in the sensorimotor and perceptual processes, except for complex forms of space and time perception.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Jajdelska, Elspeth. "Being there yet not there: why don’t embodied responses to literary texts jar with one another?" Journal of Literary Semantics 45, no. 1 (January 1, 2016): 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/jls-2016-0002.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractLanguage and literature can stimulate the embodied resources of perception. I argue that there is a puzzle about why we experience sequences of these embodied responses as integrated and coherent, even though they are not anchored in space and time by a perceiving body. Some successions of embodied representations would even be impossible in real world experience, yet they can still be experienced as coherent and flowing in response to verbal texts. One possibility is that embodied responses to language are fleeting; they need not be integrated because they do not depend on, or relate to, one another as they would in perception. Yet it is the potential for embodied representations to linger and connect with one another which underlies new and persuasive embodied literary theories of vividness, narrative coherence and metaphor comprehension. Another possibility is that readers anchor their embodied representations in a notional human body, one endowed with superhuman powers, such as omniscience. But this account relies on implausible, post hoc explanations. A third possibility is that integrating embodied representations produced by language need be no more problematic than integrating the deceptively patchy information harvested from the environment by perception, information which gives rise to an experience of the world in rich and continuous detail. Real world perceptual cues, however, sparse though they might be, are still integrated through grounding in specific points in time and space. To explain the integration of embodied effects, I draw on sensorimotor theories of perception, and on Clark’s suggestion (1997,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Albert, Scott T., and Reza Shadmehr. "Estimating properties of the fast and slow adaptive processes during sensorimotor adaptation." Journal of Neurophysiology 119, no. 4 (April 1, 2018): 1367–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00197.2017.

Full text
Abstract:
Experience of a prediction error recruits multiple motor learning processes, some that learn strongly from error but have weak retention and some that learn weakly from error but exhibit strong retention. These processes are not generally observable but are inferred from their collective influence on behavior. Is there a robust way to uncover the hidden processes? A standard approach is to consider a state space model where the hidden states change following experience of error and then fit the model to the measured data by minimizing the squared error between measurement and model prediction. We found that this least-squares algorithm (LMSE) often yielded unrealistic predictions about the hidden states, possibly because of its neglect of the stochastic nature of error-based learning. We found that behavioral data during adaptation was better explained by a system in which both error-based learning and movement production were stochastic processes. To uncover the hidden states of learning, we developed a generalized expectation maximization (EM) algorithm. In simulation, we found that although LMSE tracked the measured data marginally better than EM, EM was far more accurate in unmasking the time courses and properties of the hidden states of learning. In a power analysis designed to measure the effect of an intervention on sensorimotor learning, EM significantly reduced the number of subjects that were required for effective hypothesis testing. In summary, we developed a new approach for analysis of data in sensorimotor experiments. The new algorithm improved the ability to uncover the multiple processes that contribute to learning from error. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Motor learning is supported by multiple adaptive processes, each with distinct error sensitivity and forgetting rates. We developed a generalized expectation maximization algorithm that uncovers these hidden processes in the context of modern sensorimotor learning experiments that include error-clamp trials and set breaks. The resulting toolbox may improve the ability to identify the properties of these hidden processes and reduce the number of subjects needed to test the effectiveness of interventions on sensorimotor learning.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Mitrokhin, A., P. Sutor, C. Fermüller, and Y. Aloimonos. "Learning sensorimotor control with neuromorphic sensors: Toward hyperdimensional active perception." Science Robotics 4, no. 30 (May 15, 2019): eaaw6736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/scirobotics.aaw6736.

Full text
Abstract:
The hallmark of modern robotics is the ability to directly fuse the platform’s perception with its motoric ability—the concept often referred to as “active perception.” Nevertheless, we find that action and perception are often kept in separated spaces, which is a consequence of traditional vision being frame based and only existing in the moment and motion being a continuous entity. This bridge is crossed by the dynamic vision sensor (DVS), a neuromorphic camera that can see the motion. We propose a method of encoding actions and perceptions together into a single space that is meaningful, semantically informed, and consistent by using hyperdimensional binary vectors (HBVs). We used DVS for visual perception and showed that the visual component can be bound with the system velocity to enable dynamic world perception, which creates an opportunity for real-time navigation and obstacle avoidance. Actions performed by an agent are directly bound to the perceptions experienced to form its own “memory.” Furthermore, because HBVs can encode entire histories of actions and perceptions—from atomic to arbitrary sequences—as constant-sized vectors, autoassociative memory was combined with deep learning paradigms for controls. We demonstrate these properties on a quadcopter drone ego-motion inference task and the MVSEC (multivehicle stereo event camera) dataset.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lachmair, Martin, Susana Ruiz Fernandez, Nils-Alexander Bury, Peter Gerjets, Martin H. Fischer, and Otmar L. Bock. "How Body Orientation Affects Concepts of Space, Time and Valence: Functional Relevance of Integrating Sensorimotor Experiences during Word Processing." PLOS ONE 11, no. 11 (November 3, 2016): e0165795. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165795.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Zarahn, Eric, Gregory D. Weston, Johnny Liang, Pietro Mazzoni, and John W. Krakauer. "Explaining Savings for Visuomotor Adaptation: Linear Time-Invariant State-Space Models Are Not Sufficient." Journal of Neurophysiology 100, no. 5 (November 2008): 2537–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.90529.2008.

Full text
Abstract:
Adaptation of the motor system to sensorimotor perturbations is a type of learning relevant for tool use and coping with an ever-changing body. Memory for motor adaptation can take the form of savings: an increase in the apparent rate constant of readaptation compared with that of initial adaptation. The assessment of savings is simplified if the sensory errors a subject experiences at the beginning of initial adaptation and the beginning of readaptation are the same. This can be accomplished by introducing either 1) a sufficiently small number of counterperturbation trials (counterperturbation paradigm [ CP]) or 2) a sufficiently large number of zero-perturbation trials (washout paradigm [ WO]) between initial adaptation and readaptation. A two-rate, linear time-invariant state-space model (SSMLTI,2) was recently shown to theoretically produce savings for CP. However, we reasoned from superposition that this model would be unable to explain savings for WO. Using the same task (planar reaching) and type of perturbation (visuomotor rotation), we found comparable savings for both CP and WO paradigms. Although SSMLTI,2 explained some degree of savings for CP it failed completely for WO. We conclude that for visuomotor rotation, savings in general is not simply a consequence of LTI dynamics. Instead savings for visuomotor rotation involves metalearning, which we show can be modeled as changes in system parameters across the phases of an adaptation experiment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Sensorimotor experience, space, time"

1

RINALDI, LUCA. "Sensorimotor experience biases human attention through space and time." Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/100579.

Full text
Abstract:
Space and time are fundamental dimensions that contribute to make human minds grounded in the physical world. Researchers across the cognitive sciences have recently addressed some key questions about the role of the sensorimotor system in spatial and temporal processing (Chapter 1). The present thesis adds to this debate by exploring the hypothesis that prior directional sensorimotor experience contributes to the human sense of space and time. The first part of the thesis investigates whether sensorimotor experience influences visuospatial attention. A first study shows that humans have a manual and ocular leftward bias in bisection task in near but not in far space (Chapter 2). This leftward bias, for long mainly explained in terms of a right hemispheric dominance in visuospatial processing, is modulated by directional routines. For instance, individuals from different cultures show visuospatial asymmetries that can predicted by their reading habits (Chapter 3). Similarly, exposure to formal education exerts a strong influence on children’s visuospatial attention (Chapter 4). Nonetheless, the impact of cultural routines is further constrained by situational requirements. In fact, bidirectional readers reorient their visual scanning depending on the language of the task at hand (Chapter 5). In line with this, visuospatial biases can be rapidly induced by learned contingent odor-object associations (Chapter 6). On these grounds, it is therefore suggested that biological factors (i.e., hemispheric specialization) interplay with both cultural (i.e., directional scanning associated with language processing) and situational factors (i.e., current constraints imposed by task demands) in modulating visuospatial attention, likely under a hierarchical relationship (Chapter 7). Since space and time are supposed to be tightly coupled in the human mind by motor actions, the second part of the thesis investigates whether sensorimotor experience influences the spatial representation of time. A first study shows that both finger counting and reading habits are flexibly exploited to map ordered information on the bodily space (Chapter 8). The sensorimotor involvement in representational processes was confirmed in a study showing that eye movements mediate the search and the retrieval of temporally ordered information (Chapter 9). In addition, the view that the egocentric representation of time originates from our walking experience was empirically supported by showing that temporal processing affects step movements along the sagittal space (Chapter 10). Finally, the systematic tendency to experience the future as psychologically closer than the past, derived from our experiential movement through time, was found to be altered in people with slower walking speed and distorted motion perception, i.e., anxious and depressed individuals. (Chapter 11). These studies, therefore, suggest that the processing of time is governed by the same mechanisms that orient our attention in the physical space (Chapter 12). Overall, this thesis indicates that prior sensorimotor experience affects the way humans attend to space and time (Chapter 13).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

RIZZI, EZIA. "A JOURNEY THROUGH TIME. SPACE-TIME REPRESENTATION IN ADULTS AND CHILDREN." Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2019. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/243942.

Full text
Abstract:
È noto che la mente umana spesso crea una rappresentazione del tempo attraverso dimensioni più concrete, come lo spazio. Abitualmente, parliamo del passato riferendoci allo spazio dietro di noi e al futuro allo spazio di fronte a noi. Questa tesi di dottorato esplora l'origine e lo sviluppo dell'associazione tra tempo e spazio nell'infanzia e nell'età adulta. La prima sezione fornisce una panoramica del background teorico e discute gli studi precedenti che si sono focalizzati su questo argomento. Descriviamo gli elementi mancanti e individuiamo che il tipo di informazioni elaborate (cioè gli eventi che si riferiscono alla memoria personale e non personale) possano influenzare la costruzione della linea del tempo mentale e i relativi fotogrammi spaziali di riferimento coinvolti. Il secondo capitolo sperimentale indaga direttamente se eventi personali e non personali sono mappati in modo diverso nello spazio in età adulta, coinvolgendo madrelingua italiana. I risultati descritti mostrano che mentre gli eventi personali sono mappati preferenzialmente lungo lo spazio sagittale, gli eventi non personali sono più probabilmente mappati sullo spazio orizzontale. Questi risultati sono stati replicati in un campione di madrelingua adulti inglesi utilizzando una procedura simile e indicano che il tipo di contenuto elaborato in memoria influisce sul modo in cui l'individuo rappresenta il tempo nello spazio. Il terzo capitolo è incentrato sull'ontogenesi della linea temporale mentale. Un primo studio ha esplorato la rappresentazione di eventi personali e non personali lungo lo spazio sagittale in bambini madrelingua inglese della scuola elementare, estendendo così la principale domanda teorica alla base di questa tesi a livello dello sviluppo. In un secondo studio, i bambini della scuola elementare italiana sono stati coinvolti in due compiti che esploravano le origini linguistiche e sensomotorie della linea mentale del tempo sagittale. I risultati indicano che la rappresentazione del tempo lungo lo spazio sagittale si basa fortemente su processi sensorimotori già in giovane età. Insieme, questo corpus di prove fornisce nuove intuizioni sui meccanismi cognitivi e sensomotori che guiderebbero gli umani a rappresentare il tempo lungo le coordinate spaziali.
It is well known that the human mind often creates a representation of time through more concrete dimensions, such as space. Habitually, we talk about past referring to the space behind us and about future referring to the space in front of us. This doctoral thesis explores the origin and development of the association between time and space in childhood and adulthood. The first section provides an overview of the theoretical background and discuss previous studies that have been focused on this topic. We outline the missing pieces of evidence and pinpoint that the type of information processed at hand (i.e., events referring to personal and non-personal memory) may impact on how the mental time line is constructed and on the relative spatial frames of reference involved. The second, empirical chapter investigates directly whether personal and non-personal events are differently mapped on space in adulthood, by involving native Italian speakers. The results described show that whereas personal events are preferentially mapped along the sagittal space, non-personal events are more likely mapped on the horizontal space. These findings were replicated in a sample of English adult speakers using a similar procedure and indicate that the type of content processed in memory affects how the individual represents time in space. The third chapter is focused on the ontogeny of the mental time line. A first study explored the representation of personal and non-personal events along the sagittal space in native English primary school children, thus extending the main theoretical question underlying this thesis at the developmental level. In a second study, Italian primary school children were involved in two tasks probing the linguistic and sensorimotor origins of the sagittal mental time line. Results indicate that the representation of time along the sagittal space strongly relies on sensorimotor processes already from a young age. Together, this body of evidence provides new insights on the cognitive and sensorimotor mechanisms that would drive humans to represent time along spatial coordinates.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Rivera, Monica Alexandra. "Slowing Down Time, studies on spatial time." Thesis, Virginia Tech, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/33992.

Full text
Abstract:
The experience of time is not fixed by a rigid mathematical measure, instead, it flows at vaying rates. There are certain occasions in which we would like to extend time with all our force, up to the limit of our stretched arms and further. Conversely there are moments which we'd like to last no more that the sparkle of a flash, but as we all have noticed, those are the longest in our life. How does the space that we inhabit influence on our perception of time? May we identify especial elements that contribute in one or other sense to accelerate or slowdown the time? It's said that time and space is an inseparable unity, as two aspects of the same thing. If this is so, then it also must be true that by shaping space in one way or another, we might influence the experience of time through it. Wouldn't it be delightful to believe that we may be magicians of time through manipulation of architecture?
Master of Architecture
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Purcell, Marisa. "Ancestral Spaces: Time, Memory and the Liminal Experience of Painting." University of Sydney, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/2123/2763.

Full text
Abstract:
Master of Visual Arts
Abstract of Dissertation Where a person is situated in space and time determines the way an artwork is perceived. The result of this experience implies a relationship between the viewer and the artwork, thereby creating a liminal space. The terms liminal space and nonduality in this paper refer to the threshold, or in-between space that both separates and unites two opposing forces, creating a unique place that transcends memory and time. An artwork can serve as a mediatory object between artist and viewer because with each encounter, a unique meeting occurs. Thus, the meeting of audience and art object is transitory, ephemeral and temporal by nature and will be discussed in relation to the artwork as a vehicle to foster a subjective perception. Using my ancestral memories as a starting point, I refer to the art object as a means to explore time as a cross section of experience. Like dreams, where time is non-linear and memories exist side by side, I refer to the nondual space that exists between artist, artwork and audience as an opportunity to access an intuitive reaction to perception. The yearning to represent subjective space stems from my desire to understand perception and the brain. By presenting an overview of approaches from art history and contemporary art, this paper will discuss the various philosophical approaches that have been employed to represent space and time. I emphasise the ability of visual art to record the multifarious nature of experience, and the ability of the picture plane as a means to employ illusory and abstract space simultaneously. I have approached the research of time, memory and space through the lens of my own ancestry, which is essentially a combination of eastern and western in origin. Through this model I explore the tendencies throughout art history to depict space and time and the influences that culture and science have had upon the visual arts. My own paintings, and the work of Louise Bourgeois, Amy Cutler and Mamma Andersson are discussed with the intention of describing how the subjectivity of space can be expressed through a method that embraces the theories of nonduality and liminal space. Between the junction of east/west and abstract/illusory space, lies a point of union that I will refer to as ‘transcendent space’. By existing in the nondual, access is granted into a field that transcends the ‘either/or’ and allows access into a temporal space that permeates all experience. Studio work The studio component of the MVA will comprise of a series of paintings and an installation entitled, Only the memories are new. The paintings are of small scale and play with depictions of flatness and illusion. I have referenced Arabic miniatures as a means to employ a vertical perspective, whilst the inclusion of windows and doorways imply an opening to the nondual and the liminal. For the installation, components of the paintings come to life and occupy a space that invites the viewers’ participation. The installation presents an environment that asks the viewer to navigate the space that they occupy by way of memory and time.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Purcell, Marisa. "Ancestral spaces time, memory and the liminal experience of painting /." Connect to full text, 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/2123/2763.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (M.V.A.)--University of Sydney, 2008.
Title from title screen (viewed 11 September, 2008). Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Visual Arts to the Sydney College of the Arts. Degree awarded 2008; thesis submitted 2007. Includes bibliographical references.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Badenhorst, Ursula. "The eschatological garden : sacred space, time and experience in the monastic cloister garden." Master's thesis, University of Cape Town, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/11427/11905.

Full text
Abstract:
Includes bibliographical references (p. 169-186).
The argument of this dissertation is that the garden can be considered a proleptic eschatological landscape outside of time. To prove this argument I pull together strands of philosophical reflections on death, history of religions analysis concerning sacred space and time and monastic spirituality. I develop this argument by focusing on the enclosed garden, which has connected with it, in myth and metaphor, abundant meanings concerning life after death in a paradisiacal state of bliss. These meanings also become evident in the physical layout of the garden, which, when analyzing it in terms of substantial and situational definitions of sacred space, becomes a prime example of a sacred space, linked physically and symbolically to an eschatological space. The enclosed garden plays a very important role in monastic spirituality as it is not only associated with the cloister, but also with the Virgin Mary, which both offer the monk a gateway to eternity in Paradise. Physically the enclosed garden becomes the very center of the monastic precinct, offering through a ritual-sensory experience of its spatial qualities an experience which allows the monk a moment of spiritual transcendence. It is also, thus, in this moment, when the monk’s physical experience of the garden is woven together with ideas of paradise as an abode of eternity, that the garden becomes a sacred space which can lift him outside of time to experience paradisiacal happiness. This requires a process of hermeneutical interpretation from the monk and the theorist reflecting on this encounter. It is a dialogue between the garden and its interpreters, which leads to the conclusion that an encounter with the sacred never stands in isolation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Lima, Daniel Mattos de Araujo. "Contemporary images of space and time in Caio Fernando Abreu." Universidade Federal do CearÃ, 2007. http://www.teses.ufc.br/tde_busca/arquivo.php?codArquivo=10736.

Full text
Abstract:
nÃo hÃ
A experiÃncia urbana de espaÃo e de tempo na literatura do escritor Caio Fernando Abreu à o objeto de estudo dessa dissertaÃÃo. As experiÃncias subjetivas gestadas na cultura de consumo em que vivemos deflagram modos de pensar, agir, sentir e amar articulados Ãs percepÃÃes de tempo e espaÃo do sujeito contemporÃneo. Nomadismo, errÃncia, solidÃo, narcisismo e impulsividade sÃo algumas das caracterÃsticas deste sujeito. O tempo à mais acelerado e o espaÃo mais compactado de modo que a velocidade forÃada a que somos submetidos nos leva Ãs psicopatologias contemporÃneas tÃpicas do ritmo de vida das grandes cidades. As formas de sociabilidade contemporÃneas ganham laÃos mais tÃnues, tendo em vista que a cultura hoje enfatiza valores como a descartabilidade, o presente imediato, a emergÃncia, o imediatismo das aÃÃes e a compulsÃo. A percepÃÃo, a atenÃÃo e a memÃria sÃo transformadas num contexto histÃrico-cultural e individual-coletivo, de tal maneira que a interpretaÃÃo da realidade e a consciÃncia que o sujeito tem do mundo sofre significativas transformaÃÃes. A literatura de Caio Fernando Abreu apresenta as diferentes faces dos processos de subjetivaÃÃo contemporÃneos em personagens, enredos e cenÃrios figurativos da experiÃncia urbana globalizada, lanÃando um olhar particularmente revelador de tais condiÃÃes no contexto brasileiro. Com efeito, seus contos, romances e seu epistolÃrio desvelam uma crÃtica social profunda à cultura contemporÃnea na tematizaÃÃo do espectro de situaÃÃes nefastas em que està mergulhado o indivÃduo no final do sÃculo XX. De modo mais incisivo, seus textos pensam as peculiaridades da experiÃncia do brasileiro contemporÃneo ligadas, entre outros fatores, à inserÃÃo perifÃrica do paÃs ao capitalismo, aos rumos da polÃtica nacional desde a ditadura militar e Ãs vivÃncias mais subjetivas em termos de valores, utopias, visÃes de vida e de arte para o escritor e para sua geraÃÃo. O resultado à uma literatura que guarda traÃos da âcontraculturaâ e do âpÃs-modernismoâ, tanto em termos temÃticos como nas formas de narrar.
This essay discusses contemporary urban experience as presented in the fiction of Brazilian Writer Caio Fernando Abreu (1948-1996) with special attention to his imagery of time and space. The experience of individuals living in a consumer society produces particular modes of thought, action, feeling and loving which are related to contemporary perceptions of time and space. Errantry, loneliness, narcissism and impulsivity are some of the psychological features of postmodern subjectivity. Accelerated time and compact space developed/emerged specially in the second half of twentieth century along with new technologies of communication and transport seem connected to the new psychopathologies observed in typical contemporary urban life. Todayâs social patterns gain fragile ties due to the reinforcement of values like dischargeability, emphasis on immediate present time and compulsion. Perception, attention and memory have changed according to substantial transformations in the historical and cultural context of late capitalism and these changes also occur on the levels of individual and collective conscience and interpretation of reality. Abreuâs literature present the various faces of contemporary processes of production of subjectivity in characters, plots and scenarios which represent global urban experience, with particular focus on Brazilian context and conditions of modernization. In fact his short-stories, novels and personal letters reveal deep social criticism to grievous situations in which individuals at the end of the century find themselves. Particularly his texts reflect on the singularities of Brazilian globalization experience which involve broad aspects broad aspects like the peripherical insertion of the nation in capitalism and some political paths that led to military dictatorship, as well as more intimate and subjective experiences figured in terms of values, dreams, fears, points of view of life and art shared by the writer and his generation. The result is a literature that reveals traits of countercultural and postmodernist tendencies in its themes and forms of narration.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Zhang, Qiu Jun. "How Chinese - English Bilinguals Think About Time : The Effects of Language on Space-Time Mappings." Thesis, Stockholms universitet, Centrum för tvåspråkighetsforskning, 2020. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-184684.

Full text
Abstract:
The last decades have witnessed the resurgence of research on linguistic relativity, which provides empirical evidence of possible language effects on thought across various perceptual domains. This study investigated the linguistic relativity hypothesis in the abstract domain of time by looking at how L1 Chinese - L2 English bilinguals conceptualize time in two-dimensional space. English primarily relies on horizontal spatial items to talk about time (e.g., back to youth); in addition to horizontal spatial metaphors (e.g., ‘front year’), Chinese speakers also commonly use vertical metaphors to describe time (e.g., ‘up week’). If language has an effect on thought, then spatial-temporal metaphors should shape people’s temporal cognition. In this study, we examined whether spatial-temporal metaphors impact online processing of time and long-term habitual thinking about time. Experiment 1 showed that bilinguals could automatically access the timeline which corresponded to the immediate linguistic context. In Experiment 2, a majority of bilinguals demonstrated salient vertical bias for temporal reasoning, whereas a small number of participants relied on the horizontal axis to represent time. The dominant thinking patterns for time documented here (65% prefer a vertical representation of time; 35% horizontal) run counter to the fact that horizontal metaphors are twice as common in Chinese as vertical metaphors. Further, it was found that bilinguals who used English more frequently were more likely to have a less vertical bias, which suggested a role of L2 experience in conceptual representations. Taken together, the evidence in this study showed that spatial-temporal metaphors have both short-term and long-term effects on mental representations of time, but also that space-time mappings do not depend solely on linguistic factors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Cheong, Yong Jeon. "Worlds of Musics: Cognitive Ethnomusicological Inquiries on Experience of Time and Space in Human Music-making." The Ohio State University, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1555598154844572.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Ankeny, Samuel Robert. "Absolute architecture scaled experience /." Thesis, Montana State University, 2007. http://etd.lib.montana.edu/etd/2007/ankeny/AnkenyS0507.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Books on the topic "Sensorimotor experience, space, time"

1

Space and place: The perspective of experience. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1997.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Michael, Stadter, and Scharff David E. 1941-, eds. Dimensions of psychotherapy, dimensions of experience: Time, space, number, and state of mind. New York, NY: Routledge, 2005.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

The Catholic experience: Space, time, silence, prayer, sacraments, story, persons, catholicity, community, and expectations. New York: Crossroad, 1985.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Alverson, Hoyt. Semantics and experience: Universal metaphors of time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1994.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

1937-, Golledge Reginald G., and Stimson R. J, eds. Person-environment-behavior research: Investigating activities and experience in spaces and environments. New York: Guilford Press, 2008.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Haddix, Margaret Peterson. Game Changer. New York: Simon & Schuster Books for Young Readers, 2011.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Gate, Heavens. How and When "Heaven's Gate" (The Door to the Physical Kingdom Level Above Human) May Be Entered: An Anthology of Our Materials. Mill Spring, Usa: Wildflower Press, 1997.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Hadda, Lamia, ed. Médina. Florence: Firenze University Press, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.36253/978-88-5518-248-5.

Full text
Abstract:
Dedicated to the medina in the Mediterranean space, this book is essentially based on detailed historical and photographic research into the characteristics of city design and its evolution, as well as some case studies from direct experience. The main objective of the present study consists of its documentary and evocative value, without forgetting the analysis of the multiple architectural spaces with monumental complexes of extraordinary cultural importance arranged according to precise hierarchies and specific uses. The research summarises the different experiences from this immense Arab-Muslim architectural heritage and its urban evolution. These aspects are expressed both by the large number of case studies (from Cordoba to Palermo, passing through Fez, Séfrou, Marrakech and Tunis) as well as by the quality of the built spaces as a whole. The several contributions show an urban framework that is still legible and significant, consisting of grids of houses with forms, structures and functions that show a concentration of spaces, places and monuments stratified over time and developed in the Mediterranean countries, producing extremely diverse situations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Silberstein, Michael, W. M. Stuckey, and Timothy McDevitt. Relational Blockworld: Experience, Time, and Space Reintegrated. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198807087.003.0009.

Full text
Abstract:
Chapter 8 argues that the Relational Blockworld (RBW) account naturally admits a kind of neutral monism that simultaneously deflates the generation/hard problem and explains time as experienced. Thus, the claim that the block universe is incompatible with time as experienced is refuted. The first section sets the stage, the second focuses on the Passage of time, and the third focuses on the Direction of time. Section four argues that embodied, embedded, and extended cognitive science and phenomenology support the neutral monism of RBW. The fifth section focuses on freedom, spontaneity, and creativity in RBW. Objections to the block universe picture based on free will in human action and creativity and spontaneity in the universe writ large are refuted. It is shown that RBW has all the freedom, creativity, and spontaneity anyone could reasonably hope for. Section six characterizes Presence in detail, and its relation to time as experienced is discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Perizonius, W. R. K. Bones: Treasuries of Human Experience in Time and Space. Brill Academic Pub, 1989.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Sensorimotor experience, space, time"

1

Faye, Jan. "Space, Time, and Space-Time." In Experience and Beyond, 245–77. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-31077-0_8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

McKenna, William. "Objectivity and Inter-Cultural Experience." In Space, Time, and Culture, 111–18. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-2824-3_8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Rafael, Vicente L. "The experience of translation." In Time, Space, Matter in Translation, 19–32. London: Routledge, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003259732-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Richardson, Louise. "Space, Time and Molyneux's Question." In The Structure of Perceptual Experience, 125–47. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781119061113.ch6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Milroy, A. K. "Making Time (and Space) for the Journey." In The Doctoral Experience, 15–24. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-18199-4_2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Marti, Irene. "Space, Time, Embodiment." In Doing Indefinite Time, 95–110. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-12590-4_3.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis chapter presents the theoretical framework of the study. In this book, the prison and the experience of imprisonment are analysed using space, time and embodiment as key concepts. The study adopts a phenomenological and pragmatist perspective, drawing on the notion of inhabiting. The chapter argues that this analytical approach provides a unique and fruitful perspective on the prison and the subjective experience of incarceration. First, it allows us to understand the prison not as a space in the sense of a (pre-defined) container, but as a formally established set of arrangements of space and time that are lived. Second, the concept of inhabiting enables us to refocus the lens of prison studies away from the often-applied framework of power and resistance and, instead, to explore prisoners’ embodied, agentic and practical engagement with imprisonment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Wildt, Daniel, and Rafael Prikladnicki. "Transitioning from Distributed and Traditional to Distributed and Agile: An Experience Report." In Agility Across Time and Space, 31–46. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-12442-6_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

van Grunsven, Janna, and Wijnand IJsselsteijn. "Confronting Ableism in a Post-COVID World: Designing for World-Familiarity Through Acts of Defamiliarization." In Philosophy of Engineering and Technology, 185–200. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-08424-9_10.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe COVID-19 pandemic has brought about a pervasive digitalization of our social and practical lives. For many, this has signified a substantial loss, with the pandemic underscoring that in-person interactions play a key if not constitutive role in well-being. At the same time, many disabled people and disability rights activists have celebrated the increased accessibility to practical and social spaces enabled by the pandemic-induced embracing of online communication platforms and other digital technologies. With that, the pandemic offers the opportunity to rethink post-pandemic values; prompting us to ask what the pandemic may have taught us about the significance of accessibility and what it means for accessibility to be promoted through technological interventions.Our paper starts from the premise that promoting accessibility and resisting ableism in technology development are morally imperative. On this basis, we outline two distinct conceptions of accessibility, paired with two conceptions of how access thus understood can be promoted through technology. The first conception of accessibility builds off the notion of affordances, taken from the field of ecological psychology. Using the pandemic as a powerful illustrative case, we show that an affordance-based notion of access underscores the link between a person’s sense of well-being and their habitual sensorimotor embeddedness in a world that they experience as a space of familiarity. In Sect. 10.4, we will present Warm Technology as a paradigmatic example of a design-approach aimed at designing for world-familiarity – thus supporting accessibility in one sense of the word. The second conception of accessibility comes from the field of Crip Technoscience and underscores technology’s potential to create access not by promoting world-familiarity but precisely by creating friction and disruption within habitual familiar practices and ways of perceiving the world – particularly when those practices and perceptions reflect an ableist value-system. Though these two perspectives may appear to be in conflict with one another, our goal is to defend the importance of both. Promoting accessibility, we suggest, involves a readiness to oscillate between two normative imperatives: (1) recognizing how human well-being depends on world-familiarity, which, in turn, can be promoted or thwarted through design and (2) recognizing how world-familiarity can harbor pernicious biases that can be called into question through material gestures of defamiliarization. By presenting these two perspectives as mutually required in efforts to design for accessibility, and, furthermore, by framing the pandemic as an event that has placed us, en masse, in a defamiliarized position capable of attuning us to the normative significance of world-familiarity, we hope to better enable technologists and laypersons alike to reflectively evaluate if and how a technological innovation may (or may not) be access-promoting, such that it can contribute to a more just post-COVID world.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Feldt, Jakob Egholm, and Maja Gildin Zuckerman. "Experience, Space, and Time in Jewish Cultural History." In New Perspectives on Jewish Cultural History, 1–26. New York : Routledge, 2020. | Series: Studies for the International Society for Cultural History: Routledge, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780429324048-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Harris, Marguerite. "The Open Void – Embodiment and Experience – In Film/Video/Numeric-Computer Art and Immersive Environments." In Phenomenology of Space and Time, 167–78. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02039-6_13.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Sensorimotor experience, space, time"

1

Wu, Xiang, Zejia Zheng, and Juyang Weng. "Sensorimotor in Space and Time: Audition." In 2018 International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (IJCNN). IEEE, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ijcnn.2018.8489455.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Wang, Huan, Jianning Chi, Chengdong Wu, Xiaosheng Yu, and Qian Hu. "A Multi-Objective Recognition Algorithm with Time and Space Fusion." In WCX SAE World Congress Experience. 400 Commonwealth Drive, Warrendale, PA, United States: SAE International, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.4271/2019-01-1047.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Schulster, J. R. "Time-Critical Decision-Making in Spaceflight Operations: the Mars Express experience." In Space OPS 2004 Conference. Reston, Virigina: American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.2514/6.2004-631-427.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Jirovsky, Vaclav. "Entropy in Reaction Space - Upgrade of Time-to-Collision Quantity." In WCX™ 17: SAE World Congress Experience. 400 Commonwealth Drive, Warrendale, PA, United States: SAE International, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.4271/2017-01-0113.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Mitchell, Bonnie. "THE IMMERSIVE ARTISTIC EXPERIENCE AND THE EXPLOITATION OF SPACE." In CAT 2010: Ideas before their time : Connecting the past and present in computer art. BCS Learning & Development, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.14236/ewic/cat2010.11.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Thayer, John Gregg. "High Reliability System Design Experience with the Gamma Ray Large Area Space Telescope." In 2007 15th IEEE-NPSS Real-Time Conference. IEEE, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/rtc.2007.4382764.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Lou, Yushuang. "Research on Space-time Construction and Perceptual Experience of New Media Art Installation." In Proceedings of the International Conference on Art Design and Digital Technology, ADDT 2022, 16-18 September 2022, Nanjing, China. EAI, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.4108/eai.16-9-2022.2324873.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Maroldi, Fabio, and Fiamma Colette Invernizzi. "LEARNING AND TEACHING "SPACE-TIME" (TRANS)FORM-ACTION: A SCHOOL METADESIGN COOPERATION EXPERIENCE." In International Conference on Education and New Learning Technologies. IATED, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.21125/edulearn.2016.0602.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Jang, Sun-Young, and Kim Sung-Ah. "SMART ALLEY: A Platform for Sharing Experience in a Community Space Augmented by Urban Media." In eCAADe 2015 : Real time - Extending the reach of computation. eCAADe, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.52842/conf.ecaade.2015.1.529.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Süyük Makakli, Elif, and Ebru Yücesan. "Spatial Experience Of Physical And Virtual Space." In SPACE International Conferences April 2021. SPACE Studies Publications, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.51596/cbp2021.jrvm8060.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Fictional spaces produced with multidisciplinary research using improving technologiescreate settings that provoke new questions and have diff erent answers. This comes about bybroadening the horizons in virtual space studies, space concept, design, and experience. Evaluatingvirtual space as a layer of reality represents architectural space that belongs to the physical world.The principal factors that form the physicality of a space, its shape and content, are related tocultural, public, societal, perceptual, and intellectual codes. The space concept can be explained asa physical concept. In the sense of human interaction with space, the feelings it elicits, perceptualfactors, both in the subjective and abstract dimensions, that can be described as feelings, and 3Dphysicality. Spaces designed and produced for human use can be perceived diff erently and meanother things to diff erent people through human–space interactions. Perceiving, interpreting, anddescribing a space is a complex process that can only occur by experiencing it.Although virtual reality emerged as a simulation of physical space, there are increasing attempts toform an emotional and physical connection to such spaces today. New technologies used to createnew spaces and descriptions such as virtual reality, virtual space, cyberspace, and hybrid space arearticulated as new layers within the spatial memory accumulated to date.Virtual reality technologies, which can be explained as an interface between humans and machinesand describe diff erent life systems, give one the feeling of being in another space. Although thesespaces are virtual, they can be related to the space concept as they can be experienced and give thefeeling of being somewhere. These settings, which present multi-dimensional spatial experiences bytaking humans into a digital reality, are created using computer support and are experienced usingvarious electronic tools. These settings in which human and machine, organic and non-organicentities meet are also crucial in design education as they improve creative processes related to thefuture, machine-human interaction, and the space concept and its formation.As virtuality beingevaluated as a layer of reality becomes a representation of architectural space that belongs to thephysical world, it also has the potential to approach space design in a new way.It has the potential to aff ect and improve the perception of creating space and deliver spatialsolutions, understand new living conditions, and discover the future by responding to technologicalimprovements.Virtual reality creates a personal space experience that diff racts space and time—improvingtechnologies set these spaces, which simulate reality, as a layer of fact, a refl ection or representation.The cyber and virtual experiences that have emerged in new media spaces have reduced space’sdependency on the physical world through the integration of improving technologies and art. ‘SALT Research’ within Salt Galata, a monumental building in Galata-İstanbul, and ‘Virtual Archive’, a media art project by Refik Anadol that questions the virtual-digital space concept, were chosen as experience spaces. It was emphasized that there are holistic composition differences between spaces due to the current physical space experience that composes the infrastructure of the study and virtual space. It is composed of different elements and is perceived just like real space. The dataset includes a detailed assessment of two different spaces with similar contexts and contains the physical and virtual space analysis through syntactic, semantic and pragmatic scales. Volunteer participants emphasized the differences in holistic composition between the two spaces. They noted that the virtual space differs from the physical space and is composed of different elements and that the user has the perception of belonging just like in a physical space.The physical space, SALT Research, was evaluated as satisfactory and high-quality in terms of aesthetics and equipment. Phrases used to describe it were neat, high spaces, comfort, spaciousness, light, dark areas, tranquillity, silence, acoustic balance, harmony, historical, gripping, transformation, aesthetic and functional, and plain. In contrast, participants saw the Virtual Archive is a new, exciting, different, and innovative experience. The bodily freedom of the virtual space experience was described as optimistic. Through a brief understanding of the space, they overcame the difficulties of physical existence that arose when accessing information in this new environment.Fictional space produced with a multidisciplinary study using improving technologies creates settings where new questions are asked, and different answers are made, broadening the horizons in virtual space studies, space concept, design, and experience. Virtuality being evaluated as a layer of reality represents architectural space that belongs to the physical world.Virtual reality technology changes and influences our time, dimension, and architectural perceptions, the modes of expression and interaction models in art and architecture by taking us into a different universe experienced spiritually and mentally in new space creations.The space experience through the journey of interpretation and understanding of space and architecture tells different things for each person on each occasion. Perceiving space through the physical space experience and active senses via intellectual feedback also affects virtual reality interactions.Different disciplines examine the machine, human, space, and future relations in an interdisciplinary environment. Different designs’ varieties and opportunities have a place in architecture and interior architecture. In the future, the integration of physical space, virtual space, and machine intelligence into space design and design education and the role and effect of the designer will continue to be discussed.Today, new representation environments present new evolutions that improve, evaluate, and interpret spatial ideas. Despite changing technologies, humans must exist somewhere, and existence is related to our sensory, emotional, and memorial creations. In this sense, the place of humans and designers will continue to be questioned in the new spaces created. Keywords: Patrik Schumacher, ethics, ethical paradigms in architecture, humanitarian architecture, architectural media platforms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Reports on the topic "Sensorimotor experience, space, time"

1

Tymoshyk, Mykola. UKRAINIAN CHILDREN’S MAGAZINE ON EMIGRATION AS A SPECIFIC TYPE OF PUBLICATION (ON THE MATERIALS OF THE LONDON MONTHLY “YOUNG FRIENDS”). Ivan Franko National University of Lviv, February 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.30970/vjo.2022.51.11394.

Full text
Abstract:
For the first time, one of the popular children’s magazines of the Western Ukrainian Diaspora “Young Friends” became the subject of research. Founded in March 1955, it ceased to exist in 1984. There is no complete filing of this newspaper in any book collection of Ukraine, it has not been digitized yet, the editorial office did not have a site. For this reason, the author conducted a study of this journal in the library-archive of the Union of Ukrainians in Great Britain (UUB) in London. The peculiarities of journal formation and the specifics of the editorial policy are clarified. The experience of publishing a Ukrainian children’s magazine abroad for a long time (in color and on chalk paper) without any financial support from the state, but only by public money, is quite instructive for the current situation in Ukraine when children’s periodicals have almost disappeared from the national information space due to indifferent contemplation of the state.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Yaremchuk, Olesya. TRAVEL ANTHROPOLOGY IN JOURNALISM: HISTORY AND PRACTICAL METHODS. Ivan Franko National University of Lviv, February 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.30970/vjo.2021.49.11069.

Full text
Abstract:
Our study’s main object is travel anthropology, the branch of science that studies the history and nature of man, socio-cultural space, social relations, and structures by gathering information during short and long journeys. The publication aims to research the theoretical foundations and genesis of travel anthropology, outline its fundamental principles, and highlight interaction with related sciences. The article’s defining objectives are the analysis of the synthesis of fundamental research approaches in travel anthropology and their implementation in journalism. When we analyze what methods are used by modern authors, also called «cultural observers», we can return to the localization strategy, namely the centering of the culture around a particular place, village, or another spatial object. It is about the participants-observers and how the workplace is limited in space and time and the broader concept of fieldwork. Some disciplinary practices are confused with today’s complex, interactive cultural conjunctures, leading us to think of a laboratory of controlled observations. Indeed, disciplinary approaches have changed since Malinowski’s time. Based on the experience of fieldwork of Svitlana Aleksievich, Katarzyna Kwiatkowska-Moskalewicz, or Malgorzata Reimer, we can conclude that in modern journalism, where the tools of travel anthropology are used, the practical methods of complexity, reflexivity, principles of openness, and semiotics are decisive. Their authors implement both for stable localization and for a prevailing transition.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Crispin, Darla. Artistic Research as a Process of Unfolding. Norges Musikkhøgskole, August 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.22501/nmh-ar.503395.

Full text
Abstract:
As artistic research work in various disciplines and national contexts continues to develop, the diversity of approaches to the field becomes ever more apparent. This is to be welcomed, because it keeps alive ideas of plurality and complexity at a particular time in history when the gross oversimplifications and obfuscations of political discourses are compromising the nature of language itself, leading to what several commentators have already called ‘a post-truth’ world. In this brutal environment where ‘information’ is uncoupled from reality and validated only by how loudly and often it is voiced, the artist researcher has a responsibility that goes beyond the confines of our discipline to articulate the truth-content of his or her artistic practice. To do this, they must embrace daring and risk-taking, finding ways of communicating that flow against the current norms. In artistic research, the empathic communication of information and experience – and not merely the ‘verbally empathic’ – is a sign of research transferability, a marker for research content. But this, in some circles, is still a heretical point of view. Research, in its more traditional manifestations mistrusts empathy and individually-incarnated human experience; the researcher, although a sentient being in the world, is expected to behave dispassionately in their professional discourse, and with a distrust for insights that come primarily from instinct. For the construction of empathic systems in which to study and research, our structures still need to change. So, we need to work toward a new world (one that is still not our idea), a world that is symptomatic of what we might like artistic research to be. Risk is one of the elements that helps us to make the conceptual twist that turns subjective, reflexive experience into transpersonal, empathic communication and/or scientifically-viable modes of exchange. It gives us something to work with in engaging with debates because it means that something is at stake. To propose a space where such risks may be taken, I shall revisit Gillian Rose’s metaphor of ‘the fold’ that I analysed in the first Symposium presented by the Arne Nordheim Centre for Artistic Research (NordART) at the Norwegian Academy of Music in November 2015. I shall deepen the exploration of the process of ‘unfolding’, elaborating on my belief in its appropriateness for artistic research work; I shall further suggest that Rose’s metaphor provides a way to bridge some of the gaps of understanding that have already developed between those undertaking artistic research and those working in the more established music disciplines.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography