Journal articles on the topic 'Self-referential'

To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Self-referential.

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Self-referential.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Aste, T., P. Butler, and T. Di Matteo. "Self-referential order." Philosophical Magazine 93, no. 31-33 (September 17, 2013): 3983–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14786435.2013.835495.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Zinck, Alexandra. "Self-referential emotions." Consciousness and Cognition 17, no. 2 (June 2008): 496–505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.014.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Whittle, Bruno. "Self-referential propositions." Synthese 194, no. 12 (September 26, 2016): 5023–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1191-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Propp, Jim. "Self-Referential Aptitude Test." Math Horizons 12, no. 3 (February 2005): 35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10724117.2005.12021810.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Miller, Eric Randolph. "Is Literature Self-Referential?" Philosophy and Literature 20, no. 2 (1996): 475–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/phl.1996.0075.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Tao, Yong. "Self-referential Boltzmann machine." Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 545 (May 2020): 123775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2019.123775.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Mele, Alfred R. "Are intentions self-referential?" Philosophical Studies 52, no. 3 (November 1987): 309–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00354051.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Liu, Cuihong, Wenjie Li, Rong Wang, Yaohan Cai, and Jie Chen. "Temporal features of individual and collective self-referential processing: an event-related potential study." PeerJ 8 (April 9, 2020): e8917. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.8917.

Full text
Abstract:
Background Individual and collective self are two fundamental self-representations and are important to human experience. The present study aimed to investigate whether individual and collective self have essential difference in neural mechanism. Methods Event-related potentials were recorded to explore the electrophysiological correlates of individual and collective self in a self-referential task in which participants were asked to evaluate whether trait adjectives were suitable to describe themselves (individual self-referential processing), a famous person (individual non-self-referential processing), Chinese (collective self-referential processing) or American (collective non-self-referential processing). Results At the early stages, results showed that larger P2 and smaller N2 amplitudes were elicited by individual self-referential than by individual non-self-referential processing whereas no significant differences were observed between collective self-referential and collective non-self-referential processing at these stages. In addition, at the late P3 stage (350–600 ms), larger P3 amplitudes were also elicited by individual self-referential than by individual non-self-referential processing during 350–600 ms interval. However, the collective self-reference effect, indicated by the differences between collective self-referential and collective non-self-referential processing, did not appear until 450 ms and extended to 600 ms. Moreover, individual self-reference effect was more pronounced than collective self-reference effect in the 350–500 ms interval, whereas individual and collective self-reference effect had no significant difference in the 500–600 ms interval. These findings indicated that the time courses of neural activities were different in processing individual and collective self.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Sui, Jie, Chang Hong Liu, Lingyun Wang, and Shihui Han. "Short Article: Attentional Orientation Induced by Temporarily Established Self-Referential Cues." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62, no. 5 (May 2009): 844–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210802559393.

Full text
Abstract:
Self-referential stimuli such as self-face surpass other-referential stimuli in capture of attention, which has been attributed to attractive perceptual features of self-referential stimuli. We investigated whether temporarily established self-referential stimuli are different from other-referential cues in guiding voluntary visual attention. Temporarily established self-referential or friend-referential shapes served as central cues in Posner's endogenous cueing task. We found that, relative to friend-referential cues, self-referential cues induced smaller cueing effect (i.e., the difference in reaction times to targets at cued and uncued locations) when the interstimulus interval was short but larger cueing effect when the interstimulus interval was long. Our findings suggest that temporarily established self-referential cues are more efficient to capture reflexive attention at the early stage of perceptual processing and to shift voluntary attention at the later stage of perceptual processing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Shi, Zhan, and Liguo He. "Mindfulness: Attenuating Self-Referential Processing and Strengthening Other-Referential Processing." Mindfulness 11, no. 3 (December 10, 2019): 599–605. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12671-019-01271-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Borisov, Evgeny V. "Is Yablo's Paradox Self-Referential?" Vestnik Tomskogo gosudarstvennogo universiteta. Filosofiya, sotsiologiya, politologiya, no. 50 (August 1, 2019): 233–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.17223/1998863x/50/20.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Drange, T. M. "Slater on self-referential arguments." Analysis 54, no. 1 (January 1, 1994): 61–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/analys/54.1.61.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Hill, Christopher S. "HARMAN ON SELF REFERENTIAL THOUGHTS." Philosophical Issues 16, no. 1 (September 2006): 346–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1533-6077.2006.00118.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Wastler, Heather M., and Mark F. Lenzenweger. "Self-referential hypermentalization in schizotypy." Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 10, no. 6 (November 2019): 536–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/per0000344.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Kapitan, Tomis. "INTENTIONS AND SELF-REFERENTIAL CONTENT." Philosophical Papers 24, no. 3 (November 1995): 151–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/05568649509506528.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Barnsley, Michael F. "Transformations between Self-Referential Sets." American Mathematical Monthly 116, no. 4 (April 2009): 291–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00029890.2009.11920941.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Zimbarg Sobrinho, J. "Definability in self-referential systems." Notre Dame Journal of Formal Logic 29, no. 4 (September 1988): 574–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1305/ndjfl/1093638022.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Barnsley, Michael F. "Transformations between Self-Referential Sets." American Mathematical Monthly 116, no. 4 (April 1, 2009): 291–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.4169/193009709x470155.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Turk, Monica J. "Self-referential gestures in conversation." Discourse Studies 9, no. 4 (August 2007): 558–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1461445607079166.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

BRENDEL, Elke. "SELF-REFERENTIAL ARGUMENTS IN PHILOSOPHY." Grazer Philosophische Studien 74, no. 1 (2007): 177–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/9789401204651_010.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Zentall, Thomas R. "The Self-Referential Independent Brain." Contemporary Psychology 47, no. 6 (December 2002): 677–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/001267.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Whittle, Bruno. "Correction to: Self-referential propositions." Synthese 196, no. 6 (March 21, 2018): 2541. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-1714-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Dinulescu, Stejara, Talha Alvi, David Rosenfield, Cecile S. Sunahara, Junghee Lee, and Benjamin A. Tabak. "Self-Referential Processing Predicts Social Cognitive Ability." Social Psychological and Personality Science 12, no. 1 (April 2, 2020): 99–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550620902281.

Full text
Abstract:
Self-referential processing is critical for making sense of others. However, there remains surprisingly little research examining associations between behavioral assessments of self-referential processing and social cognition (i.e., tasks assessing one’s understanding of others’ thoughts and emotions). This study ( n = 396) examined this link by associating accuracy in a self-referential processing task with two assessments of social cognition (i.e., theory of mind and empathic accuracy). Exploratory analyses included an examination of the relationship between self-referential processing and autism-related traits, as well as depression symptoms, both of which have been previously associated with decreased social cognitive ability. Self-referential processing was positively related to performance in both social cognition tasks, and these relationships were not valence-specific or moderated by gender. Moreover, no associations were found between self-referential processing and autism-related traits or depressive symptoms. Our findings provide behavioral evidence for a relation between self-referential processing and social cognition.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Lawrence, Ross, and Xiaoqian J. Chai. "Self-referential encoding of source information in recollection memory." PLOS ONE 16, no. 4 (April 15, 2021): e0248044. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248044.

Full text
Abstract:
Information that is encoded in relation to the self has been shown to be better remembered, yet reports have disagreed on whether the memory benefit from self-referential encoding extends to source memory (the context in which information was learned). In this study, we investigated the self-referential effect on source memory in recollection and familiarity-based memory. Using a Remember/Know paradigm, we compared source memory accuracy under self-referential encoding and semantic encoding. Two types of source information were included, a “peripheral” source which was not inherent to the encoding activity, and a source information about the encoding context. We observed the facilitation in item memory from self-referential encoding compared to semantic encoding in recollection but not in familiarity-based memory. The self-referential benefit to source accuracy was observed in recollection memory, with source memory for the encoding context being stronger in the self-referential condition. No significant self-referential effect was observed with regards to peripheral source information (information not required for the participant to focus on), suggesting not all source information benefit from self-referential encoding. Self-referential encoding also resulted in a higher ratio of “Remember/Know” responses rate than semantically encoded items, denoting stronger recollection. These results suggest self-referential encoding creates a richer, more detailed memory trace which can be recollected later on.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

김경미 and 이도준. "Self-positivity bias in self-referential source memory." 한국심리학회지: 사회및성격 24, no. 1 (February 2010): 47–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.21193/kjspp.2010.24.1.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Lang, Stefan. "Nonconceptual Self-Awareness and the Constitution of Referential Self-Consciousness." ProtoSociology 36 (2019): 491–515. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/protosociology20193621.

Full text
Abstract:
This essay argues that persons not only have nonconceptual bodily self-awareness and nonconceptual mental anonymous self-awareness but also, at least if they produce the expression ‘I’, nonconceptual mental egological self-awareness. It contains information of ‘I’ being produced by oneself. It is argued that this can be seen if we examine the constitution of referential self-consciousness, i.e. the consciousness of being the referent of ‘I’ oneself. The main argument is: A. It is not possible to explain the constitution of referential self-consciousness if it is not assumed that persons have nonconceptual mental egological self-awareness. B. It is possible to explain the constitution of referential self-consciousness if it is assumed that persons have nonconceptual mental egological self-awareness. C. Thus it is reasonable to assume that persons have nonconceptual mental egological self-awareness. The justification of the thesis that persons have nonconceptual mental egological self-awareness is presented while discussing Tomis Kapitan’s analysis of conceptual egological self-consciousness. Conceptual egological self-consciousness contains infor­mation of being a subject oneself. It is argued that it is not possible to explain the constitution of referential self-consciousness with the help of Kapitan’s interpretation of conceptual self-consciousness. However, it is possible to ex­plain the constitution of referential self-consciousness within the framework of Kapitan’s account if it is assumed that persons have nonconceptual mental egological self-awareness.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Franks, Angela. "End-less and Self-Referential Desire." National Catholic Bioethics Quarterly 18, no. 4 (2018): 629–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/ncbq201818467.

Full text
Abstract:
Is postlapsarian sexual desire primarily altruistic or disordered? This paper utilizes the resources in the thought of St. Thomas Aquinas and in the contemporary magisterium to argue that recent phenomena such as the #MeToo movement underscore the inherently unstable and aggressive nature of sexual desire when it is uprooted from its natural end (i.e., is end-less). Aquinas highlights three aspects of desire that more sex-positive accounts of sexuality would do well to heed: its natural infinity, its self-referential nature (grounded in amor concupiscentiae), and its power of rationalization. By directing the motor of desire toward its natural ends, virtue—led by reason—can redirect desire away from self and toward the good.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Keith, Michael. "Circle digits a self-referential story." Mathematical Intelligencer 8, no. 3 (September 1986): 56–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf03025792.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Alexander, Victoria N. "Creativity: Self-Referential Mistaking, Not Negating." Biosemiotics 6, no. 2 (August 26, 2012): 253–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12304-012-9158-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Vernitski, Alexei. "Finite quasivarieties and self-referential conditions." Studia Logica 78, no. 1-2 (November 2004): 337–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11225-005-7348-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Cling, Andrew D. "Eliminative materialism and self-referential inconsistency." Philosophical Studies 56, no. 1 (May 1989): 53–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00646209.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Kuznets, Roman. "Self-Referential Justifications in Epistemic Logic." Theory of Computing Systems 46, no. 4 (April 7, 2009): 636–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00224-009-9209-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Allison, Lloyd. "Circular programs and self-referential structures." Software: Practice and Experience 19, no. 2 (February 1989): 99–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/spe.4380190202.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Stepanov, Vladimir A. "Dynamic Approximation of Self-Referential Sentences." Studia Humana 11, no. 3-4 (December 1, 2022): 25–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/sh-2022-0013.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Non-classical logic via approximation of self-referential sentences by dynamical systems are consistently presented. The new 6-valued truth values <T, va, A, V, av, F> (here A=Liar, V=TruthTeller) are presented as a function of the classical truth values x i ∈ {0,1}, which resulted in a philosophical standpoint known as Suszko’s Thesis. Three-valued truth tables were created corresponding to Priest’s tables of the same name. In the process of constructing 4-valued truth tables, two more new truth values (va, av) were revealed that do not coincide with the four original ones. Therefore, the closed tables turned out to be 6-valued. Prof Dunn’s 4-valued truth tables are compared with our 4-valued truth tables. De Morgan’s laws are confirmed by six-valued truth tables. Constructed 3-, 4- and 6-valued lattices obeying De Morgan’s laws.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Dastjerdi, M., B. L. Foster, S. Nasrullah, A. M. Rauschecker, R. F. Dougherty, J. D. Townsend, C. Chang, et al. "Differential electrophysiological response during rest, self-referential, and non-self-referential tasks in human posteromedial cortex." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108, no. 7 (January 31, 2011): 3023–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1017098108.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Finlayson-Short, Laura, Christopher G. Davey, and Ben J. Harrison. "Neural correlates of integrated self and social processing." Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 15, no. 9 (September 2020): 941–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa121.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Self-referential and social processing are often engaged concurrently in naturalistic judgements and elicit activity in overlapping brain regions. We have termed this integrated processing ‘self-other referential processing’ and developed a task to measure its neural correlates. Ninety-eight healthy young people aged 16–25 (M = 21.5 years old, 67% female) completed our novel functional magnetic resonance imaging task. The task had two conditions, an active self-other referential processing condition in which participants rated how much they related to emotional faces and a control condition. Rating relatedness required thinking about oneself (self-referential processing) and drawing a comparison to an imagined other (social processing). Self-other referential processing elicited activity in the default mode network and social cognition system; most notably in the ‘core self’ regions of the medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex. Relatedness and emotional valence directly modulated activity in these core self areas, while emotional valence additionally modulated medial prefrontal cortex activity. This shows the key role of the medial prefrontal cortex in constructing the ‘social-affective self’. This may help to unify disparate models of medial prefrontal cortex function, demonstrating its role in coordinating multiple processes—self-referential, social and affective processing—to allow the self to exist in a complex social world.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Dakhalaeva, E. C. "«ONE’S OWN CIRCLE» - «OUTER CIRCLE»: FRAGMENTS OF DYNAMIC MODEL OF SELF- REFERENCE AND INO-REFERENCE COMPONENTS INTERACTION IN THE DISCOURSE FEMALE VICTIMS OF DOMESTIC VIOLENCE." Culture and Text, no. 45 (2021): 198–210. http://dx.doi.org/10.37386/2305-4077-2021-2-198-210.

Full text
Abstract:
The object of this research is the female victims’ first-person discourse. To describe the self-referential discourse of this type of a speaker, along with the notions of “self-reference” and “ino-reference” the author resorts to such notions as “one’s own circle” and “outer circle”. This expands the meaning of the notion of “self-reference”. The purpose of this article is to reconstruct the fragments of the dynamic model of interacting self-referential and non-referential discourse components on the material of French-speaking female victims’ evidence. The speakers were subjected to domestic violence in their past. The author reveals lexical, grammatical and syntactic components, which are used in the victims’ discourse within the framework of “one’s own self-referential circle” and “outer ino-referential circle”, a complex set of their interaction is schematically demonstrated.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Daley, Ryan T., Holly J. Bowen, Eric C. Fields, Katelyn R. Parisi, Angela Gutchess, and Elizabeth A. Kensinger. "Neural mechanisms supporting emotional and self-referential information processing and encoding in older and younger adults." Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 15, no. 4 (April 2020): 405–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa052.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Emotion and self-referential information can both enhance memory, but whether they do so via common mechanisms across the adult lifespan remains underexplored. To address this gap, the current study directly compared, within the same fMRI paradigm, the encoding of emotionally salient and self-referential information in older adults and younger adults. Behavioral results replicated the typical patterns of better memory for emotional than neutral information and for self-referential than non-self-referential materials; these memory enhancements were present for younger and older adults. In neural activity, young and older adults showed similar modulation by emotion, but there were substantial age differences in the way self-referential processing affected neural recruitment. Contrary to our hypothesis, we found little evidence for overlap in the neural mechanisms engaged for emotional and self-referential processing. These results reveal that—just as in cognitive domains—older adults can show similar performance to younger adults in socioemotional domains even though the two age groups engage distinct neural mechanisms. These findings demonstrate the need for future research delving into the neural mechanisms supporting older adults’ memory benefits for socioemotional material.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

YAOI, Ken, Mariko OSAKA, and Naoyuki OSAKA. "Neural correlates of the self referential process." Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association 74 (September 20, 2010): 3AM061. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/pacjpa.74.0_3am061.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Hwang, Suyoung. "Self-referential Logic of Duration and Creation." Modern Philosophy 16 (October 31, 2020): 5–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.52677/2020.10.16.5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Hwang, Suyoung. "Self-referential Logic of Duration and Creation." Modern Philosophy 16 (October 31, 2020): 5–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.52677/mph.2020.10.16.5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Sobrinho, J. Zimbarg. "On the consistency of self-referential systems." Journal of Symbolic Logic 52, no. 2 (June 1987): 425–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2274392.

Full text
Abstract:
In [2], Hiller and myself have introduced what we have called self-referential systems. Under this name we categorize models of a theory whose predicates refer to objects which are themselves predicates of the language—this particular interpretation being considered as the intended one. is formulated in a language whose variables are typed: as types become “larger”, the corresponding objects become “smaller”; for this reason, we have suggestively chosen types to be negative integers: 0, −1, −2, and so on.The consistency of has been questioned several times, and it has also been referred to as an “intriguing possibility”. Under such motivation, we are most willing to present a consistency proof for , and we do it relative to the theory ZFC + “there exists a Ramsey cardinal”. The only use of the Ramsey cardinal is to justify the existence of 0#, and the existence of 0# is known to be weaker than the existence of a Ramsey cardinal. For this reason, we note that the minimum we need for our proof is the existence of the set 0#.To begin with, we prove that the consistency of implies the existence of intended models for : these are models in which every element is definable by a one-free-variable formula of the language. In intended models we also show that the axiom holds, and, besides, these models possess -indescribable cardinals. These two results imply the relative consistency of with respect to and, moreover, that any proof of the consistency of cannot be carried out without the use of some large cardinal axiom: such an axiom which, when added to ZFC, produces a theory equiconsistent to —if it exists—remains unknown. At the end of this article we finally exhibit the consistency proof of , our main result.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

HIGASHI, Toshimitsu, Kosuke SEKIYAMA, and Toshio FUKUDA. "Self-referential Structure in Collective Agent System." Transactions of the Society of Instrument and Control Engineers 37, no. 5 (2001): 438–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.9746/sicetr1965.37.438.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

김려실. "Takeshi Kitano’s Self-referential Aesthesis in Takeshis’." Contemporary Film Studies 7, no. 1 (May 2011): 119–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.15751/cofis.2011.7.1.119.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Mock, Roberta. "“WithoutyouI'm nothing”: Sandra Bernhard's self‐referential postmodernism." Women's Studies 30, no. 4 (August 2001): 543–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00497878.2001.9979394.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Lombardo, Michael V., Jennifer L. Barnes, Sally J. Wheelwright, and Simon Baron-Cohen. "Self-Referential Cognition and Empathy in Autism." PLoS ONE 2, no. 9 (September 12, 2007): e883. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000883.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Nicolis, J. S., T. Bountis, and K. Togias. "The dynamics of self-referential paradoxical games." Dynamical Systems 16, no. 4 (December 2001): 319–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14689360110081741.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Pincus, Tamar, Shirley Pearce, Alastair McClelland, and Lynn Turner-Stokes. "Self-referential selective memory in pain patients." British Journal of Clinical Psychology 32, no. 3 (September 1993): 365–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1993.tb01069.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Mu, Yan, and Shihui Han. "Neural oscillations involved in self-referential processing." NeuroImage 53, no. 2 (November 1, 2010): 757–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Friedman, Harvey, and Michael Sheard. "An axiomatic approach to self-referential truth." Annals of Pure and Applied Logic 33 (1987): 1–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-0072(87)90073-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography