Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Psychology of Memory Cognitive psychology'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Psychology of Memory Cognitive psychology.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Dawson, Spencer Charles. "Memory, Arousal, and Perception of Sleep." Thesis, The University of Arizona, 2018. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10640182.
Full textPeople with insomnia overestimate how long it takes to fall asleep and underestimate the total amount of sleep they attain. While memory is normally decreased prior to sleep onset, this decrease is smaller in insomnia. Insomnia generally and the phenomena of underestimation of sleep and greater memory prior to sleep area associated with arousal including cortical, autonomic, and cognitive arousal. The goal of the present study was to simultaneously examine arousal across these domains in relation to memory and accuracy of sleep estimation.
Forty healthy adults completed baseline measures of sleep, psychopathology, and memory, then maintained a regular sleep schedule for three nights at home before spending a night in the sleep laboratory. On the night of the sleep laboratory study, participants completed measures of cognitive arousal, were allowed to sleep until five minutes of contiguous stage N2 sleep in the third NREM period. They were then awoken and asked to remain awake for fifteen minutes, after which they were allowed to resume sleeping. For the entire duration that they were awake, auditory stimuli (recordings of words) were presented at a rate of one word per 30 seconds. Participants slept until morning, estimated how long they were awake and then completed memory testing, indicating whether they remembered hearing each of the words previously presented along with an equal number of matched distracter words.
Memory was greatest for words presented early in the awakening, followed by the middle and end of the awakening. High cortical arousal prior to being awoken was associated with better memory, particularly for the early part of the awakening. High autonomic arousal was associated with better memory for the late part of the awakening. Cognitive arousal was not associated with memory. Longer duration of sleep prior to being awoken was associated with better memory for the middle of the awakening. Better memory at baseline was associated with better memory, specifically in the middle of the awakening. Contrary to expectation, memory for the awakening was not associated with accuracy of the perceived length of the awakening.
The present study found complementary associations between cortical and autonomic arousal and memory for an awakening from sleep. This suggests that decreasing arousal in both domains may reduce the discrepancy between subjective and objective sleep in insomnia. This also suggests the initial magnitude of decrements in cognitive performance after being awoken are related to deeper proximal sleep initially, while speed of improvement in cognitive performance is related to longer prior sleep duration.
Stewart, James Roosevelt Jr. "Memory and cognitive processes in childhood depression /." The Ohio State University, 1988. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1487597424138804.
Full textPrescott, Carmella Maria. "Self-Reported Memory as a Function of Clinical Versus Everyday Memory Tasks." W&M ScholarWorks, 1990. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539625637.
Full textAfzalnia, Mohammed Reza. "Memory studies from comparative media : four experimental studies: a study in cognitive psychology." Thesis, Manchester Metropolitan University, 2000. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.364829.
Full textLittrell, Morgan. "The Relationship Between Eating Disorder Symptomology, Critical Body Comments, and Memory Recall." TopSCHOLAR®, 2012. http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses/1226.
Full textMorris, Eva Marie. "Semantic Memory in Alzheimer's Disease." W&M ScholarWorks, 1999. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539626235.
Full textHasinski, Adam E. "Interactions between Prediction, Perception and Episodic Memory." The Ohio State University, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1437731857.
Full textPierson, Eric E. McBride Dawn M. "Mood and memory mapping the cognitive-emotive structure /." Normal, Ill. : Illinois State University, 2007. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?index=0&did=1390309741&SrchMode=1&sid=4&Fmt=2&VInst=PROD&VType=PQD&RQT=309&VName=PQD&TS=1203095001&clientId=43838.
Full textTitle from title page screen, viewed on February 15, 2008. Dissertation Committee: Dawn M. McBride (chair), Alvin E. House, Karla J. Doepke, Robert Peterson. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 94-100) and abstract. Also available in print.
Tang, Yue. "Post-Learning Activities and Memory Consolidation: the Effect of Physical and Cognitive Activities on Memory Consolidation." Oberlin College Honors Theses / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=oberlin1371024975.
Full textMiser, Tracey Marie. "Attention, Memory, and Development of Inductive Generalization." The Ohio State University, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1460404591.
Full textDavis, Carrie Adrian. "Examining a Hierarchical Linear Regression Model of Overgeneral Memory| Methodological Issues, CaR-FA-X Model Mechanisms, and Memory Encoding as Represented by Cognitive Attributional Style." Thesis, San Jose State University, 2018. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10742726.
Full textOvergeneral memory (OGM) is a phenomenon of reduced autobiographical memory specificity observed in major depressive disorder (MDD) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Individuals demonstrating OGM tend to describe past events generally rather than specifically recalling single memory occurrences. Research shows that OGM is perpetuated by three mechanisms: capture in the memory hierarchy due to trait rumination (CaR), functional avoidance of specific memory retrieval (FA), and impaired executive control (X), which together make up the CaR-FA-X model of OGM. Research on the CaR-FA-X model has historically looked at each mechanism in isolation. The current research aimed to compare the contributions of all three mechanisms to a measure of OGM, as well as to investigate possible interactions between the mechanisms, and compare the contributions of the CaR-FA-X model to those of an encoding predictor. Psychometric data on the three CaR-FA-X mechanisms, autobiographical memory specificity, cognitive attributional style, and mental health were collected from 107 undergraduate psychology students via online surveys, then analyzed in a hierarchical linear regression model. Executive control explained significant unique variance in OGM, with rumination making an indirect contribution. No other anticipated contributions from the CaR-FA-X model or memory encoding were observed. Methodological issues in non-clinical and computerized OGM research are highlighted.
Demmin, Docia L. "Influence of Emotional Stimuli on Working Memory in Schizotypy." W&M ScholarWorks, 2015. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539626808.
Full textSteitz, David W. Verhaeghen Paul. "Age differences in memory performance and strategy use for grocery items and imagery/familiarity-matched non-grocery words a study in everyday memory /." Related electronic resource: Current Research at SU : database of SU dissertations, recent titles available full text, 2004. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/syr/main.
Full textSenkfor, Ava Joy. "Event-related potential investigations of source and item memory." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/289044.
Full textBolden, Khalima Alicia. "Implicit socioemotional modulation of working memory brain activity in schizophrenia." Thesis, University of California, San Diego, 2016. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10144260.
Full textThe neural substrate of interactions of working memory (WM) with socio-emotional processing is poorly understood in schizophrenia. This study builds on published papers using a delayed match to sample design to study the interaction of WM load with type of distracter (socially relevant faces vs. socially irrelevant geometric designs [FvG]) presented briefly during the WM maintenance period. Based on previously published findings, we hypothesize: (1) The FvG difference in brain activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in the task maintenance period will be largest at the highest WM load. (2) Among schizophrenia/ schizoaffective patients and healthy controls the magnitude of the face vs. geometric design (FvG) contrast in brain activity in the amygdala during the task maintenance period will follow a quadratic pattern across WM load when averaged over face type. (3) Among schizophrenia patients, the magnitude of the FvG contrast in brain activity in the amygdala and DLPFC at the greatest WM load will be correlated with negative symptoms.
Individuals between the ages of 18-55 diagnosed with schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder (N = 12) and non-psychiatric controls (N = 20) matched with the patients on age, gender, paternal education and paternal socioeconomic status underwent structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). To assess the effect of implicit socioemotional modulation on brain activity during WM, the effect of facial distraction on brain activation was assessed for WM of pseudowords at three syllable loads (1, 2, and 3) across several face valence types and contrasted with the effect of a geometric distracter.
Results: Although patients performed significantly above chance, they were less accurate than controls with no difference in response latency. When the FvG contrast was tested for response latency, we observed a significant quadratic effect of WM load in healthy controls but a linear effect among patients. Similar patterns were found for response accuracy but were not statistically significant. With regard to neural activity, we found a significant bilateral linear trend of percent signal change on WM load for the FvG contrast in the DLPFC. among controls, with brain activation to faces greater than activation to designs only at the highest WM load. In the amygdala we observed a significant bilateral quadratic effect of percent signal change on WM load for the FvG contrast in the control group. We observed a significant difference in neural activation patterns in patients compared to controls in the DLPFC and the amygdala. Specifically, in patients, we observed a quadratic instead of a linear trend in the DLPFC but only in the right hemisphere. In the amygdala, the patients displayed a quadratic trend also only in the right hemisphere. In neither controls nor patients did individual differences in the quadratic effect of brain activity in the amygdala correlate with the quadratic effect in response time or accuracy. Although the correlation between the magnitude of the quadratic trend in the right amygdala at the highest WM load with general psychopathology was moderately large in patients, neither this effect nor any other brain activation effects were significantly correlated with psychopathology.
Confirming hypothesis one, controls showed the largest difference in brain activity of the FvG contrast in the DLPFC during the maintenance period at the highest WM load. However, in patients we saw significantly decreased percent signal change in DLPFC at the highest WM load on the FvG contrast in the maintenance period. For hypothesis two we observed a quadratic pattern of WM load on the FvG contrast in the maintenance period for both controls and patients, although this effect was only present in the right hemisphere of patients. Furthermore, contrary to hypothesis 3 we did not observe significant correlations between symptom severity and the magnitude of the FvG contrast in brain activity in the amygdala and DLPFC at the greatest WM load. These results suggest a separate process of social-discrimination is taking place in controls. However, this process appears to be impaired in individuals with schizophrenia. This disruption may be due to poor integration of different brain areas and interhemispheric communication. (Abstract shortened by ProQuest.)
Bohbot, Veronique Deborah 1969. "The medial temporal lobes and human memory." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1997. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/288748.
Full textKing, Michael J. "The Capacity of Visual Working Memory During Visual Search." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1586876852806965.
Full textMathias, Brian. "Effects of context on memory retrieval in music performance." Thesis, McGill University, 2011. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=104834.
Full textPlusieurs modèles de récupération en mémoire proposent que le contexte d'une séquence est intégré aux représentations planifiées lors de la production de cette séquence. Les erreurs d'ordre sériel, où des évènements corrects sont produits aux mauvais endroits dans la séquence, démontrent que l'accès à de multiples éléments dans une séquence se fait simultanément. La production de séquences musicales remet en question les modèles de planification qui se basent sur le contexte. Les relations contextuelles entre les évènements d'une séquence musicale pourraient améliorer la planification et la récupération des séquences, ce qui contredit l'idée que de longues séquences nuisent à la mémoire. Nous avons testé les prédictions du Range Model (Palmer & Pfordresher, 2003; Pfordresher et al., 2007), un modèle formel de récupération contextuelle, concernant les effets du contexte sur la planification de séquences musicales. Vingt-six pianistes expérimentés ont pratiqué de nouveaux extraits musicaux qui étaient placés dans des contextes longs et courts, jusqu'à ce qu'ils atteignent une performance sans erreurs. Ensuite, ils ont joué les séquences à des tempos rapides et modérés, qui ont été choisis pour induire des erreurs. Pour les contextes longs, les erreurs dans l'ordre sériel des notes étaient associées à de plus grandes distances entre les évènements en question dans la séquence, ainsi qu'à des évènements similaires au niveau métrique. Ces résultats confirment les prédictions du Range Model. Les contextes longs, comparés aux contextes courts, augmentaient également l'effet du tempo sur le taux d'erreur. Ces résultats suggèrent que les contextes longs facilitent la planification de séquences en augmentant la saillance des relations hiérarchiques entre les évènements, ce qui permet aux pianistes de planifier de plus longues étendues d'évènements dans la séquence. L'avantage que ces contextes longs apportent à la planification d'évènements appuie les modèles incrémentiels, et nuance les théories qui considèrent l'information contextuelle comme étant nuisible à la récupération. Dans le domaine de la performance musicale, les contextes longs peuvent faciliter la planification de séquences en renforçant les associations entre les évènements proches et similaires. Ceci est conforme aux théories contextuelles de mémoire pour les hauteurs musicales.
Valiante, A. Grace (Antonella Grace). "Natural feeding enhances human neonatal memory for spoken words." Thesis, McGill University, 2002. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=29484.
Full textNelson, Angela B. "Examining the co-evolution of knowledge and event memory." [Bloomington, Ind.] : Indiana University, 2009. http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:3380118.
Full textTitle from PDF t.p. (viewed on Jul 20, 2010). Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 70-12, Section: B, page: 7875. Adviser: Richard M. Shiffrin.
Pryor, Jennifer Maureen. "The Positivity Effect: Is it a Memory Retrieval Bias?" W&M ScholarWorks, 2011. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539626674.
Full textBrown, Rachel. "Auditory-motor integration in music performance, learning, and memory." Thesis, McGill University, 2013. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=119512.
Full textCertaines habiletés auditivomotrices, telles que parler ou jouer d'un instrument de musique, requièrent des compétences particulières sur les plans du traitement auditif des sons produits et de la production des actions menant à l'émission de ces sons. À cet égard, un nombre croissant de preuves empiriques suggère que les composantes de perception et de production des habiletés auditivomotrices s'incèrent à l'intérieur d'interactions réciproques entre le système auditif et le système moteur. Plusieurs questions concernant la façon dont des séquences auditives complexes s'alignent avec des séquences complexes de mouvements, telles que retrouvées dans la parole ou les performances musicales, demeurent néanmoins irrésolues. Notamment par rapport à la façon dont les interactions entre les systèmes auditif et moteur influencent l'apprentissage et la rétention de nouvelles séquences auditivomotrices chez des executants compétents. Les recherches décrites dans cette thèse visent à aborder ces questions dans le contexte de performances musicales. En effet, étant donné qu'elles requièrent un contrôle précis de la hauteur du son et de la sequence temporelle des événements, les performances musicales sont des comportements auditivomoteurs communs et complexes représentant un modèle avantageux dans l'examen des capacités auditivomotrices. Trois études sont proposées afin d'examiner l'influence des interactions entre les systèmes auditif et moteur sur la façon dont des musiciens compétents alignent la hauteur sonore et les sequences temporelles avec les mouvements requis lors de performances musicales, de même que sur la façon dont ils apprennent et retiennent une séquence musicale. La première étude examine la manière dont la hauteur sonore et la structuretemporelle d'une séquence musicale engagent certains réseaux neuronaux du système moteur sur le plan des interactions auditivomotrices (Chapitre 2). Cette étude révèle que les réseaux du système moteur sont sensibles à l'intensité sonore et à la structure temporelle lorsque des musiciens écoutent et jouent de la musique. Ces résultats suggèrent que le système moteur intègre de multiples dimensions relatives à la structure de la séquence auditive lorsque ces sequences auditives sont alignées avec des séquences motrices lors de performances musicales. La deuxième étude examine l'utilisation des informations auditives et motrices dans l'apprentissage de séquences auditives (Chapitre 3). Cette etude révèle que les musiciens reconnaissent mieux les séquences auditives qu'ils ont eu à apprendre en les jouant avec rétroaction auditive, par rapport à celles qu'ils ont uniquement eu à écouter. Ces résultats indiquent que l'apprentissage moteur facilite la mémorisation d'information auditive chez les exécutants compétents. La troisième étude examine l'influence des différences individuelles sur le plan des habiletés d'imagerie auditive et motrice, sur l'apprentissage de nouvelles séquences musicales et sur le rappel de ces mêmes séquences (Chapitre 4). Cette étude révèle que les habiletés d'imagerie auditive aident les exécutants à apprendre de nouvelles séquences musicales en compensant pour les sons manquants, de même qu'en réduisant l'interférence liée à l'informations non pertinente; les habiletés d'imagerie auditive améliorent également le rappel de séquences musicales lors de performances comportant une plus grande régularitétemporelle. De manière générale, ces résultats suggèrent que les habiletés d'imagerie auditive aident différemment à l'apprentissage de nouvelles sequences musicales et à leur rappel. Ensemble, ces études illustrent le fonctionnement des intégrations auditivomotrices chez les exécutants compétents, ainsi que leur contribution à l'apprentissage et à la mémorisation de séquences auditivomotrices.
Clement, Melanie. "Aspects of memory capacity and confidence in contingency judgements." Thesis, University of Ottawa (Canada), 2005. http://hdl.handle.net/10393/29207.
Full textBowers, J. M. "Schema theory and memory." Thesis, University of Cambridge, 1986. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.383077.
Full textChavira, Maria Romo 1968. "Cultural differences in reasoning and memory: A follow-up." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1996. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/289477.
Full textTitcomb, Allison Louise 1963. "False memories and fuzzy-trace theory: Misinforming gist versus verbatim memory." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1996. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/264412.
Full textAchim, Amélie M. "Neural correlates of associative and item memory." Thesis, McGill University, 2003. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=80160.
Full textJardin, Elliott C. "Recognition Memory Revisited: An Aging and Electrophysiological Investigation." Cleveland State University / OhioLINK, 2018. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=csu1548157727480549.
Full textBuitron, David A. "Radio Dispatch Cognitive Abilities and Working Memory." CSUSB ScholarWorks, 2017. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd/490.
Full textWarburg, Richard. "Assessment of memory problems by clinical neuropsychologists." Thesis, Bangor University, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.390999.
Full textDarby, Kevin Patrick. "The cost of learning: Interference effects in memory development." The Ohio State University, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1398685555.
Full textMolina, Andres Antonio Haye. "Collective memory : an investigation into its cognitive and group processes." Thesis, University of Sheffield, 2003. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.289656.
Full textWelch, Melissa Kae. "The Experience of Imagery in Relation to Memory and Problem Solving." W&M ScholarWorks, 1992. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539625777.
Full textMojardin-Heraldez, Ambrocio 1963. "The underlying memory processes of adults' spontaneous and implanted false memories." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/288855.
Full textPollock, Susan 1965. "The role of articulatory-phonological and lexical-semantic factors in short-term memory span /." Thesis, McGill University, 1997. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=27391.
Full textDavidson, Patrick. "Item and source memory with emotional materials in young and older adults." Diss., The University of Arizona, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/280467.
Full textSaqer, Haneen Rezik. "Mitigating effects of working memory constraints on automation use through interface redesign." Thesis, George Mason University, 2015. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3720807.
Full textThis dissertation investigated the role of individual differences in human use of automation in a simulated command and control task. Using this knowledge we then sought to redesign the simulation interface to improve human-automation interaction. In the first study, participants completed a battery of cognitive tasks to measure working memory capacity, simple memory span, and controlled attention ability. They then performed a simulated air defense task under varying levels of workload and automation assistance. Eye tracking data recorded fixations to capture eye movements during completion of each scenario. Although individual difference measures correlated with primary task performance, they did not predict use of automation. Only average percent of fixations on the automation messaging interface correlated with automation use. Therefore, the second study introduced a redesigned automation interface with the integration of an auditory chime and a visual flicker to promote additional fixations to the message interface and encourage increased automation use. However, this redesign did not increase average fixation percentage and surprisingly resulted in lower use of automation. This finding emphasized Parasuraman and Riley’s (1997) warning that automation can change user behavior in unintended ways. Another notable finding from the study is the unexpected result that short term memory predicted primary task performance. Further, this study provides evidence to support the use of eye tracking measures as a continuous unobtrusive measure of automation use in complex systems. Limitations and future research are also discussed.
Green, Nicola Ellen Anne. "Semantic memory anomalies in patients with schizophrenia : a cognitive psychological approach." Thesis, University of Hertfordshire, 2002. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.269429.
Full textHitchins, Matthew G. "Domain Disparity| Informing the Debate between Domain-General and Domain-Specific Information Processing in Working Memory." Thesis, The George Washington University, 2017. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10607221.
Full textWorking memory is a collection of cognitive resources that allow for the temporary maintenance and manipulation of information. This information can then be used to accomplish task goals in a variety of different contexts. To do this, the working memory system is able to process many different kinds of information using resources dedicated to the processing of those specific types of information. This processing is modulated by a control component which is responsible for guiding actions in the face of interference. Recently, the way in which working memory handles the processing of this information has been the subject of debate. Specifically, current models of working memory differ in their conceptualization of its functional architecture and the interaction between domain-specific storage structures and domain-general control processes. Here, domain-specific processing is when certain components of a model are dedicated to processing certain kinds of information, be it spatial or verbal. Domain-general processing is a when a component of a model can process multiple kinds of information. One approach conceptualizes working memory as consisting of various discrete components that are dedicated to processing specific kinds of information. These multiple component models attempt to explain how domain-specific storage structures are coordinated by a domain-general control mechanism. They also predict that capacity variations in those domain-specific storage structures can directly affect the performance of the domain-general control mechanism. Another approach focuses primarily on the contributions of a domain-general control mechanism to behavior. These controlled attention approaches collapse working memory and attention and propose that a domain-general control mechanism is the primary source of individual differences. This means that variations in domain-specific storage structures are not predicted to affect the functioning of the domain-general control mechanism. This dissertation will make the argument that conceptualizing working memory as either domain-specific or domain-general creates a false dichotomy. To do this, different ways of measuring working memory capacity will first be discussed. That discussion will serve as a basis for understanding the differences, and similarities between both models. A more detailed exposition of both the multiple component model and controlled attention account will follow. Behavioral and physiological evidence will accompany the descriptions of both models. The emphasis of the evidence presented here will be on load effects: observed changes in task performance when information is maintained in working memory. Load effects can be specific to the type of information being maintained (domain-specific), or occur regardless of information type (domain-general). This dissertation will demonstrate how the two models fail to address evidence for both domain-specific and domain-general load effects. Given these inadequacies, a new set of experiments will be proposed that will seek to demonstrate both domain-specific and domain-general effects within the same paradigm. Being able to demonstrate both these effects will go some way towards accounting for the differing evidence presented in the literature. A brief conceptualization of a possible account to explain these effects will then be discussed. Finally, future directions for research will be described.
Jones, Kelly M. "Self -efficacy, memory, and identity processes in older adults." Amherst, Mass. : University of Massachusetts Amherst, 2009. http://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations/AAI3379972/.
Full textStein, Lilian Milnitsky 1959. "Memory falsification in children: A developmental study of spontaneous and implanted false memories." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/282700.
Full textBaker, Christine M. "Effects of bilingualism on working memory ability." Thesis, California State University, Long Beach, 2013. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=1522557.
Full textMuch evidence exists in support of the notion known as a bilingual advantage, the idea that some bilinguals benefit from an executive functioning system superior to monolinguals. The majority of research investigating the bilingual advantage lies in metalinguistic awareness, conflict resolution, and inhibition; however, this thesis examines working-memory abilities by comparing the performance of English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual groups in a dual task paradigm, taxing lexical retrieval and memory maintenance and manipulation. Participants were asked to perform a lexical retrieval task eliciting high-frequency abstract nouns or adjectives while simultaneously memorizing an accumulating list of target abstract words to be later recalled. Although no difference in immediate recall between language groups was found, bilinguals remembered significantly more target words 5 days after testing. Evidence suggests that bilinguals may build new memory representations that are more resistant to decay than monolingual memory representations.
Galyer, Darin L. "The Influence of Reference Objects on Vector-Based Memory Representations." Thesis, The George Washington University, 2019. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=13421371.
Full textVectors, defined by distance and direction information, can represent the spatial relationships between reference objects and target objects. Reference boundaries help to define the space and are mathematically definable by lines, while reference landmarks define specific locations and are definable by points. How do vectors, containing two sources of information relate references and targets? Congruent with neuroscientific evidence we argued that humans rely differentially on distance and direction information when recalling the spatial location of objects. We showed that direction information was better encoded or remembered than distance information relative to landmarks, and that distance information was better encoded or remembered than direction information relative to boundaries. We proposed that the type of reference influences the fidelity of distance and direction information in the spatial representation.
Fitzgerald, Laura Elaine. "Cognitive Processes and Memory Differences in Recall and Recognition in Adults." ScholarWorks, 2016. https://scholarworks.waldenu.edu/dissertations/1600.
Full textLindsey, Charles D. "Antecedents of memory confidence for a delayed marketplace transaction." [Bloomington, Ind.] : Indiana University, 2006. http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:3215216.
Full textSource: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 67-04, Section: A, page: 1442. Adviser: Shanker H. Krishnan. "Title from dissertation home page (viewed June 19, 2007)."
Lipko, Amanda Rae. "Preschoolers' persistent overconfidence in their recall memory." [Kent, Ohio] : Kent State University, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=kent1214583736.
Full textTitle from PDF t.p. (viewed Oct. 5, 2009). Advisor: William Merriman. Keywords: metacognition; recall memory; cognitive development. Includes bibliographical references (p. 65-71).
Basak, Chandramallika. "Capacity limits of the focus of attention and dynamics of the focus switch cost in the working memory." Related electronic resource: Current Research at SU : database of SU dissertations, recent titles available full text, 2005. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/syr/main.
Full textMueller, Michael. "THE FONT-SIZE EFFECT ON JUDGMENTS OF LEARNING: DOES IT EXEMPLIFY THE EFFECT OF FLUENCY ON JOLS OR REFLECT PEOPLE'S BELIEFS ABOUT MEMORY?" Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1379950901.
Full textRiesen, Eleanor. "The relationship between spatial memory and postural balance in seniors with good and poor balance." Thesis, University of Ottawa (Canada), 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/10393/29311.
Full textJAMES, KEITH. "PERCEIVED CONFLICT OF OCCUPATIONAL AND FAMILIAL ORIENTATIONS AND INDIVIDUAL COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE (GENDER, CREATIVITY, MEMORY, REPRESSION)." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1986. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/183884.
Full text