Academic literature on the topic 'Precedence effect; Sound location'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Precedence effect; Sound location.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Precedence effect; Sound location"

1

Tollin, Daniel J., and Tom C. T. Yin. "Psychophysical Investigation of an Auditory Spatial Illusion in Cats: The Precedence Effect." Journal of Neurophysiology 90, no. 4 (2003): 2149–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00381.2003.

Full text
Abstract:
The precedence effect (PE) describes several spatial perceptual phenomena that occur when similar sounds are presented from two different locations and separated by a delay. The mechanisms that produce the effect are thought to be responsible for the ability to localize sounds in reverberant environments. Although the physiological bases for the PE have been studied, little is known about how these sounds are localized by species other than humans. Here we used the search coil technique to measure the eye positions of cats trained to saccade to the apparent locations of sounds. To study the PE
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Tollin, Daniel J., Luis C. Populin, and Tom C. T. Yin. "Neural Correlates of the Precedence Effect in the Inferior Colliculus of Behaving Cats." Journal of Neurophysiology 92, no. 6 (2004): 3286–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00606.2004.

Full text
Abstract:
Several auditory spatial illusions, collectively called the precedence effect (PE), occur when transient sounds are presented from two different spatial locations but separated in time by an interstimulus delay (ISD). For ISDs in the range of localization dominance (<10 ms), a single fused sound is typically located near the leading source location only, as if the location of the lagging source were suppressed. For longer ISDs, both the leading and lagging sources can be heard and localized, and the shortest ISD where this occurs is called the echo threshold. Previous physiological studies
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Gai, Yan, Janet L. Ruhland, and Tom C. T. Yin. "Behavior and modeling of two-dimensional precedence effect in head-unrestrained cats." Journal of Neurophysiology 114, no. 2 (2015): 1272–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00214.2015.

Full text
Abstract:
The precedence effect (PE) is an auditory illusion that occurs when listeners localize nearly coincident and similar sounds from different spatial locations, such as a direct sound and its echo. It has mostly been studied in humans and animals with immobile heads in the horizontal plane; speaker pairs were often symmetrically located in the frontal hemifield. The present study examined the PE in head-unrestrained cats for a variety of paired-sound conditions along the horizontal, vertical, and diagonal axes. Cats were trained with operant conditioning to direct their gaze to the perceived soun
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Spitzer, Matthew W., Avinash D. S. Bala, and Terry T. Takahashi. "A Neuronal Correlate of the Precedence Effect Is Associated With Spatial Selectivity in the Barn Owl's Auditory Midbrain." Journal of Neurophysiology 92, no. 4 (2004): 2051–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01235.2003.

Full text
Abstract:
Sound localization in echoic conditions depends on a precedence effect (PE), in which the first arriving sound dominates the perceived location of later reflections. Previous studies have demonstrated neurophysiological correlates of the PE in several species, but the underlying mechanisms remain unknown. The present study documents responses of space-specific neurons in the barn owl's inferior colliculus (IC) to stimuli simulating direct sounds and reflections that overlap in time at the listener's ears. Responses to 100-ms noises with lead-lag delays from 1 to 100 ms were recorded from neuro
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Litovsky, Ruth Y., Brad Rakerd, Tom C. T. Yin, and William M. Hartmann. "Psychophysical and Physiological Evidence for a Precedence Effect in the Median Sagittal Plane." Journal of Neurophysiology 77, no. 4 (1997): 2223–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.77.4.2223.

Full text
Abstract:
Litovsky, Ruth Y., Brad Rakerd, Tom C. T. Yin, and William M. Hartmann. Psychophysical and physiological evidence for a precedence effect in the median sagittal plane. J. Neurophysiol. 77: 2223–2226, 1997. A listener in a room is exposed to multiple versions of any acoustical event, coming from many different directions in space. The precedence effect is thought to discount the reflected sounds in the computation of location, so that a listener perceives the source near its true location. According to most auditory theories, the precedence effect is mediated by binaural differences. This repor
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Mickey, Brian J., and John C. Middlebrooks. "Sensitivity of Auditory Cortical Neurons to the Locations of Leading and Lagging Sounds." Journal of Neurophysiology 94, no. 2 (2005): 979–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00580.2004.

Full text
Abstract:
We recorded unit activity in the auditory cortex (fields A1, A2, and PAF) of anesthetized cats while presenting paired clicks with variable locations and interstimulus delays (ISDs). In human listeners, such sounds elicit the precedence effect, in which localization of the lagging sound is impaired at ISDs ≲10 ms. In the present study, neurons typically responded to the leading stimulus with a brief burst of spikes, followed by suppression lasting 100–200 ms. At an ISD of 20 ms, at which listeners report a distinct lagging sound, only 12% of units showed discrete lagging responses. Long-lastin
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Moore, Christopher A., Jerry L. Cranford, and Angela E. Rahn. "Tracking of a “Moving” Fused Auditory Image Under Conditions that Elicit the Precedence Effect." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 33, no. 1 (1990): 141–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3301.141.

Full text
Abstract:
Pursuit auditory tracking of a fused auditory image (FAI), based on stimulus conditions known to elicit the precedence effect phenomenon in sound localization, was investigated in 36 normal subjects and in a small group of subjects with known neuropathology. Movement of the FAI was simulated by incrementally varying the delay between two clicks presented, one each, from two loudspeakers placed on opposite sides of the listener. The group of normal listeners tracked the movement of the FAI without difficulty and with great accuracy; the perceived location of the FAI varied linearly with the int
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Cranford, Jerry L., Marci A. Andres, Kristi K. Piatz, and Kay L. Reissig. "Influences of Age and Hearing Loss on the Precedence Effect in Sound Localization." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 36, no. 2 (1993): 437–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3602.437.

Full text
Abstract:
Cranford, Boose, & Moore (1990a) reported that many elderly persons exhibit problems in perceiving the apparent location of fused auditory images in a sound localization task involving the Precedence Effect (PE). In the earlier study, differences in peripheral hearing sensitivity between young and elderly subjects were not controlled. In the present study, four groups of young and elderly subjects, matched with respect to age and the presence or absence of sensorineural hearing loss, were examined to determine the effects of these two factors on performance with the PE task. Although signi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Brown, Andrew D., Heath G. Jones, Alan Kan, et al. "Evidence for a neural source of the precedence effect in sound localization." Journal of Neurophysiology 114, no. 5 (2015): 2991–3001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00243.2015.

Full text
Abstract:
Normal-hearing human listeners and a variety of studied animal species localize sound sources accurately in reverberant environments by responding to the directional cues carried by the first-arriving sound rather than spurious cues carried by later-arriving reflections, which are not perceived discretely. This phenomenon is known as the precedence effect (PE) in sound localization. Despite decades of study, the biological basis of the PE remains unclear. Though the PE was once widely attributed to central processes such as synaptic inhibition in the auditory midbrain, a more recent hypothesis
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Dent, Micheal L., Daniel J. Tollin, and Tom C. T. Yin. "Influence of Sound Source Location on the Behavior and Physiology of the Precedence Effect in Cats." Journal of Neurophysiology 102, no. 2 (2009): 724–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00129.2009.

Full text
Abstract:
Psychophysical experiments on the precedence effect (PE) in cats have shown that they localize pairs of auditory stimuli presented from different locations in space based on the spatial position of the stimuli and the interstimulus delay (ISD) between the stimuli in a manner similar to humans. Cats exhibit localization dominance for pairs of transient stimuli with |ISDs| from ∼0.4 to 10 ms, summing localization for |ISDs| < 0.4 ms and breakdown of fusion for |ISDs| > 10 ms, which is the approximate echo threshold. The neural correlates to the PE have been described in both anesthetized a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Precedence effect; Sound location"

1

Tollin, Daniel Joshua. "Some aspects of the lateralization of echoed sound in man." Thesis, University of Oxford, 1997. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.363729.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Shub, Daniel E. (Daniel Eric) 1974. "The role of the precedence effect in sound source lateralization." Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2001. http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/86768.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Paterson, Miles Andrew McLean. "Sound localization in reverberant environments : physiological bases of the precedence effect." Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2005. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1445900/.

Full text
Abstract:
Localization dominance, a phenomenon of the precedence effect, refers to the dominance of directional cues conveyed by sound arriving directly from the source over cues conveyed by reflected copies on the perception of sound source location. One theory of localization dominance is that leading sounds suppress neural responses to lagging sounds (Yin, 1994 Litovsky & Yin, 1998 a, b). Neurons in auditory nuclei respond best to a leading sound and have a reduced response to a lagging sound, supporting this hypothesis. It has been proposed that GABA-ergic or glycinergic inhibition suppresses neural
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Yao, Norikazu. "Auditory localisation : contributions of sound location and semantic spatial cues." Queensland University of Technology, 2007. http://eprints.qut.edu.au/16504/.

Full text
Abstract:
In open skill sports and other tasks, decision-making can be as important as physical performance. Whereas many studies have investigated visual perception there is little research on auditory perception as one aspect of decision making. Auditory localisation studies have almost exclusively focussed on underlying processes, such as interaural time difference and interaural level difference. It is not known, however, whether semantic spatial information contained in the sound is actually used, and whether it assists pure auditory localisation. The aim of this study was to investigate the effect
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

"Investigating Compensatory Mechanisms for Sound Localization: Visual Cue Integration and the Precedence Effect." Master's thesis, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/2286/R.I.34880.

Full text
Abstract:
abstract: Sound localization can be difficult in a reverberant environment. Fortunately listeners can utilize various perceptual compensatory mechanisms to increase the reliability of sound localization when provided with ambiguous physical evidence. For example, the directional information of echoes can be perceptually suppressed by the direct sound to achieve a single, fused auditory event in a process called the precedence effect (Litovsky et al., 1999). Visual cues also influence sound localization through a phenomenon known as the ventriloquist effect. It is classically demonstrated by a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Ziemer, Tim. "Perceptual sound field synthesis concept for music presentation." 2017. https://slub.qucosa.de/id/qucosa%3A70632.

Full text
Abstract:
A perceptual sound field synthesis approach for music is presented. Its signal processing implements critical bands, the precedence effect and integration times of the auditory system by technical means, as well as the radiation characteristics of musical instruments. Furthermore, interaural coherence, masking and auditory scene analysis principles are considered. As a result, the conceptualized sound field synthesis system creates a natural, spatial sound impression for listeners in extended listening area, even with a low number of loudspeakers. A novel technique, the “precedence fade”, as w
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Li, Na 1980 Oct 2. "Binaural mechanism revealed with in vivo whole cell patch clamp recordings in the inferior colliculus." Thesis, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/2152/ETD-UT-2010-12-2065.

Full text
Abstract:
Many cells in the inferior colliculus (IC) are excited by contralateral and inhibited by ipsilateral stimulation and are thought to be important for sound localization. These excitatory-inhibitory (EI) cells comprise a diverse group, even though they exhibit a common binaural response property. Previous extracellular studies proposed specific excitatory and/or inhibitory events that should be evoked by each ear and thereby generate each of the EI discharge properties. The proposals were inferences based on the well established response features of neurons in lower nuclei, the projections of t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Precedence effect; Sound location"

1

An, Sung Jun, Rhee Man Kil, and Byoung-Gi Lee. "Zero-Crossings with the Precedence Effect for Sound Source Localization in Reverberant Conditions." In Neural Information Processing. Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-42051-1_38.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Toole, Floyd E. "Reflections, Images, and the Precedence Effect." In Sound Reproduction. Elsevier, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-240-52009-4.50010-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

"Refl ections, Images, and the Precedence Effect." In Sound Reproduction. Routledge, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780080888019-13.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Tierno, Michael. "It’s all for effect." In Location and Postproduction Sound for Low-Budget Filmmakers. Routledge, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780429331305-19.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Mann, J. I., and A. S. Truswell. "Diseases of affluent societies and the need for dietary change." In Oxford Textbook of Medicine, edited by Katherine Younger. Oxford University Press, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med/9780198746690.003.0222.

Full text
Abstract:
Nutritional problems of a country depend more upon the stage of technical and economic development than geographical location. People in affluent societies have ready access to food all year round. The diet is typically energy-dense, high in fat and often also in sugar. There are multiple sources of nutritional advice, not all based on sound science. Obesity is the most obvious and important nutritional disease in affluent societies, with comorbidities including type 2 diabetes, coronary heart disease, hypertension, some cancers, gallstones, osteoarthritis, and obstructive sleep apnoea. Obese people may also be disadvantaged by social, economic, and psychological effects. Particular dietary constituents promote or protect against coronary heart disease by their effect on cardiovascular risk factors, and some promote or protect against various cancers.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Douglas, Raymond S., and Robert A. Goldberg. "Evaluation and Spectrum of Orbital Diseases." In Surgery of the Eyelid, Lacrimal System, and Orbit. Oxford University Press, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195340211.003.0024.

Full text
Abstract:
Although orbital disorders are not frequently encountered in the comprehensive ophthalmologist’s practice, it is essential to be able to diagnose patients with orbital disease and manage them accordingly. Various disease processes can affect the orbit. This chapter endeavors to provide a thoughtful, stepwise, and logical approach to the evaluation of orbital disease. The discussion begins with differential diagnosis, adds an intelligent history-taking and physical examination, and then focuses on efficient use of diagnostic tests to finally arrive at the correct diagnosis. The staging and management of two common orbital disorders, orbital inflammation and thyroid-associated ophthalmopathy, will also be discussed. The differential diagnosis of orbital disease is extensive, and most listings of orbital disease divide the causes between histopathologic and mechanistic categories. This type of grouping is intellectually sound and scientifically useful but does not provide a framework that the clinical practitioner can easily grasp and directly use in sorting through the differential diagnosis of any given patient. In broad terms, orbital disease can be considered in terms of location, extent, and biologic activity. The classification used in this chapter is broken down along clinical lines and takes advantage of the fact that the orbit has a somewhat limited repertoire of ways that it can respond to pathologic conditions. Orbital disease can be categorized into five basic clinical patterns: inflammatory, mass effect, structural, vascular, and functional. Although many cases cross over into several categories, the vast majority of clinical presentations fit predominantly into one of these patterns. As the clinician walks through each step of the evaluation process—history, physical examination, laboratory testing, orbital imaging—a conscious effort should be made to categorize the presentation within this framework. If the practitioner approaches orbital disease with this framework of discrete patterns of clinical presentation, then at every step of the diagnostic pathway (history, physical examination, orbital imaging studies, and special tests), he or she can draw from a defined set of differential diagnoses that characterize each pattern of orbital disease and use that information to efficiently and confidently orchestrate diagnosis and management.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Precedence effect; Sound location"

1

Owens, Steffan, and Stuart Cunningham. "Auditory Masking and the Precedence Effect in Studies of Musical Timekeeping." In AM'18: Sound in Immersion and Emotion. ACM, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3243274.3243312.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Zhao, Wei, Qifei He, Zhuoran Yang, and Zihan Chen. "Promotion Effect of Sound Image Location by Unity Audio-Visual Interaction." In 2018 Ninth International Conference on Intelligent Control and Information Processing (ICICIP). IEEE, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icicip.2018.8606697.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Finnegan, Shane Leslie, Craig Meskell, and Samir Ziada. "The Effect of Sound Pressure on the Aeroacoustic Sources Around Two Ducted Tandem Cylinders." In ASME 2010 3rd Joint US-European Fluids Engineering Summer Meeting collocated with 8th International Conference on Nanochannels, Microchannels, and Minichannels. ASMEDC, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/fedsm-icnmm2010-30271.

Full text
Abstract:
An empirical investigation of the spatial distribution of aeroacoustic sources around two tandem cylinders subject to ducted flow and forced transverse acoustic resonance is described. The work builds on a previous investigation by the authors and utilises Howe’s theory of aerodynamic sound. The influence of the sound pressure level in the duct on the strength and location of the aeroacoustic sources in the flow was the main focus of the investigation and experiments to resolve the aeroacoustic source distribution were concentrated at a low main-stream flow velocity (before acoustic-Strouhal c
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Gu¨lac¸ti, B., S. Aubrun, A. Seraudie, and D. Arnal. "The Effect of the Source Location on the Acoustic Excitation on Laminar-Turbulent Transition on Swept Wing Boundary Layer." In ASME 2002 Joint U.S.-European Fluids Engineering Division Conference. ASMEDC, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/fedsm2002-31397.

Full text
Abstract:
The effect of the source location and the direction of the propagation on the laminar-turbulent transition on swept-wing three-dimensional boundary layers are investigated experimentally. Also the crossflow case is handled in detail. The source for the acoustic excitation is placed in four different locations: in front of the wing, on top of the test section, behind the wing and in front of the wind tunnel. Three different experimental cases (streamwise, crossflow and mixed cases) are examined for each location with two different excitation bands. For the most efficient frequency ranges and th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Herman, Cila. "Quantitative Visualization of the Thermoacoustic Effect." In 2010 14th International Heat Transfer Conference. ASMEDC, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/ihtc14-23286.

Full text
Abstract:
Thermoacoustic energy conversion was introduced into engineering systems during the past three decades as a new, alternative, environmentally safe energy conversion technology. It uses noble gases and mixtures of noble gases as working fluids rather than hazardous refrigerants required for the vapor compression cycle. A thermoacoustic system can operate both as a prime mover/engine (a temperature gradient and heat flow imposed across the stack lead to the generation of acoustic work/sound in the resonator) and, when reversing the thermodynamic cycle, as a refrigerator (acoustic work is used to
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Golubev, Vladimir, Cody Sewell, Lap Nguyen, and Reda Mankbadi. "Effect of Synthetic-Jet Actuation on Airfoil Acoustic Radiation." In ASME 2012 Fluids Engineering Division Summer Meeting collocated with the ASME 2012 Heat Transfer Summer Conference and the ASME 2012 10th International Conference on Nanochannels, Microchannels, and Minichannels. American Society of Mechanical Engineers, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/fedsm2012-72462.

Full text
Abstract:
Synthetic jet actuators (SJAs) have been primarily investigated as means of unsteady flow control on aircraft wing. The current work for the first time explores the effectiveness of SJAs for control of a low-speed airfoil acoustic radiation both with the uniform upstream flow conditions (with noise dominated by the trailing-edge sources) and in the presence of an upstream flow disturbance (with noise dominated by the leading-edge sources or stall-related phenomena depending on the disturbance character and amplitude). In the high-fidelity numerical study, the effect of the selected SJA locatio
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Elnajjar, Emad, Mohamed Y. E. Selim, and Farag Omar. "Effect of Dual Fuel Engine Parameters and Fuel Type on Engine Noise Emissions." In ASME 2010 10th Biennial Conference on Engineering Systems Design and Analysis. ASMEDC, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/esda2010-24253.

Full text
Abstract:
Investigating experimentally the effects of different fuel types and engine parameters on the overall generated engine noise levels. Engine parameters such as: Engine speed, Injection timing angle, engine loading, different pilot fuel to gases fuel ratio and engine compression ratio. Engine noises due to combustion, turbulent flow and motoring were reported in this study by direct sound pressure level SPL (dB) measurements and compared to the maximum cylinder pressure rise rate with respect to the engine crank angle (dP/dθ)max. Experimental procedures conducted using a Ricardo diesel version v
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Vázquez, Raúl, Diego Torre, and Adolfo Serrano. "The Effect of Airfoil Clocking on Efficiency and Noise of Low Pressure Turbines." In ASME Turbo Expo 2013: Turbine Technical Conference and Exposition. American Society of Mechanical Engineers, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/gt2013-94243.

Full text
Abstract:
The effect of airfoil clocking (stator-stator interaction) on efficiency and noise of low pressure turbines (LPT) was investigated experimentally in a multistage turbine high-speed rig. The rig consisted of three stages of a state-of-the-art LPT. The stages were characterized by a very high wall-slope angle, reverse cut-off design, very high lift and very high aspect ratio airfoils. The rig had identical blade count for the second and third stators. The circumferential position of the second stator was individually adjusted with respect to the third stator. Eight different circumferential cloc
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Patching, Richard G. "Pipe Supports and Skid Platforms: An Overlooked Noise Problem." In 2000 3rd International Pipeline Conference. American Society of Mechanical Engineers, 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/ipc2000-269.

Full text
Abstract:
When an energy industry facility must meet environmental noise regulations, the primary noise sources are the drivers (such as engines and motors), driven tools (such as compressors and pumps), air moving devices, and turbulent flow in valves and piping. The primary sound transmission path is the airborne radiation of noise, which is controlled by enclosures, lagging and silencers. The opportunity for sound energy to be transmitted through structural vibration and reradiated at another location is largely overlooked in typical acoustic impact analyses. Pipe support and skid structures often ha
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Ozer, Mehmet Bulent, and Thomas J. Royston. "Applications of the Sherman-Morrison Matrix Inversion Formula in Linear and Non-Linear Vibrations, Controls and Acoustics." In ASME 2003 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference. ASMEDC, 2003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/detc2003/vib-48554.

Full text
Abstract:
Applications of the Sherman-Morrison matrix inversion formula are reviewed and demonstrated for several problems in sound and vibration control. The inversion formula enables one to easily separate the effect of a perturbation or subcomponent on the dynamic behavior of the overall system. Applications of this technique that are demonstrated here include: identifying optimal PZT electrical shunt parameter values to minimize sound radiation from a PZT-plate structure, identifying the optimal location and parameter values of a tuned dynamic vibration absorber attached to a multi-degree of freedom
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!