Journal articles on the topic 'Person perception'

To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Person perception.

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Person perception.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Seemann, Axel. "Person perception." Philosophical Explorations 11, no. 3 (September 2008): 245–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13869790802239201.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

McAndrew, Francis T., and Chelsea Rae De Jonge. "Electronic Person Perception." Social Psychological and Personality Science 2, no. 4 (December 20, 2010): 403–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550610393988.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Young, Andrew W., and Vicki Bruce. "Understanding person perception." British Journal of Psychology 102, no. 4 (June 23, 2011): 959–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02045.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Peltzer, Karl. "PERSONALITY AND PERSON PERCEPTION IN AFRICA." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 30, no. 1 (January 1, 2002): 83–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2002.30.1.83.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper reviews personality and social behavior in Africa. Three distinctive components of the people inhabiting present-day sub-Saharan Africa are identified: (1) traditional persons who are yet little affected by modernization, (2) transitional persons, and (3) modern individuals. The socialization of traditional and transitional persons can be illustrated in the form of a model in three dimensions: the authority dimension (vertical, diachronic, historic); the group dimension (horizontal, synchronic, social); the body – mind – environment dimension. Various personhood attributes are identified along the three dimension such as that the traditional person is socialized primarily by people, while the modern person is socialized primarily by objects. By being exposed to people, the traditional person will develop more social intelligence than technological intelligence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Harvey, John H., Tara L. Anthony, and Hans-Werner Bierhoff. "Person Perception and Attribution." Contemporary Sociology 19, no. 5 (September 1990): 755. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2072392.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Garcia-Marques, Teresa, and Diane M. Mackie. "Familiarity impacts person perception." European Journal of Social Psychology 37, no. 5 (September 21, 2006): 839–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.387.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

West, Robert. "Issues in person perception." Personality and Individual Differences 6, no. 2 (January 1985): 287–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(85)90126-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Slepian, Michael L., Nicholas O. Rule, and Nalini Ambady. "Proprioception and Person Perception." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 38, no. 12 (September 5, 2012): 1621–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167212457786.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hamilton, David L., and Natalie A. Wyer. "Person Perception and Interpersonal Perception: Bridging the Gap." Psychological Inquiry 7, no. 3 (July 1996): 259–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0703_13.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Behling, Dorothy. "School Uniforms and Person Perception." Perceptual and Motor Skills 79, no. 2 (October 1994): 723–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1994.79.2.723.

Full text
Abstract:
For this study of perceived behavior and academic ability in a secondary school setting, four clothing styles including two styles of school uniforms were photographed and manipulated. 270 students and 20 teachers from a public high school and a private school, in which uniforms are required, participated. A repeated-measures analysis of variance as well as a Tukey's test were used for data analysis. Independent variables were sex of the model, school, and style of clothing. Dependent variables were perception of behavior, scholastic achievement, and scholastic potential. Perception of school-related behavior and scholastic ability of the models dressed in four styles of clothing varied significantly by style of dress. There were also significant effects for sex of the model and status (teacher vs student) of the perceiver. Results were similar for the two schools in that a school uniform positively affected the perception of academic abilities and school-related behavior of the clothed models for students and teachers.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Oltmanns, Thomas F., and Eric Turkheimer. "Person Perception and Personality Pathology." Current Directions in Psychological Science 18, no. 1 (February 2009): 32–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01601.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

McCann, C. Douglas, Thomas M. Ostrom, Linda K. Tyner, and Mark L. Mitchell. "Person perception in heterogeneous groups." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49, no. 6 (1985): 1449–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1449.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Patterson, Miles L. "Interaction Behavior and Person Perception." Small Group Research 25, no. 2 (May 1994): 172–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1046496494252002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Katz, Phyllis A., Louise Silvern, and Diane K. Coulter. "Gender Processing and Person Perception." Social Cognition 8, no. 2 (June 1990): 186–202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.1990.8.2.186.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Friedman, Asia, and Ashley S. Waggoner. "Subcultural Influences on Person Perception." Social Psychology Quarterly 73, no. 4 (November 11, 2010): 325–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0190272510389002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Chapin, John. "Third-Person Perception and Facebook." International Journal of Cyber Behavior, Psychology and Learning 4, no. 3 (July 2014): 34–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijcbpl.2014070103.

Full text
Abstract:
A survey of adolescents (N = 1,488) documented third-person perception (TPP) regarding Facebook use and cyber bullying. As Facebook establishes itself as the dominant social network, users expose themselves to a level of bullying not possible in the analog world. The study found that 84% of adolescents (middle school through college undergraduates) use Facebook, and that most users log on daily. While 30% of the sample reported being cyber bullied, only 12.5% quit using the site, and only 18% told a parent or school official. Despite heavy use and exposure, adolescents exhibit TPP, believing others are more likely to be negatively affected by Facebook use. The study contributes to the TPP literature, by linking the perceptual bias to self-protective behaviors. A range of self-protective behaviors from precautionary (deleting or blocking abusive users) to reactionary (quitting Facebook) were related to decreased degrees of TPP. TPP was also related to optimistic bias, experience, liking of and use of Facebook, and perceived subjective norms and age. Implications for prevention education are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

HENRIKSEN, LISA, and JUNE A. FLORA. "Third-Person Perception and Children." Communication Research 26, no. 6 (December 1999): 643–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365099026006001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Newtson, Darren. "From Person to Social Perception." Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 37, no. 5 (May 1992): 441–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/032105.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Harrison, Deborah. "Perception and the older person." Nursing and Residential Care 12, no. 5 (May 2010): 241–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.12968/nrec.2010.12.5.47779.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Macrae, C. Neil, and Galen V. Bodenhausen. "Social cognition: Categorical person perception." British Journal of Psychology 92, no. 1 (February 2001): 239–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712601162059.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Antonakis, John. "Person Perception in Organizational Processes." Contemporary Psychology 47, no. 4 (August 2002): 381–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/001159.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Goodwin, Geoffrey P. "Moral Character in Person Perception." Current Directions in Psychological Science 24, no. 1 (February 2015): 38–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550709.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Siebler, Frank. "Emergent Attributes in Person Perception." Social Psychology 39, no. 2 (January 2008): 83–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335.39.2.83.

Full text
Abstract:
In person perception, emergent attributes are attributes that people ascribe to members of a rare or novel category combination, although they would not ascribe the same attributes to members of either of the constituent categories. The present paper first describes the processing mechanisms suggested by three theoretical models of attribute emergence. Then, competing response time predictions are derived from the models’ respective mechanisms. An empirical test of these predictions in a laboratory experiment with university students (N = 45) is reported. Results support Hastie, Schroeder, and Weber’s (1990 ) two-stage model, but not Kunda, Miller, and Claire’s (1990 ) impression-formation model or Smith and DeCoster’s (1998 ) connectionist account.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Glucksberg, Sam. "From speech perception to person perception? Not quite yet." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12, no. 4 (December 1989): 765–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00025723.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Yichen, CUI, and WANG Pei. "The dynamic interactive model of person construal on person perception." Advances in Psychological Science 26, no. 4 (2018): 678. http://dx.doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1042.2018.00678.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Greven, Inez M., Paul E. Downing, and Richard Ramsey. "Linking person perception and person knowledge in the human brain." Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11, no. 4 (February 25, 2016): 641–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv148.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Chapin, John. "Third-Person Perception and Health Beliefs." Psychology 02, no. 04 (2011): 355–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2011.24055.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Boski, Pawel. "Cross-Cultural Studies of Person Perception." Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 19, no. 3 (September 1988): 287–328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022188193002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Quadflieg, Susanne, Natasha Flannigan, Gordon D. Waiter, Bruno Rossion, Gagan S. Wig, David J. Turk, and C. Neil Macrae. "Stereotype-based modulation of person perception." NeuroImage 57, no. 2 (July 2011): 549–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Wible, David S., and C. Harry Hui. "Perceived Language Proficiency and Person Perception." Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 16, no. 2 (June 1985): 206–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022002185016002005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Andreassen, Paul B. "A Balanced Look at Person Perception." Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 30, no. 11 (November 1985): 887–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/023333.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Ryder, John. "Person perception in post-school education." Vocational Aspect of Education 39, no. 104 (December 1987): 81–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10408347308002941.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Leone, Ron, and Kimberly L. Bissell. "Movie Ratings and Third-Person Perception." Atlantic Journal of Communication 13, no. 4 (December 2005): 272–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15456889ajc1304_4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Leone, Ron, Wendy Chapman Peek, and Kimberly L. Bissell. "Reality Television and Third-Person Perception." Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 50, no. 2 (June 2006): 253–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem5002_5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Bodenhausen, Galen V., and C. Neil Macrae. "Putting A Face on Person Perception." Social Cognition 24, no. 5 (October 2006): 511–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2006.24.5.511.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Howell, Robert J. "Perception from the First-Person Perspective." European Journal of Philosophy 24, no. 1 (November 12, 2013): 187–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ejop.12065.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Chapin, John. "Third‐person perception and school violence." Communication Research Reports 19, no. 3 (June 2002): 216–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08824090209384850.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Nilsson, Ingrid, and Bo Ekehammar. "Person-positivity bias in political perception?" European Journal of Social Psychology 17, no. 2 (April 1987): 247–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420170210.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Smith, Eliot R., and Elizabeth C. Collins. "Contextualizing person perception: Distributed social cognition." Psychological Review 116, no. 2 (2009): 343–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015072.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Scharrer, Erica. "Third-Person Perception and Television Violence." Communication Research 29, no. 6 (December 2002): 681–704. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365002237832.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

He, Wuming, Siyuan Guo, Jing Jiang, Xinyue Zhou, and Ding-Guo Gao. "Physical pain induces negative person perception." Asian Journal of Social Psychology 19, no. 3 (April 25, 2016): 189–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12134.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Clifford, C. W. G., I. Mareschal, Y. Otsuka, and T. L. Watson. "A Bayesian approach to person perception." Consciousness and Cognition 36 (November 2015): 406–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.03.015.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Card, Alison L., Linda A. Jackson, Gary E. Stollak, and Nicholas S. Ialongo. "Gender role and person-perception accuracy." Sex Roles 15, no. 3-4 (August 1986): 159–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00287481.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Forgas, Joseph P., and Gordon H. Bower. "Mood effects on person-perception judgments." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53, no. 1 (1987): 53–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.53.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Oh, DongWon, Mirella Walker, and Jonathan B. Freeman. "Person knowledge shapes face identity perception." Cognition 217 (December 2021): 104889. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104889.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Kim, Hyunjung. "Impact of Perception: Third-Person Perception, Anxiety, and Attitude Change." Communication Research Reports 31, no. 2 (April 2014): 154–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08824096.2014.907143.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Lee, Angela M., and Renita Coleman. "‘We’re more ethical than they are’: Third-person and first-person perceptions of the ethical climate of American journalists." Journalism 21, no. 9 (May 30, 2018): 1157–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1464884918778249.

Full text
Abstract:
This study advances our understanding of mainstream US journalists’ perceptions of their industry’s ethical climate by extending first- and third-person perception to this domain. These journalists believe that colleagues in their same organization act unethically significantly less often and act ethically significantly more often than those at other organizations and in related industries. The first- and third-person perceptions are a linear function of social distance but are not the mirror image of each other in this domain. In addition to theoretical contributions, suggestions for ways these perceptions can be used to improve journalists’ moral judgment are offered.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Driscoll, Paul D., and Michael B. Salwen. "Self-Perceived Knowledge of the O.J. Simpson Trial: Third-Person Perception and Perceptions of Guilt." Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly 74, no. 3 (September 1997): 541–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/107769909707400308.

Full text
Abstract:
This study tested the “third-person effect” during the O.J. Simpson double-murder trial. The perceptual component of the third-person effect predicts that people judge themselves to be less susceptible to media influence than other people. Findings from a nationwide telephone survey indicated that respondents' self-perceived knowledge about the legal issues involved in the Simpson trial was correlated with third-person perception of a perceived “neutral” media message. Self-perceived knowledge was not correlated with third-person perceptual bias of a perceived “biased” message. It was suggested that the biased message primed respondents' perceptions of Simpson's guilt or innocence. The relative contributions of various predictors of third-person perception were assessed using regression analysis.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Papeo, Liuba, and Angelika Lingnau. "First-person and third-person verbs in visual motion-perception regions." Brain and Language 141 (February 2015): 135–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2014.11.011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Van Lange, Paul A. M., and Wim B. G. Liebrand. "On perceiving morality and potency: Social values and the effects of person perception in a give‐some dilemma." European Journal of Personality 3, no. 3 (September 1989): 209–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2410030306.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study examines a two‐person give‐some dilemma characterized by the conflict between the pursuit of own benefits (not giving) and collective benefits (giving). The major purpose was two‐fold: (a) to examine the effects of person perceptions manipulated along the dimensions of morality (goodness) and potency (strength) on co‐operation, and (b) to examine whether pre‐existing differences between individuals in their preference for specific self‐other outcome distributions (social values) would modify the effects of person perception. First, we predicted and found that across social values the degree of co‐operative behaviour increased as a linear function of the extent to which the other was seen as moral. Concerning the perceptions in terms of potency, we found a significant quadratic trend; another seen as moderate on potency elicited more co‐operative behaviour than another seen as either high or low on potency. These effects of person perception were not moderated by social value. More interesting was the finding that even though persons classified as pro‐social (co‐operators and altruists) and pro‐self (individualists and competitors) held about the same expectation about the magnitude of another's co‐operation, pro‐socials behaved more co‐operatively than pro‐selfs. This suggests that under certain conditions behavioural differences between pro‐socials and pro‐selfs are not conditional upon expectational differences between those two social values.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography