Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Perceptual discrimination'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 31 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Perceptual discrimination.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Fair, Joseph Edward. "Infant Facial Discrimination and Perceptual Narrowing." BYU ScholarsArchive, 2009. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/2154.
Full textLavis, Yvonna Marie Psychology Faculty of Science UNSW. "An investigation of the mechanisms responsible for perceptual learning in humans." Publisher:University of New South Wales. Psychology, 2008. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/42882.
Full textMauney, Lisa M. "Individual Differences in Cognitive, Musical, and Perceptual Abilities." Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/13972.
Full textLalonde, Jasmin. "Task-dependent transfer of perceptual to memory representations during delayed spatial frequency discrimination." Thesis, McGill University, 2001. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=33911.
Full textChen, Xing. "Perceptual learning of contrast discrimination and its neural correlates in macaque V4 and V1." Thesis, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10443/2403.
Full textTaylor, Andrea. "Assessing the Effects of Stress Resilience Training on Visual Discrimination Skills: Implications for Perceptual Resilience in U.S. Warfighters." VCU Scholars Compass, 2012. http://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd/2745.
Full textWeddell, Di, and n/a. "The relationship of colour systems to the perceptual discrimination of colour in Year 7 students." University of Canberra. Education, 1995. http://erl.canberra.edu.au./public/adt-AUC20061110.113934.
Full textKing, Robert A. "Perceptual grouping selection rules in visual search : methods of sub-group selection in multiple target visual search tasks." Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/32821.
Full textCarnot, Mary Jo. "The role of identical component information in similarity, discrimination, grouping and detection tasks." The Ohio State University, 2004. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1100589505.
Full textGong, Mingliang. "Orientation discrimination in periphery: Surround suppression or crowding?" Miami University / OhioLINK, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=miami1430433449.
Full textAime, Mattia. "Circuit mechanisms for encoding discriminative learning in the dorsal prefrontal cortex of behaving mice." Thesis, Bordeaux, 2017. http://www.theses.fr/2017BORD0805/document.
Full textThe ability of an organism to predict forthcoming events is crucial for survival, and depends on the repeated contingency and contiguity between sensory cues and the events (i.e. danger) they must predict. The resulting learned association provides an accurate representation of the environment by increasing discriminative skills between threat and safety signals, most likely as a result of the interaction between the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the basolateral amygdala (BLA). Although it suggests that local neuronal networks in the PFC might encode opposing memories that are preferentially selected during recall by recruiting specific cortical or subcortical structures, whether such a discriminative representation is wired within discrete prefrontal circuits during learning and by which synaptic mechanisms remain unclear. Here, the work at issue demonstrates that discrimination learning of both safe and fear-conditioned stimuli depends on full activity of the frontal association cortex, and is associated with the formation of cue-specific neuronal assemblies therein. During learning, prefrontal pyramidal neurons were potentiated through sensory-driven dendritic non-linearities supported by the activation of long-range inputs from the basolateral amygdala (BLA). Taken together, these data provide evidence for a new synaptic level mechanism that coincidently link (or meta-associate) during learning features of perceived experience with BLA mediated emotional state into prefrontal memory assemblies
Jantzen, Kelly J. "Evidence that plasticity in SI is not responsible for perceptual improvement resulting from training on a tactile frequency discrimination task." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1999. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape9/PQDD_0024/NQ51875.pdf.
Full textPosid, Tasha Irene. "The small-large divide: The development of infant abilities to discriminate small from large sets." Thesis, Boston College, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/2345/bc-ir:104371.
Full textThesis advisor: Ellen Winner
Evidence suggests that humans and non-human animals have access to two distinct numerical representation systems: a precise "object-file" system used to visually track small quantities (<4) and an approximate, ratio-dependent analog magnitude system used to represent all natural numbers. Although many studies to date indicate that infants can discriminate exclusively small sets (e.g., 1 vs. 2, 2 vs. 3) or exclusively large sets (4 vs. 8, 8 vs. 16), a robust phenomenon exists whereby they fail to compare sets crossing this small-large boundary (2 vs. 4, 3 vs. 6) despite a seemingly favorable ratio of difference between the two set sizes. Despite these robust failures in infancy (up to 14 months), studies suggest that 3-year old children no longer encounter difficulties comparing small from large sets, yet little work has explored the development of this phenomenon between 14 months and 3 years of age. The present study investigates (1) when in development infants naturally overcome this inability to compare small vs. large sets, as well as (2) what factors may facilitate this ability: namely, perceptual variability and/or numerical language. Results from three cross-sectional studies indicate that infants begin to discriminate between small and large sets as early as 17 months of age. Furthermore, infants seemed to benefit from perceptual variability of the items in the set when making these discriminations. Moreover, although preliminary evidence suggests that a child's ability to verbally count may correlate with success on these discriminations, simply exposure to numerical language (in the form of adult modeling of labeling the cardinality and counting the set) does not affect performance
Thesis (PhD) — Boston College, 2015
Submitted to: Boston College. Graduate School of Arts and Sciences
Discipline: Psychology
Sawadogo, Ousmane. "PERCEPTION OF /q/ IN THE ARABIC /q/-/k/ CONTRAST BY NATIVE SPEAKERS OF AMERICAN ENGLISH: A DISCRIMINATION TASK." OpenSIUC, 2015. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/theses/1695.
Full textGalois, Patrick. "Turtle nest sensory perception by raccoon (Procyon lotor) and striped skunk (Mephitis mephitis) : an approach through discrimination learning of potential nest cues." Thesis, McGill University, 1996. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=42038.
Full textAlbareda, Castellot Bàrbara. "Developmental changes in vowel perception: how input interplays with initial perceptual biases." Doctoral thesis, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/7213.
Full textL'objectiu d'aquesta tesis és analitzar la dinàmica que s'estableix entre els biaixos acústics i l'experiència amb el llenguatge durant el primer any de vida. Aquest és un període decisiu durant el qual les categories fonètiques s'ajusten a les propietats de la llengua materna. Aquest objectiu s'assoleix integrant els resultats de dos estudis. En el primer estudi s'explora la presència d'asimetries en la percepció de les vocals en bebès de 4 a 12 mesos d'edat, en funció de la saliència acústica i les propietats distribucionals de la llengua. El segon estudi investiga els resultats contradictoris obtinguts amb bebès de 8 mesos d'edat bilingües, i la seva relació amb les demandes de la tasca i les propietats de l'input bilingüe. Els resultats d'aquests estudis indiquen dos tendències al desenvolupament. Per una banda, que les asimetries en la percepció de les vocals són modulades per biaixos acústics abans i durant la reorganització perceptiva i per les propietats distribucionals després de la reorganització perceptiva; per l'altra banda, els resultats mostren que les habilitats de discriminació dels bilingües de 8 mesos d'edat són corresponents a les dels monolingües quan es fa servir un procediment d'anticipació de la mirada, posant en rellevància la importància de les demandes de la tasca en determinar el rendiment dels bebès.
Hodgson, Eric P. "The interaction of transient and enduring spatial representations using visual cues to maintain perceptual engagement /." Oxford, Ohio : Miami University, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=miami1217959226.
Full textMarchelli, Leopoldo Francisco Barletta. "Envolvimento de processos atencionais em tarefas de escolhas relacionadas à detecção e discriminação de estímulos sonoros, em ratos: avaliação comportamental." Universidade de São Paulo, 2016. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/41/41135/tde-22032017-104659/.
Full textThe influence of attention in decision making processes is usually evaluated using two-alternative choice tasks in which the subjects react relying on detection and discrimination of two discrete stimuli. The possibilities of manipulating attentional demands in this kind of task is relatively restricted. The experimental design employed in the present experiments aimed at providing ways to manipulate the attentional demands during performance of an auditory task. This was achieved by presenting variable sequences of non-informative 6 kHz beeps, bilaterally, followed by one distinctive (either 3 or 10 kHz) informative beep presented either (1) bilaterally such that the frequency identifies the required response (Experiment 1) or (2) unilaterally such that the place of presentation identifies the required response (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1 data showed a decrease in both reaction times for target stimuli and percentage of comission errors as a function of the number of non-informative stimuli. These effects occurred early in training for both, 3 and 10 kHz target stimuli, and only for 10 kHz target stimulus after reversal of training. Interestingly, reaction times for 10 kHz target stimuli were longer relative to those seen for 3 kHz target stimuli. In contrast, reduction of the percentage of comission errors was stronger in trials using 10 kHz target stimuli. In Experiment 2 reaction times increased as a function of the increase in the number of non-informative stimuli. In addition, reaction times for 10 kHz target stimuli presented in the left side were longer as compared to both 10 kHz target stimuli presented in the right side and 3 kHz stimuli presented in both sides. Congruently, a smaller percentage of correct responses and a greater percentage of comission errors were seen when the 10 kHz target stimuli were presented in the left side. The percentage of correct responses and reaction times reduction were greater in the Experiment 2 as compared to Experiment 1, suggesting that in the present experimental conditions, identification of the place for stimulus presentation is prompter as compared to the difference in the frequency of the target stimuli. Together these results show that engagement and maintenance of attentional resources along time vary as a function of the peripheral and simbolic nature of the target stimuli. The frequency of target stimuli presented after a variable sequence of non-informative stimuli also interferes in performance of the subject
Göhringer, Frederic [Verfasser], and Thomas [Akademischer Betreuer] Schenk. "To what extent are functions of the perceptual and the motor system separable? : an investigation using dual-tasking, the Garner paradigm, and the visual threshold of size discrimination / Frederic Göhringer ; Betreuer: Thomas Schenk." München : Universitätsbibliothek der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, 2020. http://d-nb.info/122827066X/34.
Full textAraújo, Lucas Delfino. "Transferência de Função no Controle de Respostas Verbais e Perceptuais: Uma Questão de Procedimento." Pontifícia Universidade Católica de Goiás, 2015. http://localhost:8080/tede/handle/tede/1903.
Full textThe aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of different procedures on the transfer function between perceptual and verbal stimuli. Three experiments were conducted with 10 participants each, of both sex and university. In Experiment 1 was performed a conditional discrimination procedure using Matching-to-Smple having a stimulus as a model and three as a comparison. A set A was composed of three perceptual stimuli (pictures), a joint B consisted of three words without meaning and set C consisted of three words with meaning. Training was made between set B and set C then set A to set B (Group 1) and inverse training (group 2). The Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1, but comparisons were made gradually over the first comparison blocks to complete 3 comparisons and all followed the Group s training sequence 1 Experiment 1. In Experiment 3 was made a training with presentation of stimuli verbal (sentences) and a conditional discrimination training between meaningless words and words with meaning. Data from the three procedures indicate that there is transfer function between words with meaning, figures and meaningless words, and 40% of the participants in Experiment 1 were obtained transfer function for the three meaningless words, 80% of the participant in Experiment 2 achieved transfer function for the three meaningless words and 100% of the participants in Experiment 3 obtained transfer function for the three meaningless words.
O objetivo do presente trabalho foi verificar o efeito de diferentes procedimentos sobre a transferência de função entre estímulos perceptuais e verbais. Foram realizados três experimentos contendo 10 participantes cada, de ambos os sexos e universitários. No Experimento 1 foi realizado um procedimento de discriminação condicional utilizando Matching-to-Smple tendo um estímulo como modelo e três como comparação. Um conjunto A compunha-se de três estímulos perceptuais (figuras), um Conjunto B compunha-se de três palavras sem sentido e um Conjunto C compunha-se de três palavras com sentido. Foi feito treino entre Conjunto B e Conjunto C e depois de Conjunto A com Conjunto B (Grupo 1) e treino inverso (Grupo 2). No Experimento 2 foi semelhante ao Experimento 1, mas as comparações eram apresentadas gradualmente durante os blocos de 1 comparação até completar 3 comparações e todos seguiram a sequência de treino do Grupo 1 do Experimento 1. No Experimento 3 foi feito um treino com apresentação de estímulos verbais (sentenças) e um treino de discriminação condicional entre palavras sem sentido e palavras com sentido. Os dados dos três procedimentos indicam que há transferência de função entre palavras com sentido, figuras e palavras sem sentido, sendo que 40% dos participantes do Experimento 1 obtiveram transferência função para as três palavras sem sentido, 80% dos participantes do Experimento 2 obtiveram transferência de função para as três palavras sem sentido e 100% dos participantes do Experimento 3 obtiveram transferência de função para as três palavras sem sentido.
Ghosh, Natasha. "Discrimination of perceptually similar natural categories by pigeons." Thesis, University of Exeter, 2002. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.392783.
Full textFagelson, Marc A., and C. A. Champlin. "Auditory Filters Measured at Neighboring Center Frequencies." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 1997. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/1581.
Full textHadley, Hillary R. "Does Early Perceptual Experience Influence Later Perceptual and Neural Discrimination in Children?" 2013. https://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses/1010.
Full textSezikeye, François Xavier. "Mechanisms of texture discrimination : what is behind the perceptual performance asymmetry?" Thesis, 2005. http://spectrum.library.concordia.ca/8332/1/MR04323.pdf.
Full textHagen, Simen. "The influence of real-world object expertise on visual discrimination mechanisms." Thesis, 2017. https://dspace.library.uvic.ca//handle/1828/8942.
Full textGraduate
2018-12-12
OuYang, Kuo-An, and 歐陽國安. "A Study on Effect of Graphic User Interface designs on Discrimination Ability-Applying Perceptual Grouping Theory." Thesis, 2006. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/65569858775780646129.
Full text義守大學
資訊管理學系碩士班
94
A Study on the Effect of Graphic User Interface designs on Discrimination Ability –Applying Perceptual Grouping Theory Abstract Graphic user interface (GUI ) for information system have become the mainst- ream of interface design in recent years. With the application of GUI getting more and more wide and the screen more and more complicated, users tend to have uneasy or tired feelings after long time use and the ability of discrimination decrease. This resea- rch explores, by using rules of Perceptual Grouping Theory, the way of screen designs that helps users to efficiently recognize the targets on GUI. The experiments designed in this research are based on the grouping rules such as “Law of Similarity”, “Law of Proximity”, “Law of Closed Forms” and “Law of Figure and Ground”. The response times of searching target are recorded as measure of discrimination ability for further analysis. We adopt statistical analysis and hypothesis testing on the experimental results and propose the following conclusions: 1) The colors, forms, sizes and positions of the interface component significantly influence discrimination ability; 2) The distances between components significantly influence discrimination ability; 3) Using the techniques of grouping or splitting on the components such as GroupBox, RadioGroup and Panel can promote the ability of discrimination significantly. Key word:Graphic user interface , Perceptual Grouping Theory, discrimination.
Reed, Amanda Christine. "Plasticity in the auditory cortex and changes in perceptual discrimination after nucleus basalis stimulation in rats /." 2008. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1597612641&sid=6&Fmt=2&clientId=10361&RQT=309&VName=PQD.
Full textAdhikari, Bhim M. "Oscillatory Network Activity in Brain Functions and Dysfunctions." 2014. http://scholarworks.gsu.edu/phy_astr_diss/67.
Full textKlapwijk, Jonathan Menno. "A validation of the Visual Perceptual Aspects Test using a bifactor exploratory structural equation modelling approach." Diss., 2018. http://hdl.handle.net/10500/25693.
Full textPsychology
M.A. (Research Psychology)
Vavassis, Angela. "Perceptual learning specificity to the trained context of stimulus display durations in difficult visual discriminations." Thesis, 2007. http://spectrum.library.concordia.ca/975571/1/MR34654.pdf.
Full textCarland, Matthew A. "A theoretical and experimental dissociation of two models of decision‐making." Thèse, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/1866/12038.
Full textDecision‐making is a computational process of fundamental importance to many aspects of animal behavior. The prevailing model in the experimental study of decision‐making is the drift‐diffusion model, which has a long history and accounts for a broad range of behavioral and neurophysiological data. However, an alternative model – called the urgency‐gating model – has been offered which can account equally well for much of the same data in a more parsimonious and theoretically‐sound manner. In what follows, we will first trace the origins and development of the DDM, as well as give a brief overview of the manner in which it has supplied an explanatory framework for a large number of behavioral and physiological studies in the domain of decision‐making. In so doing, we will attempt to build a strong and clear case for its strengths so that it can be fairly and rigorously compared to potential alternative models. We will then re‐examine a number of the implicit and explicit theoretical assumptions made by the drift‐diffusion model, as well as highlight some of its empirical shortcomings. This analysis will serve as the contextual backdrop for our introduction and discussion of the urgency‐gating model. Finally, we present a novel experiment, the methodological design of which uniquely affords a decisive empirical dissociation of the models, the results of which illustrate the empirical and theoretical shortcomings of the drift‐diffusion model and instead offer clear support for the urgency‐gating model. We finish by discussing the potential for the urgency gating model to shed light on a number of clinical disorders, highlighting a number of future directions for research.