Academic literature on the topic 'Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson"
Barreto, J., and J. Wendland. "Image du corps, vécu corporel et bonding chez les femmes dans les six mois suivant l’accouchement." Périnatalité 14, no. 1 (March 2022): 27–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.3166/rmp-2022-0145.
Full textSpooner, Diane, Marcelle Ricard, and Jean-Francois Saucier. "L'interaction mère-enfant et la genèse du soi chez le nourrisson*." Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 37, no. 6 (August 1992): 406–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/070674379203700610.
Full textSalomonsson, Björn. "L’évitement du regard dans la psychothérapie parents-bébé : manifestations et suggestions techniques." L’Année psychanalytique internationale Volume 2023, no. 1 (October 6, 2023): 21–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/lapsy.231.0021.
Full textRiquet, S., E. Hinsinger, E. Bernard, and C. Zakarian. "Évaluation du stress en post-partum des mères lors de la consultation postnatale." Périnatalité 11, no. 3 (September 2019): 126–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.3166/rmp-2019-0058.
Full textTordo, Frédéric. "Sensations, perceptions, images psychiques et représentations chez l’homme connecté." psychologie clinique, no. 49 (2020): 101–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1051/psyc/202049101.
Full textVinolo, Stéphane. "DECONSTRUCTION DE LA PERCEPTION – VOIR ET ECRIRE DANS LA PHILOSOPHIE DE DESCARTES." ENDOXA, no. 48 (December 20, 2021): 47–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.5944/endoxa.48.2021.25621.
Full textSauvagnargues, Anne. "Le sujet cinématographique, de l’arc sensorimoteur à la voyance." Cinémas 16, no. 2-3 (March 22, 2007): 96–114. http://dx.doi.org/10.7202/014617ar.
Full textDorvillé, Christian. "Représentations de l'enseignant d'E.P.S. par les parents d'élèves et les professeurs." STAPS 12, no. 24 (1991): 7–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/staps.1991.1204.
Full textPérès, Marie. "De la modélisation à l’image virtuelle : images et réel." Figures de l'Art. Revue d'études esthétiques 11, no. 1 (2006): 197–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/fdart.2006.1412.
Full textChamberland, Estelle. "Les difficultés d'apprentissage chez l'adulte: la valeur heuristique des représentations symboliques." Canadian Journal for the Study of Adult Education 12, no. 1 (May 1, 1998): 47–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.56105/cjsae.v12i1.2016.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson"
Spriet, Céline. "The development of visual object categorization." Electronic Thesis or Diss., Lyon 1, 2024. http://www.theses.fr/2024LYO10231.
Full textVisual object categorization is at the interface between "seeing" and "thinking". Evidences from functional MRI (fMRI) studies have described an organization of the ventral stream by visual categories, especially between animate and inanimate entities, that decomposed into finer-grained distinctions. How does this specific organization come about in humans? This thesis presents a set of 4 studies addressing this issue. First, I will hypothesize that the first categories infants represent are constrained by these dimensions organizing object representation in the visual cortex (chapter 3) and study the role of brain maturation and experience in this representation (chapter 4). Then, I will investigate how the speed of presentation influences the animate/inanimate categorization in the first year of life and in adulthood (chapter 5), and what visual features act in this categorization in adults (chapter 6). Results show that infants will first be attracted by non-categorical visual features such as the size of stimuli, before completely relying on categorical features, representing first the animate and inanimate entities. This transition is essentially limited by the brain maturation. This first big categorization gets faster and faster with age, and can be based on low-level visual features, although the more features available, the better the categorization. I suggest that the brain maturation help infants to represent more and more visual features when growing up, allowing them to represent more (finer-grained) categories. This maturation also elicit an acceleration of the representation of the big animate/inanimate categorization with age. Part of this big categorization is actually already possible based on lower-level visual features that covariate with the categories, but adults’ behavior gets influenced by categories only when enough features are presented in the image
Streri, Arlette. "Voir, atteindre, toucher : les relations entre la vision et le toucher chez le bébé." Paris 5, 1989. http://www.theses.fr/1989PA05H036.
Full textThe purpose of this thesis is to study the origins of the relations between perceptual systems such as touch and vision, and the development of their relations during the first year of life. The problem of the unity of senses at birth is old. This question has been answered to in two different ways : prehension of visual object and intermodal transfer or multimodal exploration. Prehension is a relation between the visual sense and the tactual motor system whereas intermodal transfer and multimodal exploration is a relation between senses. The two fields were studied separately until now and it became necessary to bring them together in order to have a complete view of the different relations between perceptual systems which organize our behaviour from birth. A complete analysis of the litterature in the two fields and our own experiments on intermodal transfer may suggest an early unity of the systems at birth. However, this unity is fragile in prehension and a reorganization of behaviour is observed which may be attributed to the development of the tactual motor system. The unity is established between sensory modes but the relations between vision and touch are not reversible at each age. This lack of reversibility may be attributed to the different speeds of the development of the two systems. We ask the question of the stages of cognition that the infants construct in the first year from perceptual and motor organization
Millêtre, Béatrice. "Habituation visuelle et traitement de l'information chez le nourrisson de 3 et 5 mois." Paris 5, 1991. http://www.theses.fr/1991PA05H063.
Full textVisual habituation is often considered as linked to information processing, but very few studies have emphasized the relations. Our work has tried to define the relations between visual habituation and information processing. At the end of our experiments, it clearly appears that informaiton processing is achieved before habituation is reached. Habituation can then be considered as involving two successive operations, the first during which information is encoded and a representation made, the second one during which the perceptive imput is compared with the preceeding mnemonic trace, and information recognized as the same. Habituation shows the sam two operations at 3 and 5 months
Rovira, Katia. "L'organisation perceptive et ses limites chez le bébé de 4 mois : capacités de discrimination et de catégorisation." Paris 5, 1994. http://www.theses.fr/1994PA05H081.
Full textThis work is dedicated to the study of the early perceptual organization. Following the theoretical presentation of the developmental approaches explaining the emergence of this process, we tested the sensibility of 4-month-old infants to the spatial relationships between elements belonging to a configuration. For this purpose, we used different configurations and their modifications in various discrimination and categorization situations. In a first experimental step, the configurations were made of 6 elements arranged around a vertical or horizontal virtual central axis. The modification was either a partial (2 elements) or complete (all elements) permutation around the central axis. In the discrimination task, babies had significant novelty reaction in the partial permutation condition and a preference for the familiar situation in the total permutation condition. The categorization activity was rarely observed. In a second experimental step, we used more or less regular configurations of 8 elements. The modification here consists in suppressing one element. In discrimination, the results showed both an effect of the fugure regularity level and an effect of the position of the suppressed element. We did not observe a categorization activity. This experimental work evidenced the early perceptual organization and its limitations in 4-monthold infants. The main result is that some relations are more easily perceived than others and the important change rate of stimulations in the categorization situation limits early perceptual organization
Ngon, Céline. "Issues in early phonological and lexical acquisition." Paris 6, 2013. http://www.theses.fr/2013PA066348.
Full textOver the last four decades, a growing body of research has been dedicated to the study of the cognitive mechanisms allowing infants to acquire their native language with remarkable ease. The present dissertation reports the findings of three experimental studies addressing crucial issues in phonological and lexical acquisition at different stages of development. In a first study, we explored the types of mechanisms driving the acquisition of a sound inventory. During the second half of the first year of life, infants’ phonetic perception is refined according to the native language structure, with speech sound discrimination declining for non-native contrasts and improving for native contrasts. However, a question that has remained unanswered is how to account for the order in which the different native categories are acquired. In particular, two hypotheses have been proposed: (1) a frequency-based hypothesis, according to which exemplars of sound categories occurring the most frequently in the input speech should be acquired earlier than less frequent categories; (2) a universal markedness hypothesis, according to which less marked sounds should acquire before more marked ones. To disentangle these two possibilities, we designed a cross-language developmental study, in which the discrimination of two non-native contrasts was tested across two languages at two ages, and in which the two hypotheses predict different developmental scenarios for the two languages. So far, the pattern of results seems to converge on the universal markedness account (though more robust data is needed to validate this hypothesis). However, we also argue that an alternative frequency-based explanation can account for our results. In a second study, we investigated the acquisition of a receptive lexicon. To detect word-forms in the speech stream, it has been suggested that infants rely on statistical coherence between syllables, a universal, language-independent cue that would help them get started with a first set of forms. While it is well-known that infants are sensitive to statistical cues, we provide unique evidence confirming that infants really make use of this available resource to build a lexicon. In particular, we exploited the fact that a purely statistical learning strategy should extract words but also high-frequency sound sequences that do not correspond to actual words. Infants’ receptive lexicon was simulated using a crude algorithm that extracts frequent disyllabic sequences from a corpus of French infant-directed speech, and recognition of such sequences was tested in a series of preferential listening experiments. French-learning infants of 11 months, an age at which their word segmentation capacities are still rudimentary, were found to recognize isolated high-frequency nonword sequences (e. G. Va faire, n’as plus) and fail to differentiate between these nonwords and actual words in the same frequency range (e. G. Canard, “duck”; ballon, “ball”). These results show that infants do apply statistical cues, guiding them to build a “protolexicon”, containing both words and nonwords, and which will later be pruned as robust segmentation abilities develop. In a third study, we examined the output phonological representations of words in French-learning infants of 21 months, an age at which they comprehend many words but often misarticulate them or do not attempt to produce them at all. We showed that they are able to covertly produce the phonological form of words that they do not yet produce overtly, and make internal (though coarse) judgments about their phonological length. In an anticipatory eye-movement procedure, infants were presented with images of objects whose labels they had to covertly produce and categorize according to their length. Crucially, the images represented objects whose labels were understood but not yet overtly produced by the infants, according to parental report. Successful categorization was measured by correct anticipations of the appearance of each object to a designated side on a screen (left or right), which was determined by the length of the object’s label. Infants’ performance was significantly above chance when words were monosyllabic and trisyllabic (e. G. Chat vs. Pantalon), and marginally so with words of a smaller phonological difference, i. E. Monosyllables vs. Disyllables (e. G. Chat vs. Ballon). These observations constitute unique evidence that infants possess output phonological representations for words before they start producing them. Together, these findings offer new insights into infants’ phonological and lexical development, from the perceptual abilities allowing them to recognize the sounds and word-forms of their language to the representations of words in the output lexicon. We discuss the interpretation of our experimental results and propose avenues for future research to answer new questions raised by our findings
Grondin, Pierre. "La perception des objets impliqués dans des relations causales et non causales chez les enfants de 6 mois." Master's thesis, Université Laval, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11794/43100.
Full textPascalis, Olivier. "La mémoire à long terme chez le nourrisson de 4 jours à 6 mois." Aix-Marseille 1, 1993. http://www.theses.fr/1993AIX11053.
Full textCarchon, Isabelle. "Des relations à la coordination entre l'eil et la tête chez le nourrisson." Paris 5, 1994. http://www.theses.fr/1994PA05H057.
Full textTurati, Chiara. "Il riconoscimento del volto nei primi mesi di vita : l'emergere di un sistema cognitivo specializzato." Paris 5, 2004. http://www.theses.fr/2004PA05H068.
Full textBassot, Fabienne. "Les composantes sensori-motrices de l'activité visuelle chez le bébé prématuré." Paris 5, 1995. http://www.theses.fr/1995PA05H031.
Full textIt is suggested that the association of visual and cephalic systems may predict the eye-head coordination. No research has explored preterm's visuo-cephalic activity. The focus of this study was preterm's visuo-cephalic activity in two tasks : a pursuit and a peripheral detection task. 51 preterms infants, born at 32 weeks, were observed at 37 weeks in a semi vertical position. Four groups of preterms were observed either in one or both conditions : "head fixed" and "head free". Results show that preterm can follow a target moving horizontally at eye level and at a 30 cm distance. The pursuit covers 20 to 25 of the target's total course. Evenmore peripheral detection is observed when the target appears within a 30 angle. Eye displacements were predominantly saccadic in both tasks. Comparison of visual activity between conditions show that preterms perform better in "head fixed" than in "head free" condition. In the latter, two forms of pursuit are observed : a response of ocular pursuit and a response of ocular pursuit accompagnied by a cephalic movement oriented in the same direction. This "ocular-cephalic" pattern of response is characterized by an ocular localization followed by an accelerated cephalic movement
Books on the topic "Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson"
Vauclair, Jacques. Développement du jeune enfant: Motricité, perception, cognition. Paris: Belin, 2004.
Find full textLécuyer, Roger. Bébés astronomes, bébés psychologues: L'intelligence de la première année. Liege: Mardaga, 1989.
Find full textRoss, Flom, Lee Kang, and Muir Darwin, eds. Gaze-following: Its development and significance. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2007.
Find full textCarl, Granrud, and Symposium on Cognition, (23rd : 1990 : Pittsburgh), eds. Visual perception and cognition in infancy. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates, 1992.
Find full textCarl, Granrud, and Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (23rd : 1990 : Pittsburgh, Pa.), eds. Visual perception and cognition in infancy. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum, 1993.
Find full textYvette, Hatwell, Streri Arlette, and Gentaz Édouard, eds. Toucher pour connaître: Psychologie cognitive de la perception tactile manuelle. Paris: Presses universitaires de France, 2000.
Find full textFrancesca, Simion, and Butterworth George, eds. The Development of sensory, motor, and cognitive capacities in early infancy: From perception to cognition. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press, 1998.
Find full text1960-, Fagot Joël, ed. Picture perception in animals. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press, 2000.
Find full textAlbert, Yonas, University of Minnesota. Institute of Child Development., and Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (20th : 1985 : University of Minnesota), eds. Perceptual development in infancy. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates, 1988.
Find full textJ, Lewkowicz David, and Lickliter Robert, eds. The development of intersensory perception: Comparative perspectives. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum, 1994.
Find full textBook chapters on the topic "Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson"
Streri, Arlette. "Chapitre 3. Exploration manuelle et perception tactile chez le nourrisson." In Toucher pour connaître, 51–70. Presses Universitaires de France, 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/puf.strer.2000.01.0051.
Full textConference papers on the topic "Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson"
Talenti, Simona. "Visions “humaines” ou “infernales”: les moyens de transport et la perception de la ville chez Le Corbusier." In LC2015 - Le Corbusier, 50 years later. Valencia: Universitat Politècnica València, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.4995/lc2015.2015.821.
Full textReports on the topic "Perception des images – Chez le nourrisson"
Gruber, Verena, Ingrid Peignier, and Charlotte Dubuc. Pratiques et tactiques de vente des concessionnaires automobiles au Québec. CIRANO, October 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.54932/bryk4403.
Full text