To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Perception and Spatiality.

Journal articles on the topic 'Perception and Spatiality'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Perception and Spatiality.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Liddicoat, Stephanie. "Perceptions of Spatiality: Supramodal Meanings and Metaphors in Therapeutic Environments." Interiority 1, no. 2 (July 30, 2018): 91–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.7454/in.v1i2.17.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper explores the perceptions of the spatiality of individuals who self-harm, with the aim of understanding the design aspects which foster supportive therapeutic environments. Analysis of responses found that there were key similarities in areas of perception of architectural interior space, refuting the commonly held view that all architectural response is purely subjective, and that subjective experience cannot be shared. Three examples of perceptions of interior therapeutic environments are discussed to highlight how the perceptions of spatiality of individuals who self-harm consists of a particular cluster of spatial understandings, behaviours and focuses, manifesting as a strong emotional overtone overlaid onto built environments. This includes common kinds of triggers and emotional reactions provoked by aspects of the built environment. This paper discusses architectural aspects in relation to subjectivity in perception, constructs of interiority, and the role of supramodal engagement in influencing perceptual responses to interior space. By understanding how individuals who self-harm experience a space, a greater comprehension of the design of these environments delivering mental health services may be enabled. This paper tables a series of research-derived design suggestions to facilitate supportive therapeutic spaces. This paper also proposes a series of further research directions to explore the relationships between constructs of interiority, the physical interior space, and the therapeutic function for which they are designed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Baaner, Lasse, and Line Hvingel. "Spatiality of Environmental Law." Journal for European Environmental & Planning Law 12, no. 2 (May 28, 2015): 173–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/18760104-01202005.

Full text
Abstract:
Digital society challenges the traditional perception of legal sources. The use of maps as a basis for public administration dates far back, but e-Government’s use of digital maps that include legal information creates new legal obstacles. In the coming decades, the inspire directive of 2007 will determine the interplay between geographic data and technology in the fields of environmental legislation, environmental policy and environmental management. This article examines the legal regulation of spatial information as established by the inspire directive, on one hand, and on the other hand, examines legal regulation as spatial information. It aims to deepen the understanding of spatiality as a core element of environmental law, and to connect it to the basic concept of representation used in giscience. It concludes that the future path for e-Government demands a shift in legal paradigm, from maps showing representations of applied legal norms, to maps build on datasets that have legal authority. That will integrate legal and geographic information systems, and improve the legal accountability of decision support systems used in e-Government services based on spatio-legal data.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Viik, Tõnu. "ŽMOGIŠKOJI ERDVĖ: KULTŪRINĖ KRAŠTOVAIZDŽIŲ IR VIETOVIŲ FENOMENOLOGIJA." Problemos 79 (January 1, 2011): 103–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/problemos.2011.0.1323.

Full text
Abstract:
Straipsnyje fenomenologiniu metodu analizuojamas kraštovaizdžio ir kitų erdvinių struktūrų suvokimas. Erdvinė struktūra suprantama kaip erdvės sritis ar teritorija, turinti specifinę prasmę, kurią patiria stebintis subjektas. Aiškinant, kaip erdvinė struktūra įgauna prasmę, kuri ją apibrėžia kaip kraštovaizdį, namus ar šalį, remiamasi Husserlio prasmės konstitucijos teorija. Teigiama, kad, be subjekto pozicijos ir objektų suvokimų sekos, konkretaus erdvinio darinio prasmę lemiantis elementas yra tai, ką Husserlis vadino „suvokimo prasme“ (Auffassungssinn). Ji apibrėžia konkrečiam erdviškumui būdingą žiūrą. Kai tokia „suvokimo prasmė“ tampa intersubjektyviai galiojanti ir institucionalizuota, ji įgyja statusą kultūrinės formos, kuri funkcionuoja kaip visoms visuomenėms prasmę perteikiantis pasaulio interpretavimo automatas. Galiausiai tvirtinama, kad žmogiškųjų aplinkinių pasaulių (Umwelten) erdviškumas leidžia kurti ir palaikyti prasmes, kurios antraip išnyktų laiko tėkmėje.Pagrindiniai žodžiai: kraštovaizdžio suvokimas, erdvinės struktūros, fenomenologinis metodas, suvokimo prasmė, kultūrinė forma.Human Spatiality: A Cultural Phenomenology of Landscapes and PlacesTõnu Viik SummaryThe paper applies phenomenological method to the analysis of perception of landscapes and other spatialformations. A spatial formation is seen as a region of space, or a territory, with its specific meaning that is experienced by the subject who views it. Husserl’s theory of meaning-formation is used to clarify how spatial formations obtain meanings that define them as landscape, home, or country. It is suggestedthat besides the subject’s position and the series of perceptions of objects, the decisive element that determines the meaning of the specific spatial formation is what Husserl calls a “grasping sense” (Auffassungssinn).It defines the gaze specific to a particular spatiality. When the “grasping sense” becomes intersubjectively valid and institutionalized, it obtains the status of a cultural form which functions as a meaning-bestowing automaton for interpreting the world for entire societies. Finally it is argued that the spatiality specific to human Umwelten serves the purpose of creating and maintaining meanings that would otherwise disappear in the flux of time.Keywords: perception of landscapes, spatial formations, phenomenological method, grasping sense, cultural form.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Burgese, D. F., D. P. Bassitt, D. Ceron-Litvoc, and G. B. Liberali. "The Time Perception in Contemporary." European Psychiatry 41, S1 (April 2017): S717. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.1288.

Full text
Abstract:
With the advent of new technologies, the man begins to experience a significant change in the perception of the other, time and space. The acceleration of time promoted by new technology does not allow the exercise of affection for the consolidation of ties, relations take narcissists hues seeking immediate gratification and the other is understood as a continuation of the self, the pursuit of pleasure. It is the acceleration of time, again, which leads man to present the need for immediate, always looking for the new – not new – in an attempt to fill an inner space that is emptied. The retention of concepts and pre-stressing of temporality are liquefied, become fleeting. We learn to live in the world and the relationship with the other in a frivolous and superficial way. The psychic structure, facing new phenomena experienced, loses temporalize capacity and expand its spatiality, it becomes pathological. Post-modern inability to retain the past, to analyze the information received and reflect, is one of the responsible for the mental illness of today's society. From a temporality range of proper functioning, the relationship processes with you and your peers will have the necessary support to become viable and healthy.Disclosure of interestThe authors have not supplied their declaration of competing interest.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Mikułowski Pomorski, Jerzy. "Muzeum: transformacje medium na tle procesów konwergencji." Kultura i Społeczeństwo 55, no. 4 (November 22, 2011): 129–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.35757/kis.2011.55.4.7.

Full text
Abstract:
The Author ponders changes in the perception of the institution of museum, which he treats as a medium. He considers its constitutive features: spatiality and transfer function. In his opinion, in the contemporary world of cultural convergence the museums, once limited with institutional perspective, targeted their attention at the needs and experiences of a viewer — they have become a piece of land which may be developed by people according to their own group needs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Lin, Chien Yu, Pi Hsia Hung, Yen Huai Jen, and Chien Chi Lin. "Perception of Motion Trajectory as a Spatial Concepts Activity for Children." Advanced Materials Research 187 (February 2011): 332–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amr.187.332.

Full text
Abstract:
This study proposes to develop a best treatment interface of motion trajectory as a construct for children to build up their sense of spatiality. There are 30 children from kindergarten participate in this research. There are two factors in the study, first is the “leading object” and the other is the “reference object”, and each factor contains two levels, hence, there are four treatments in this experiment. This research uses “maze” as a spatial sense conceptual tool, and the maze is constructed by 3D models as a virtual environment. The focus of this research is to find out the best treatment and does not cause cognitive burden on children in the meantime. The result of this research is that the best tr eatment of motion trajectory depends on the treatment of “leading object” with “reference object” for children when they grab spatial concepts.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Keinert, Alexa, Volkan Sayman, and Daniel Maier. "Relational Communication Spaces: Infrastructures and Discursive Practices." Media and Communication 9, no. 3 (July 23, 2021): 28–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.17645/mac.v9i3.3988.

Full text
Abstract:
Digital communication technologies, social web platforms, and mobile communication have fundamentally altered the way we communicate publicly. They have also changed our perception of space, thus making a re-calibration of a spatial perspective on public communication necessary. We argue that such a new perspective must consider the relational logic of public communication, which stands in stark contrast to the plain territorial notion of space common in communication research. Conceptualising the spatiality of public communication, we draw on Löw’s (2016) sociology of space. Her relational concept of space encourages us to pay more attention to (a) the infrastructural basis of communication, (b) the operations of synthesising the relational communication space through discursive practices, and (c) power relations that determine the accessibility of public communication. Thus, focusing on infrastructures and discursive practices means highlighting crucial socio-material preconditions of public communication and considering the effects of the power relations which are inherent in their spatialisation upon the inclusivity of public communication<em>.</em> This new approach serves a dual purpose: Firstly, it works as an analytical perspective to systematically account for the spatiality of public communication. Secondly, the differentiation between infrastructural spaces and spaces of discursive practices adds explanatory value to the perspective of relational communication spaces.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Findlay-Walsh, Iain. "Virtual auditory reality." SoundEffects - An Interdisciplinary Journal of Sound and Sound Experience 10, no. 1 (January 15, 2021): 71–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.7146/se.v10i1.124199.

Full text
Abstract:
This article examines popular music listening in light of recent research in auditory perception and spatial experience, record production, and virtual reality, while considering parallel developments in digital pop music production practice. The discussion begins by considering theories of listening and embodiment by Brandon LaBelle, Eric Clarke, Salomè Voegelin and Linda Salter, examining relations between listening subjects and aural environments, conceptualising listening as a process of environmental ‘inhabiting’, and considering auditory experience as the real-time construction of ‘reality’. These ideas are discussed in relation to recent research on popular music production and perception, with a focus on matters of spatial sound design, the virtual ‘staging’ of music performances and performing bodies, digital editing methods and effects, and on shifting relations between musical spatiality, singer-persona, audio technologies, and listener. Writings on music and virtual space by Martin Knakkergaard, Allan Moore, Ragnhild Brøvig-Hanssen & Anne Danielsen, Denis Smalley, Dale Chapman, Kodwo Eshun and Holger Schulze are discussed, before being related to conceptions of VR sound and user experience by Jaron Lanier, Rolf Nordahl & Niels Nilsson, Mel Slater, Tom Garner and Frances Dyson. This critical framework informs three short aural analyses of digital pop tracks released during the last 10 years - Titanium (Guetta & Sia 2010), Ultralight Beam (West 2016) and 2099 (Charli XCX 2019) - presented in the form of autoethnographic ‘listening notes’. Through this discussion on personal popular music listening and virtual spatiality, a theory of pop listening as embodied inhabiting of simulated narrative space, or virtual story-world, with reference to ‘aural-dominant realities’ (Salter), ‘sonic possible worlds’ (Voegelin), and ‘sonic fictions’ (Eshun), is developed. By examining personal music listening in relation to VR user experience, this study proposes listening to pop music in the 21st century as a mode of immersive, embodied ‘storyliving’, or ‘storydoing’ (Allen & Tucker).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lindborg, PerMagnus. "Interactive Sonification of Weather Data for The Locust Wrath, a Multimedia Dance Performance." Leonardo 51, no. 5 (October 2018): 466–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/leon_a_01339.

Full text
Abstract:
To work flexibly with the sound design for The Locust Wrath, a multimedia dance performance on the topic of climate change, the author developed software for interactive sonification of climate data. An open-ended approach to parameter mapping allowed tweaking and improvisation during rehearsals, resulting in a large range of musical expression. The sonifications represented weather systems pushing through Southeast Asia in complex patterns. The climate was rendered as a piece of electroacoustic music, whose compositional form—gesture, timbre, intensity, harmony, spatiality—was determined by the data. The article discusses aspects of aesthetic sonification, reports the process of developing the present work and contextualizes the design decisions within theories of cross-modal perception and listening modes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Gehmann, Ulrich, and Martin Reiche. "Advanced Spatialities." International Journal of Art, Culture and Design Technologies 3, no. 1 (January 2013): 48–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijacdt.2013010105.

Full text
Abstract:
In this article the authors are going to explore a fundamental problem of modern spaces, and modern spatiality in general: their virtualization and final annihilation by augmenting them. There are two major domains where this happened and still happens: inside real space, and inside the virtual spaces of so-called location-based games. In both cases of its real and virtual appearance, space becomes efficient and therefore loses its uniqueness and identity, with concomitant effects on the user’s very own perception of reality. The authors will concentrate upon the case of gaming; here, augmentation re-shapes the perception of the real object in space (which is not originally part of the game) by making it an active element of the game, i.e. it utilizes the object (and furthermore the surrounding space) and thus frees it of its original meaning and utility. Furthermore, it gets incorporated into the artificial (virtual) space and acquires two new properties: it becomes interactive and as a result, interchangeable. The perception of reality thus gets augmented at the same time as it gets reduced to the bare minimum of information needed to reach the goal of the game. The authors will be providing a set of rules to address these phenomena in a generic manner.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Kramer, Caroline, and Madeleine Wagner. "Enhancing Urban Sustainable Indicators in a German City—Towards Human-Centered Measurements for Sustainable Urban Planning." World 1, no. 2 (August 10, 2020): 104–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/world1020009.

Full text
Abstract:
This contribution demonstrates how more human-centered measurements for sustainable urban planning can be created by enlarging the traditional set of urban sustainability indicators. In many municipal reports, sustainable indicators concentrate on environmental issues, by collecting data at an aggregated spatial and temporal level using quantitative methods. Our approach aims to expand and improve the currently dominant quantitative–statistical methods by including perception geographical data (subjective indicators following the social indicator approach), namely additional indicators at spatial and temporal levels. Including small-scale city district levels and a temporal differentiation produces more process assessments and a better representation of everyday life. Based on a survey we conducted at district levels in the city of Karlsruhe, we cover three sustainability dimensions (ecological, social, economic) and analyze (1) how citizens are mobile in a sustainable way (bike use) and (2) how they perceive and react to heat events in the city. We argue for taking people’s perception and the spatiality and temporality of their daily activities better into account when further developing urban sustainability indicators and when aiming for a sustainable, human-centered urban development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Rajendran, Lakshmi, Fariba Molki, Sara Mahdizadeh, and Asma Mehan. "(RE)FRAMING SPATIALITY AS A SOCIO-CULTURAL PARADIGM: EXAMINING THE IRANIAN HOUSING CULTURE AND PROCESSES." JOURNAL OF ARCHITECTURE AND URBANISM 45, no. 1 (June 14, 2021): 95–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.3846/jau.2021.14032.

Full text
Abstract:
With rapid changes in urban living today, peoples’ behavioural patterns and spatial practices undergo a constant process of adaptation and negotiation. Using “house” as a laboratory and everyday life and spatial relations of residents as a framework of analysis, the paper examines the spatial planning concepts in traditional and contemporary Iranian architecture and the associated socio-cultural practices. Discussions are drawn upon from a pilot study conducted in the city of Kerman, to investigate ways in which contemporary housing solutions can better cater to the continually changing socio-cultural lifestyles of residents. Data collection for the study involved a series of participatory workshops and employed creative visual research methods, participant observation and semi structured interviews to examine the interlacing of everyday socio-spatial relations and changing perception of identity, belonging, socio-cultural and religious values and conflict. The inferences from the study showcases the emerging social and cultural needs and practices of people manifested through the complex relationship between residents, the places in which they live, and its spatial planning and organisation. For a better understanding of this complex relationship, the paper argues the need for resituating spatiality as a socio-cultural paradigm.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Paraguai, Luisa. "Mobile Devices." International Journal of Advanced Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing 2, no. 1 (January 2010): 39–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/japuc.2010010103.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper is concerned with mobile technology and its interventions on the perception of the body and the space, demanding new behavioural codes and evoking other communication patterns. This technology enables users to be always connected, creating other practices of sociability and composing the urban landscape and the body space with digital contexts. Thus, the space occupied by mobile users is no longer physical or virtual, but hybrid. Hybrid spaces are introduced, and theoretical references configure the idea that mobile technology determines specific modes of interaction, emphasising a ritual dimension. Mobile users have started to perform the same body gestures and bounded intimacies in a social context that configure a specific new bodily spatiality. The authors present artistic projects that examine aspects of significant social mobile uses, transforming users bodily states and spatial domains.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Bhattacharya, Tithi. "Tracking the Goddess: Religion, Community, and Identity in the Durga Puja Ceremonies of Nineteenth-Century Calcutta." Journal of Asian Studies 66, no. 4 (October 29, 2007): 919–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0021911807001258.

Full text
Abstract:
Focusing on colonial Calcutta in the later decades of the nineteenth century, this essay explores the evolution of a particular festival, the Durga Puja, to explore the ways in which religion negotiated its place in the ideological determinants of modernity. The essay surveys the evolution of the goddess Durga from premodern times and shows why and how the perception of both the deity (in gender terms) and the festival (in historical terms) had to be recalibrated following the imperatives of new classes and new discursive parameters. While the essay interrogates the development of new particular categories introduced by modernity, such as urban spatiality and the rhetoric of individual rights in colonial Calcutta, it also aligns these developments to answer the general paradigmatic question of the actual relationship between religion/faith and the modern moment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Rosenberger, Robert. "A Preliminary Inventory of the Transformations of Scientific Imaging." Acta Baltica Historiae et Philosophiae Scientiarum 8, no. 2 (December 10, 2020): 21–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.11590/abhps.2020.2.02.

Full text
Abstract:
Imaging technologies “transform” an object of study into something we can visually perceive in the form of an image. In science and medicine, imaging technologies enact a large variety of transformations, sometimes changing the spatiality of an object of study (e.g., making a small thing big enough to see, bringing close something far away, etc.), or changing its temporality (e.g., providing a picture of a single moment). I make use of the postphenomenological philosophical perspective, and in particular the work of its founder, Don Ihde, for guidance in exploring the different ways that imaging technologies transform our world in the process of rendering it available to visual perception. The main project of this paper is to develop a provisional categorization of a large variety of image transformations common to science and medicine.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Alfirević, Đorđe, and Sanja Simonović-Alfirević. "Parameters of spatial comfort in architecture." Arhitektura i urbanizam, no. 51 (2020): 33–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.5937/a-u0-26940.

Full text
Abstract:
The perception of reality is experienced through the senses, where each sense contributes to the way we form our picture of the feeling of comfort. When it comes to the perception of space and spatiality, the most dominant are visual and tactile influences, providing the information that outbalance other senses. A widely accepted opinion in science is that there are several main categories of comfort - visual, thermic, auditory, olfactory and hygienic. In contrast to previously mentioned terms, spatial comfort has not been clearly defined, even though it is one of the key terms when discussing human needs and functionality of space in architecture. Along with being widely used in practice and the fact that a clear scientific determination of this term is still lacking, its use is understood as the equivalent of comfort of a certain space. Our paper analyzed and systematized most significant parameters that can be applied to enable achievement of spatial comfort, which does not necessarily mean that such a spatial comfort will in fact be achieved, as this depends largely on individual perception of a space user. Aiming to clarify the term spatial comfort and determine its precise definition, the main contribution of this paper is the analysis of parameters that can contribute to spatial comfort, as well as reexamination of the thesis that spatial comfort includes the feeling of coziness and content resulting from physical, visual and tactile qualities of a certain space.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Lotman, Elen. "Exploring the Ways Cinematography Affects Viewers’ Perceived Empathy towards Onscreen Characters." Baltic Screen Media Review 4, no. 1 (December 1, 2016): 88–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/bsmr-2017-0005.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In the history of cinematography there is a noticeable tradition to deliberately highlight the elements that accentuate space and spatiality in the shots. At the same time, there is also a contrary tradition, i.e. the conscious reduction of spatiality with the help of artistic tools in order to evoke a feeling of alienation. In this article I will argue that it is highly likely that the visual reinforcement of depth has become one of a cinematographer’s most frequently used tools, because it plays an important role in the audience’s perceived empathy towards onscreen characters. Since the practices of art-making – e.g. cinematography – represent a way that the empirical experience accumulated in professional practices reflects underlying neural processes, this article will first draw upon evidence from the common tenets of cinematography and reflect on how these correspond to the respective phenomena in human perception and cognition. The second part of the article examines the theory of the para-dramatic and eso-dramatic factors established by Gal Raz and Talma Hendler as it applies to cinematography; thereby suggesting possibilities for broadening the theoretical foundations of the twofold division of the causes for the viewers’ empathetic responses. The article will also introduce the results from a pilot experiment. However, I will not argue that the rendering of cinematographic space and drawing attention to certain areas are superior tools for creating filmic empathy. I will rather point out that they are often used by cinematographers when they want to create an immersive experience, and therefore, there is reason to believe that a connection exists between emotional empathy and the usage of these cinematographic tools.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Gardner, Sally. "Beyond anthropomorphism: Odissi and the botanical." Studies in South Asian Film & Media 4, no. 2 (October 1, 2012): 157–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1386/safm.4.2.157_1.

Full text
Abstract:
Drawing on experiences that have entailed watching and learning forms of so-called ‘Indian dance’ (Bharata Natyam and Odissi), and watching Odissi dancers performing in various locations in Orissa’s ‘sacred triangle’ (Puri, Konark, Bhubaneswar), and against my own background in contemporary dance, I propose that the difference of the Odissi body is that the dancer dances with his or her feet in more than one kingdom – that is, he or she maintains a link between human bodies and the bodies of plants. Such a perception can help to displace questions of the dancer’s spatiality and representations, challenging western or westernized visions of the industrial or mechanical body, assumed hierarchies of body parts and their signifying powers, and assumptions about the role of the joints. The sense of a botanical imaginary or specific cultural body-schema at work in Odissi dance is supported by discussion of historical and ethnographic literature pertaining to the (former) female dancers of the Jagannath Temple in Puri; the temple’s links with Oriyan tribal cultures; the dancers’ traditional importance according to an axis of social auspiciousness/inauspiciousness as opposed to social purity/impurity; and the particular processes of the reconstruction of Odissi dance (separate from that of Bharata natyam) after independence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Sengupta, Ulysses, Mahmud Tantoush, May Bassanino, and Eric Cheung. "The Hybrid Space of Collaborative Location-Based Mobile Games and the City: A Case Study of Ingress." Urban Planning 5, no. 4 (December 15, 2020): 358–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.17645/up.v5i4.3487.

Full text
Abstract:
Structural changes in the way we live and interact in cities are occurring due to advances in mobile communication technologies affecting everyday practices. One such practice, at the forefront of digital technology adoption, is digital gaming or play. Location-based mobile games (LBMGs), such as Pokémon Go and Ingress have surged in popularity in recent years through their introduction of a new mode of play, employing mobile GPS and internet-enabled technology. Distinguished by their embedded GIS, LBMGs can influence how people play, interact with and perceive the city, by merging urban and virtual spaces into ‘hybrid realities.’ Despite the popularity of such games, studies into how LBMGs affect urban dweller interactions with each other and the city have been limited. This article examines how the digital interface of the large-scale collaborative LBMG Ingress affects how players experience and use the city. Ingress is a collaborative hybrid or location-based game that uses GPS location information from smartphones, Google maps, and Google POI to create virtual gameplay environments that correspond to and interact with other players and the city. The methodology cross-references the MDA framework from game studies (Mechanics-Dynamics-Aesthetics) within the urban mobility, sociability and spatiality characteristics of the hybrid realities theoretical framework. In this article, we explore how Ingress (re)produces hybrid space through deliberate design of interface game elements. By applying this analytical approach, we identify the game mechanics and their role in producing a hybrid gameplay environment with impacts on social and mobility practices altering the perception of and engagement with the city.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Honderich, Ted. "Actual Consciousness: Database, Physicalities, Theory, Criteria, No Unique Mystery." Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 76 (May 2015): 271–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1358246115000077.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract(i) Is disagreement about consciousness largely owed to no adequate initial clarification of the subject, to people in fact answering different questions – despite five leading ideas of consciousness? (ii) Your being conscious in the primary ordinary sense, to sum up a wide figurative database, is initially clarified as something's being actual – clarified as actual consciousness. (iii) Philosophical method like the scientific method includes transition from the figurative to literal theory or analysis. (iv) A new theory will also satisfy various criteria not satisfied by many existing theories. (v) The objective physical world has specifiable general characteristics including spatiality, lawfulness, being in science, connections with perception, and so on. (vi) Actualism, the literal theory or analysis of actual consciousness, deriving mainly from the figurative database, is that actual consciousness has counterpart but partly different general characteristics. Actual consciousness is thus subjectively physical. So physicality in general consists in objective and also subjective physicality. (vii) Consciousness in the case of perception is only the dependent existence of a subjective external physical world out there, often a room. (viii) But cognitive and affective consciousness, various kinds of thinking and wanting, differently subjectively physical, is internal – subjectively physical representations-with-attitude, representations that also are actual. They differ from the representations that are lines of type, sounds etc. by being actual. (ix) Thus they involve a subjectivity or individuality that is a lawful unity. (x) Actualism, both an externalism and an internalism, does not impose on consciousness a flat uniformuity, and it uniquely satisfies the various criteria for an adequate theory, including naturalism. (xi) Actual consciousness is a right subject and is a necessary part of any inquiry whatever into consciousness. (xii) All of it is a subject for more science, a workplace. (xiii) There is no unique barrier or impediment whatever to science, as often said, no want of understanding of the mind-consciousness connection (Nagel), no known unique hard problem of consciousness (Chalmers), no insuperable difficulty having to do with physicality and the history of science (Chomsky), no arguable ground at all of mysterianism (McGinn).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Heron, James, Neil W. Roach, James V. M. Hanson, Paul V. McGraw, and David Whitaker. "Audiovisual time perception is spatially specific." Experimental Brain Research 218, no. 3 (February 25, 2012): 477–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3038-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Fairhall, Scott, Jens Schwarzbach, Martijn van Koningsbruggen, Angelika Lingnau, and David Melcher. "Spatially-Specific Repetition Suppression in Transsaccadic Perception." Journal of Vision 15, no. 12 (September 1, 2015): 603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/15.12.603.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Govorov, Michael, Giedrė Beconytė, Gennady Gienko, and Viktor Putrenko. "Spatially constrained regionalization with multilayer perceptron." Transactions in GIS 23, no. 5 (July 9, 2019): 1048–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/tgis.12557.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Humphreys, Glyn W., and Nick Donnelly. "3-D constraints on spatially parallel shape perception." Perception & Psychophysics 62, no. 5 (July 2000): 1060–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03212089.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

McCarley, Jason S., Jeffrey R. W. Mounts, and Arthur F. Kramer. "Spatially mediated capacity limits in attentive visual perception." Acta Psychologica 126, no. 2 (October 2007): 98–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.11.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Hologa, Rafael, and Nils Riach. "Approaching Bike Hazards via Crowdsourcing of Volunteered Geographic Information." Sustainability 12, no. 17 (August 28, 2020): 7015. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/su12177015.

Full text
Abstract:
Information on individual hazard perception while cycling and the associated feeling of safety are key aspects to foster sustainable urban cycling mobility. Although cyclist’s perceptions must also be critically reviewed, such crowdsourced Volunteered Geographic Information (VGI) provides wide-ranging insights on diverse hazard categories in cycling. In this case study in the city of Freiburg, Germany, hazard perceptions, information about lane types, and the underlying routes were crowdsourced via an open source smartphone application by a small group with the aim of providing cyclists with effective solutions. By dealing with levels of reliability, we show that even a small group of laypersons can generate an extensive and valuable set of VGI consisting of comprehensive hazard categories. We demonstrate that (1) certain hazards are interlinked to specific lane types, and (2) the individual hazard perceptions and objective parameters, i.e., accident data, are often congruent spatially; consequently, (3) dangerous hot spots can be derived. By considering cyclists’ needs, this approach outlines how a people-based perspective can supplement regional planning on the local scale.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Gerasimova, Irina. "Thought and its «Garments»." Ideas and Ideals 12, no. 3-1 (September 23, 2020): 57–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.17212/2075-0862-2020-12.3.1-57-76.

Full text
Abstract:
The article develops a methodological conception. It is based on the principles of the philosophy of complexity. The author believes that the discussion of the problems of rationality in the space of cultural diversity will be futile if we do not take into account the cognitive and socio-cultural aspects of meaning generation. The author draws attention to the communicative nature of meaning formation, which is increasing in the context of globalization. Such forms of organization of collective thinking as an interdisciplinary and transdiciplinary dialogue spread to the philosophical community. A distinctive feature of historical and philosophical research remains a special attention to textual artifacts, but modern methodologies must also contribute to the understanding of ancient knowledge and mentality. The author offers a methodological model of meaning generation. The coordinate grid of axes is its basis. As bearing axes, the author introduces: conscious-unconscious, explicit-implicit (hidden), external-internal, linear-nonlinear, order-chaos, simple-complex, reflexive-pre-reflexive, discrete-continuum. The Genesis of ethno-cultural mentalities took place in unique natural, cultural, historical and linguistic conditions. As a result, cultures can differ in the types of perception of space-time relations. This is reflected in the variety of space-time models. The problem of pairing the personal, environmental and social worlds of time by A.A. Krushinskiy. He presented the hexagram model of time as a game of player-personality and player-society. In the complex process of meaning formation, the author identifies the conscious layer of language and speech, the semi-conscious layer of images, the unconscious layer of states and pre-reflexive understanding. In the course of global cognitive evolution, there were revolutionary turns towards the development of conscious speech from the depths of the unconscious. At the same time, different cultures had their own trajectories of rationality development, developing specific languages and mental models. Ideographic language such as Chinese stimulated the development of spatial-imaginative thinking based on visual algorithms. In alphabetic languages of Indo-European type, the linearity of speech is only the external plan of expression, while the nonlinear spatiality (geometric style) of meaning formation works in internal dimensions. Discussing the noumenal sources of meaning formation, the author addresses the understanding of the nature of thought in spiritual philosophies and modern cognitive research. Scientific research of deep, pre-reflexive layers of understanding in the general structure and dynamics of meaning formation can bring a new dimension to the discussion on the «geography of rationality». In the global world, when unique cultures interact, new harmonics of the general planetary consciousness are formed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Vukosav, Branimir, and Borna Fuerst-Bjeliš. "Medijska percepcija prostornih identiteta: konstrukcija imaginativne karte dalmatinske unutrašnjosti." Geoadria 20, no. 1 (March 4, 2015): 23. http://dx.doi.org/10.15291/geoadria.29.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of the paper is to use the selected informative medium as a source of data about spatial perceptions of five areas in the dalmatian hinterland – Zagora, Bukovica, Ravni kotari, Zabiokovlje, Neretva area. Analysing the archives of the selected media source, a detailed set of criteria is used to extract and spatially classify the relevant geographic names in order to define the extents of perception of the mentioned regions as cognitive models (the concept of vernacular region). The acquired results for the five vernacular regions, standardized on the level of administrative units, are used to construct an imaginative map of the Dalmatian hinterland as a reflection of the collective imagery of the regions’ spatial extent.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Yvonne, Maingey, Gilbert Ouma, Daniel Olago, and Maggie Opondo. "Trends In Climate Variables (Temperature And Rainfall) And Local Perceptions Of Climate Change In Lamu, Kenya." GEOGRAPHY, ENVIRONMENT, SUSTAINABILITY 13, no. 3 (October 2, 2020): 102–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.24057/2071-9388-2020-24.

Full text
Abstract:
Community adaptation to the negative impacts of climate change benefits from an analysis of both the trends in climate variables and people’s perception of climate change. This paper contends that members of the local community have observed changes in temperature and rainfall patterns and that these perceptions can be positively correlated with meteorological records. This is particularly useful for remote regions like Lamu whereby access to weather data is spatially and temporally challenged. Linear trend analysis is employed to describe the change in temperature and rainfall in Lamu using monthly data obtained from the Kenya Meteorological Department (KMD) for the period 1974–2014. To determine local perceptions and understanding of the trends, results from a household survey are presented. Significant warming trends have been observed in the study area over the period 1974–2014. This warming is attributed to a rise in maximum temperatures. In contrast to temperature, a clear picture of the rainfall trend has not emerged. Perceptions of the local community closely match the findings on temperature, with majority of the community identifying a rise in temperature over the same period. The findings suggest that the process of validating community perceptions of trends with historical meteorological data analysis can promote adaptation planning that is inclusive and responsive to local experiences.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Roberson, Gwendolyn E., Mark T. Wallace, and James A. Schirillo. "The sensorimotor contingency of multisensory localization correlates with the conscious percept of spatial unity." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, no. 5 (October 2001): 1001–2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0154011x.

Full text
Abstract:
Two cross-modal experiments provide partial support for O'Regan & Noë's (O&N's) claim that sensorimotor contingencies mediate perception. Differences in locating a target sound accompanied by a spatially disparate neutral light correlate with whether the two stimuli were perceived as spatially unified. This correlation suggests that internal representations are necessary for conscious perception, which may also mediate sensorimotor contingencies.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Thurman, Steven M., and Hongjing Lu. "Perception of Social Interactions for Spatially Scrambled Biological Motion." PLoS ONE 9, no. 11 (November 18, 2014): e112539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112539.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Osman, Robert, and Jana Jíchová. "„A přišel ti naproti?“: dohled rodičů jako neviditelná bariéra prostorového chování jejich dcer ve vzdálených univerzitních městech." Sociální studia / Social Studies 16, no. 1 (July 10, 2019): 103–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.5817/soc2019-1-103.

Full text
Abstract:
Research on perceptions of security has long focused on exploring fear for oneself, but fear for other person, so-called altruistic fear, which can contribute significantly to the overall perception of security, is equally important. Fear for others is also related to the need for surveillance and control over those for whom we worry. This article focuses on the importance of transferred, spatially absent parental control for the perception of fear for daughters who moved from a small municipality to a large, distant university city. Through semi-structured interviews with female first and second year college students, we examine how their feelings of safety and danger in the new environment transformed during their transition to university, how parents approached their leaving home and how they communicated with their daughters, and how the parents continued to supervise them despite the spatial divide. Our analysis shows the different forms that the relationship between parental control and their daughter’s fear can take, and how fear for oneself can be interwoven with fear for others.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Camacho-Valdez, Vera, Andrea Saenz-Arroyo, Andrea Ghermandi, Dario A. Navarrete-Gutiérrez, and Rocío Rodiles-Hernández. "Spatial analysis, local people’s perception and economic valuation of wetland ecosystem services in the Usumacinta floodplain, Southern Mexico." PeerJ 8 (January 31, 2020): e8395. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.8395.

Full text
Abstract:
The Usumacinta floodplain is an exceptional area for biodiversity with important ecosystem services for local people. The main objective of this paper was to estimate reference values and define local perceptions of ecosystem services provided by wetlands and overlapping them with spatially explicit socioeconomic and biodiversity indicators. We used the Usumacinta floodplain as an example of a territory where high dependence of rural people on ecosystem services is confronted with development projects that threat the flow of ecosystem services, thus affecting rural people well-being. With a combination of data from remote sensing, global databases of ecosystem service values, local perception of ecosystem services and socioeconomic and biodiversity richness indicators in a spatially explicit framework, we develop a policy-oriented approach for rapid assessment to manage wetlands and maintain people’s livelihoods. Regulating and provisioning services are identified as the most relevant ecosystem services in terms of their monetary value and local perceived importance. In a spatially explicit manner, this approach highlights the most valuable wetlands and identifies rural societies that are highly dependent on ecosystem services. Our approach can be replicated elsewhere and could provide valuable information for policymakers to design policies that can contribute to conserve wetland ecosystems where under threat of development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Wang, Yingxu. "Formal Descriptions of Cognitive Processes of Perceptions on Spatiality, Time, and Motion." International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence 3, no. 2 (April 2009): 84–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jcini.2009040105.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Press, Clare, Elena Gherri, Cecilia Heyes, and Martin Eimer. "Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, no. 10 (October 2010): 2198–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21409.

Full text
Abstract:
Several theories of the mechanisms linking perception and action require that the links are bidirectional, but there is a lack of consensus on the effects that action has on perception. We investigated this by measuring visual event-related brain potentials to observed hand actions while participants prepared responses that were spatially compatible (e.g., both were on the left side of the body) or incompatible and action type compatible (e.g., both were finger taps) or incompatible, with observed actions. An early enhanced processing of spatially compatible stimuli was observed, which is likely due to spatial attention. This was followed by an attenuation of processing for both spatially and action type compatible stimuli, likely to be driven by efference copy signals that attenuate processing of predicted sensory consequences of actions. Attenuation was not response-modality specific; it was found for manual stimuli when participants prepared manual and vocal responses, in line with the hypothesis that action control is hierarchically organized. These results indicate that spatial attention and forward model prediction mechanisms have opposite, but temporally distinct, effects on perception. This hypothesis can explain the inconsistency of recent findings on action–perception links and thereby supports the view that sensorimotor links are bidirectional. Such effects of action on perception are likely to be crucial, not only for the control of our own actions but also in sociocultural interaction, allowing us to predict the reactions of others to our own actions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Luo, Anna Xiao, and Jiaying Zhao. "Capacity limit of ensemble perception of multiple spatially intermixed sets." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 80, no. 8 (August 8, 2018): 2033–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1572-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Feldman, Yuri, and Vadim Indelman. "Spatially-dependent Bayesian semantic perception under model and localization uncertainty." Autonomous Robots 44, no. 6 (June 25, 2020): 1091–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10514-020-09921-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Biedenweg, Kelly, Lee Cerveny, and Rebecca McLain. "Values Mapping with Latino Forest Users: Contributing to the Dialogue on Multiple Land Use Conflict Management." Practicing Anthropology 36, no. 1 (January 1, 2014): 33–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.17730/praa.36.1.3j78673527m17u26.

Full text
Abstract:
Values mapping that represents how humans associate with natural environments is useful for several purposes, including recognizing and addressing different perceptions of natural resource ownership and management priorities, documenting traditional ecological knowledge, and spatially identifying the public's perception of economic and non-economic services provided by natural resources (McLain et al. 2013). The majority of this work has been conducted in developing countries and with disenfranchised communities, where participatory mapping associated with natural resource management is more widely practiced. As access to GIS technology has expanded, however, several projects have tested the benefits of values mapping for natural resource management decisions in industrialized countries (e.g., Brown 2005; Klain and Chan 2012). This article discusses one such effort: the use of spatial values mapping to incorporate the concerns of Latino forest users into federal and state policies on the Olympic Peninsula in Washington State.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Koo, Hongmi, Janina Kleemann, and Christine Fürst. "Integrating Ecosystem Services into Land-Use Modeling to Assess the Effects of Future Land-Use Strategies in Northern Ghana." Land 9, no. 10 (October 8, 2020): 379. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/land9100379.

Full text
Abstract:
In West Africa, where the majority of the population relies on natural resources and rain-fed agriculture, regionally adapted agricultural land-use planning is increasingly important to cope with growing demand for land-use products and intensifying climate variability. As an approach to identify effective future land-use strategies, this study applied spatially explicit modeling that addresses the spatial connectivity between the provision of ecosystem services and agricultural land-use systems. Considering that the status of ecosystem services varies with the perception of stakeholders, local knowledge, and characteristics of a case study area, two adjoining districts in northern Ghana were integrated into an assessment process of land-use strategies. Based on agricultural land-management options that were identified together with the local stakeholders, 75 future land-use strategies as combinations of multiple agricultural practices were elaborated. Potential impacts of the developed land-use strategies on ecosystem services and land-use patterns were assessed in a modeling platform that combines Geographic Information System (GIS) and Cellular Automaton (CA) modules. Modeled results were used to identify best land-use strategies that could deliver multiple ecosystem services most effectively. Then, local perception was applied to determine the feasibility of the best land-use strategies in practice. The results presented the different extent of trade-offs and synergies between ecosystem services delivered by future land-use strategies and their different feasibility depending on the district. Apart from the fact that findings were context-specific and scale-dependent, this study revealed that the integration of different local characteristics and local perceptions to spatially explicit ecosystem service assessment is beneficial for determining locally tailored recommendations for future agricultural land-use planning.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Fischer, Martin H., Nele Warlop, Robin L. Hill, and Wim Fias. "Oculomotor Bias Induced by Number Perception." Experimental Psychology 51, no. 2 (January 2004): 91–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.51.2.91.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Previous research with manual response methods has found evidence for an association between numbers and space. The present study investigated whether eye movements also show this association. Eye movement responses were recorded from 15 healthy participants as they categorized the digits 0-9 as odd or even. Responses were initiated faster to the left in response to small digits and faster to the right in response to large digits. Movement amplitudes were not systematically affected by either number magnitude or parity. These results provide further evidence for a spatially oriented “mental number line”.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Arterberry, Martha E., Catherine Craver-Lemley, and Adam Reeves. "Visual imagery is not always like visual perception." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, no. 2 (April 2002): 183–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x02230046.

Full text
Abstract:
The “Perky effect” is the interference of visual imagery with vision. Studies of this effect show that visual imagery has more than symbolic properties, but these properties differ both spatially (including “pictorially”) and temporally from those of vision. We therefore reject both the literal picture-in-the-head view and the entirely symbolic view.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Neubarth, Nicole L., Alan J. Emanuel, Yin Liu, Mark W. Springel, Annie Handler, Qiyu Zhang, Brendan P. Lehnert, et al. "Meissner corpuscles and their spatially intermingled afferents underlie gentle touch perception." Science 368, no. 6497 (June 18, 2020): eabb2751. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.abb2751.

Full text
Abstract:
Meissner corpuscles are mechanosensory end organs that densely occupy mammalian glabrous skin. We generated mice that selectively lacked Meissner corpuscles and found them to be deficient in both perceiving the gentlest detectable forces acting on glabrous skin and fine sensorimotor control. We found that Meissner corpuscles are innervated by two mechanoreceptor subtypes that exhibit distinct responses to tactile stimuli. The anatomical receptive fields of these two mechanoreceptor subtypes homotypically tile glabrous skin in a manner that is offset with respect to one another. Electron microscopic analysis of the two Meissner afferents within the corpuscle supports a model in which the extent of lamellar cell wrappings of mechanoreceptor endings determines their force sensitivity thresholds and kinetic properties.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Scarpaci, Jacob W., and H. Steven Colburn. "A real‐time virtual auditory system for spatially dynamic perception research." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 115, no. 5 (May 2004): 2599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4784539.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Freigang, Claudia, Wiktor Mlynarski, Marc Stöhr, Rudolf Rübsamen, Jan Bennemann, and Philipp Benner. "Perceptual ambiguity — perception and processing of spatially discordant/concordant audiovisual stimuli." Multisensory Research 26, no. 1-2 (2013): 118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-000s0086.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Frassinetti, Francesca, Barbara Magnani, and Massimiliano Oliveri. "Prismatic Lenses Shift Time Perception." Psychological Science 20, no. 8 (August 2009): 949–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02390.x.

Full text
Abstract:
Previous studies have demonstrated the involvement of spatial codes in the representation of time and numbers. We took advantage of a well-known spatial modulation (prismatic adaptation) to test the hypothesis that the representation of time is spatially oriented from left to right, with smaller time intervals being represented to the left of larger time intervals. Healthy subjects performed a time-reproduction task and a time-bisection task, before and after leftward and rightward prismatic adaptation. Results showed that prismatic adaptation inducing a rightward orientation of spatial attention produced an overestimation of time intervals, whereas prismatic adaptation inducing a leftward shift of spatial attention produced an underestimation of time intervals. These findings not only confirm that temporal intervals are represented as horizontally arranged in space, but also reveal that spatial modulation of time processing most likely occurs via cuing of spatial attention, and that spatial attention can influence the spatial coding of quantity in different dimensions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Winkel, J. Ó., B. Schottky, U. Krey, and F. Gerl. "Storing semantically correlated and spatially segmented patterns in the perceptron." Zeitschrift für Physik B Condensed Matter 101, no. 2 (March 1996): 305–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002570050213.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Cannon, Mark W. "A Model of Mechanisms Mediating Spatial Pattern Perception." Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting 36, no. 18 (October 1992): 1430–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193129203601815.

Full text
Abstract:
A model consisting of multiple tuned and oriented spatial filters followed by non-linear transducer functions is described. The model was originally derived to account for human perception of contrast while viewing isolated stimuli. The model can also account for human estimates for the image sharpness of spatially filtered real world scenes. The model has several shortcomings uncovered by recent experimental results involving suppression of the apparent contrast of a foveally presented grating patch by a peripheral grating.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Liddicoat, Stephanie. "Spirituality in Therapeutic Spaces: Perceptions of Spatiality, Trace, and Past Rituals Manifesting Present Occupation." Journal of Interior Design 44, no. 2 (January 4, 2019): 101–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/joid.12137.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Lederman, Susan J., and Roberta L. Klatzky. "Sensing and Displaying Spatially Distributed Fingertip Forces in Haptic Interfaces for Teleoperator and Virtual Environment Systems." Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 8, no. 1 (February 1999): 86–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/105474699566062.

Full text
Abstract:
This article reports a variety of sensory and perceptual consequences of eliminating, via a rigid fingertip sheath, the spatially distributed fingertip force information that is normally available during tactile and haptic sensing. Sensory measures included tactile spatial acuity, tactile force, and vibrotactile thresholds. Suprathreshold tasks included perception of roughness, perception of 2-D edge orientation, and detection of a simulated 3-D mass in simulated tissue via fingertip palpation. Of these performance measures, only vibrotactile thresholds and texture perception failed to show substantial impairment. The results are discussed in terms of their implications for the future design of haptic interfaces for teleoperator and virtual environment systems.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Pelayo Sañudo, Eva. "The ethnic flâneuse: The Right to the City and Embodied Streets in Julia Savarese’s The Weak and the Strong (1952) and Marion Benasutti’s No Steady Job for Papa (1966)." Complutense Journal of English Studies 27 (October 4, 2019): 293–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.5209/cjes.61462.

Full text
Abstract:
Drawing on Edward W. Soja’s radical critique to the prevailing narrative of history in social theory, this paper investigates how two novels on Italian/American ethnic identity are distinctively spatialized. The analysis focuses on the characters’ different experiences and perceptions of space, which attest to the interplay of identity and spatial production, paying attention to agency and spatial stories that are specifically localized. By using theory on the (re)production of space, this paper analyzes how urban representational and material patterns relate to social division, in terms of ethnicity and gender, and how the perpetuation of inequality is spatially enacted. Particularly, it examines the key gendered urban layout that is revealed in how women are often “in transit” (Gómez-Reus and Glifford 2013), “out of place” (McDowell 1997) or in fear (Valentine 1989; Pain 2001) in the “embodied spaces” of the streets (Tonkiss 2005). Through two texts of early Italian/American fiction, this paper addresses the spatial practices, as well as restrictions, of the embodied racialized and gendered subject. To this end, the figure of the ethnic flâneuse (Carrera-Suárez 2015) represents a suitable object of study on embodied spatiality which serves to subvert traditional intersectional constraints of spatial design and discourse.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography