Journal articles on the topic 'Paw preference'

To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Paw preference.

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Paw preference.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Wells and McDowell. "Laterality as a Tool for Assessing Breed Differences in Emotional Reactivity in the Domestic Cat, Felis silvestris catus." Animals 9, no. 9 (September 3, 2019): 647. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani9090647.

Full text
Abstract:
Cat breeds differ enormously in their behavioural disposition, a factor that can impact on the pet-owner relationship, with indirect consequences for animal welfare. This study examined whether lateral bias, in the form of paw preference, can be used as a tool for assessing breed differences in emotional reactivity in the cat. The paw preferences of 4 commonly owned breeds were tested using a food-reaching challenge. Cats were more likely to be paw-preferent than ambilateral. Maine Coons, Ragdolls and Bengals were more likely to be paw-preferent than ambilateral, although only the Bengals showed a consistent preference for using one paw (left) over the other. The strength of the cats’ paw use was related to cat breed, with Persians being more weakly lateralised. Direction of paw use was unrelated to feline breed, but strongly sex-related, with male cats showing a left paw preference and females displaying a right-sided bias. We propose that paw preference measurement could provide a useful method for assessing emotional reactivity in domestic cats. Such information would be of benefit to individuals considering the acquisition of a new cat, and, in the longer term, may help to foster more successful cat-owner relationships, leading to indirect benefits to feline welfare.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Rogers, Timothy T., and M. Barbara Bulman-Fleming. "Arousal mediates relations among medial paw preference, lateral paw preference, and spatial preference in the mouse." Behavioural Brain Research 93, no. 1-2 (June 1998): 51–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00141-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Biddle, Fred G., Cristina M. Coffaro, Jeanette E. Ziehr, and Brenda A. Eales. "Genetic variation in paw preference (handedness) in the mouse." Genome 36, no. 5 (October 1, 1993): 935–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/g93-123.

Full text
Abstract:
Lateralization of paw preference in laboratory mice in a single-paw reaching task has been used as a model system for left- and right-hand usage. Given a set number of paw reaches for food from a centrally placed food tube, an individual mouse will exhibit a reliable number of left and right paw reaches. Within any single inbred strain, there are approximately equal numbers of left-pawed and right-pawed mice. Nevertheless, significant strain differences have been reported for the degree of lateralization of paw preference. We report here a systematic survey of paw preference in 12 inbred strains of the mouse in which the degree of lateralization falls into two groups of weakly lateralized and highly lateralized paw preference. The genetic inference is that a single major gene may control some function, and alternate alleles at this locus are expressed as weakly and highly lateralized paw preference. Reciprocal crosses indicate the trait is additive with no maternal or X-linked effects. The direction of paw preference has previously appeared to be genetically neutral, but in some strains there is evidence of significant deviation of the numbers of mice to the left and right of equal paw usage, independent of degree of lateralization, and this suggests that direction of left–right paw usage may be a separate genetic trait in the mouse model.Key words: behavioural genetics, paw preference, handedness, mouse.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Lee, S., A. Konno, and T. Hasegawa. "Asymmetrical paw preference and personality." Journal of Veterinary Behavior 6, no. 1 (January 2011): 84–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2010.09.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Simon, Tim, Elisa Frasnelli, Kun Guo, Anjuli Barber, Anna Wilkinson, and Daniel S. Mills. "Is There an Association between Paw Preference and Emotionality in Pet Dogs?" Animals 12, no. 9 (April 29, 2022): 1153. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani12091153.

Full text
Abstract:
Research with humans and other animals has suggested that preferential limb use is linked to emotionality. A better understanding of this still under-explored area has the potential to establish limb preference as a marker of emotional vulnerability and risk for affective disorders. This study explored the potential relationship between paw preference and emotionality in pet dogs. We examined which paw the dogs preferentially used to hold a Kong™ and to perform two different locomotion tests. Dogs’ emotionality was assessed using a validated psychometric test (the Positive and Negative Activation Scale—PANAS). Significant positive correlations were found for dogs’ paw use between the different locomotion tasks, suggesting that dogs may show a more general paw preference that is stable across different types of locomotion. In comparison, the correlations between the Kong™ Test and locomotion tests were only partially significant, likely due to potential limitations of the Kong™ Test and/or test-specific biomechanical requirements. No significant correlations were identified between paw preference tests and PANAS scores. These results are in contrast to previous reports of an association between dog paw preference and emotionality; animal limb preference might be task-specific and have variable task-consistency, which raises methodological questions about the use of paw preference as a marker for emotional functioning.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Takeda, Satoshi, and Akira Endo. "Paw preference in mice: A reappraisal." Physiology & Behavior 53, no. 4 (April 1993): 727–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(93)90180-n.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Cabib, S., F. R. D'amato, P. J. Neveu, B. Deleplanque, M. Le Moal, and S. Puglish-Allegra. "Paw preference and brain dopamine asymmetries." Neuroscience 64, no. 2 (January 1995): 427–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(94)00401-p.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Barnéoud, Pascal, Gilles Bronchti, and Hendrik Van der Loos. "Vision influences paw-preference in mice." Behavioural Brain Research 62, no. 2 (June 1994): 157–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)90023-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Rogers, Lesley J. "Hand and paw preferences in relation to the lateralized brain." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364, no. 1519 (December 4, 2008): 943–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0225.

Full text
Abstract:
Hand preferences of primates are discussed as part of the broad perspective of brain lateralization in animals, and compared with paw preferences in non-primates. Previously, it has been suggested that primates are more likely to express a species-typical hand preference on complex tasks, especially in the case of coordinated hand use in using tools. I suggest that population-level hand preferences are manifested when the task demands the obligate use of the processing specialization of one hemisphere, and that this depends on the nature of the task rather than its complexity per se . Depending on the species, simple reaching tasks may not demand the obligate use of a specialized hemisphere and so do not constrain limb/hand use. In such cases, individuals may show hand preferences that are associated with consistent differences in behaviour. The individual's hand preference is associated with the expression of behaviour controlled by the hemisphere contralateral to the preferred hand (fear and reactivity in left-handed individuals versus proactivity in right-handed individuals). Recent findings of differences in brain structure between left- and right-handed primates (e.g. somatosensory cortex in marmosets) have been discussed and related to potential evolutionary advances.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Tan, Üner, Mevlüt Yaprak, and Necíp Kutlu. "Paw Preference in Cats: Distribution and Sex Differences." International Journal of Neuroscience 50, no. 3-4 (January 1990): 195–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459008987172.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Fu, Q. L., Y. Q. Shen, M. X. Gao, J. Dong, P. J. Neveu, and K. S. Li. "Brain interleukin asymmetries and paw preference in mice." Neuroscience 116, no. 3 (February 2003): 639–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00746-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Ribeiro-Carvalho, Anderson, Yael Abreu-Villaça, Danielle Paes-Branco, Cláudio C. Filgueiras, and Alex C. Manhães. "Novelty affects paw preference performance in adult mice." Animal Behaviour 80, no. 1 (July 2010): 51–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.024.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Poyser, Fay, Christine Caldwell, and Matthew Cobb. "Dog paw preference shows lability and sex differences." Behavioural Processes 73, no. 2 (September 2006): 216–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2006.05.011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Barnard, Shanis, Deborah L. Wells, and Peter G. Hepper. "Laterality as a Predictor of Coping Strategies in Dogs Entering a Rescue Shelter." Symmetry 10, no. 11 (October 23, 2018): 538. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/sym10110538.

Full text
Abstract:
It has been reported that during the first few days following entry to a kennel environment, shelter dogs may suffer poor welfare. Previous work suggests that motor bias (the preferred use of one limb over the other) can potentially be used as an indicator of emotional reactivity and welfare risk. In this study, we investigate whether paw preference could be used as a predictive indicator of stress coping (measured using cortisol levels and behavioural observation) in a sample of 41 dogs entering a rescue shelter. Cortisol levels and behavioural observations were collected for one week after admission. We scored the dogs’ paw preference during a food-retrieval task. Our results showed that increasing left-pawedness was associated with a higher expression of stress-related behaviours such as frequent change of state, vocalisations and lower body posture. These results are in keeping with previous findings showing that left-limb biased animals are more vulnerable to stress. Paw preference testing may be a useful tool for detecting different coping strategies in dogs entering a kennel environment and identifying target individuals at risk of reduced welfare.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Tan, Üner, İsmai˙l Kara, and Şule Tan. "Lithium and Imipramin Effects on Paw Preference in Cats." International Journal of Neuroscience 52, no. 1-2 (January 1990): 25–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459008994240.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Waters, Nicholas S., and Victor H. Denenberg. "A measure of lateral paw preference in the mouse." Physiology & Behavior 50, no. 4 (October 1991): 853–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(91)90030-r.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Bulman-Fleming, M. Barbara, M. Philip Bryden, and Timothy T. Rogers. "Mouse paw preference: effects of variations in testing protocol." Behavioural Brain Research 86, no. 1 (June 1997): 79–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(96)02249-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Tan, Üner, Ismail Kara, and Necip Kutlu. "The effects of testosterone on paw preference in adult cats." International Journal of Neuroscience 56, no. 1-4 (January 1991): 187–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459108985415.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

AYDINLIOGLU, ATIF, KADİR ARSLAN, NURETTİN CENGİZ, MURAT RAGBETLI, and ENDER ERDOGAN. "THE RELATIONSHIPS OF DOG HIPPOCAMPUS TO SEX AND PAW PREFERENCE." International Journal of Neuroscience 116, no. 1 (January 2006): 77–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207450690962433.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Quaranta, A., M. Siniscalchi, A. Frate, and G. Vallortigara. "Paw preference in dogs: relations between lateralised behaviour and immunity." Behavioural Brain Research 153, no. 2 (August 2004): 521–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2004.01.009.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Joly, Marine, Marina Scheumann, and Elke Zimmermann. "Posture Does Not Matter! Paw Usage and Grasping Paw Preference in a Small-Bodied Rooting Quadrupedal Mammal." PLoS ONE 7, no. 5 (May 30, 2012): e38228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038228.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Ribeiro, Andre S., Brenda A. Eales, Jason Lloyd-Price, and Fred G. Biddle. "Predictability and randomness of paw choices are critical elements in the behavioural plasticity of mouse paw preference." Animal Behaviour 98 (December 2014): 167–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.10.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Tan, Üner. "Distribution of Paw Preference in Mongrel and Tortoise-Shell Cats and the Relation of Hemispheric Weight to Paw Preference: Sexual Dimorphism in Palw use and its Relation to Hemispheric Weight." International Journal of Neuroscience 70, no. 3-4 (January 1993): 199–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459309000575.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Wells, Deborah L., Peter G. Hepper, Adam D. S. Milligan, and Shanis Barnard. "Cognitive bias and paw preference in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)." Journal of Comparative Psychology 131, no. 4 (November 2017): 317–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/com0000080.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Branson, N. J., and L. J. Rogers. "Relationship between paw preference strength and noise phobia in Canis familiaris." Journal of Comparative Psychology 120, no. 3 (August 2006): 176–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.176.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Fabre-Thorpe, M., J. Fagot, E. Lorincz, F. Levesque, and J. Vauclair. "Laterality in Cats: Paw Preference And Performance in a Visuomotor Activity." Cortex 29, no. 1 (March 1993): 15–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80208-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Fride, Ester, Robert L. Collins, Phil Skolnick, and Prince K. Arora. "Strain-dependent association between immune function and paw preference in mice." Brain Research 522, no. 2 (July 1990): 246–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(90)91468-v.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Aydinlioglu, Atif, Kadir Arslan, A. Riza Erdogan, Murat �etin Ragbetli, Papatya Keles, and Semih Diyarbakirli. "Short Communication. The Relationship of Callosal Anatomy to Paw Preference In dogs." European Journal of Morphology 38, no. 2 (April 1, 2000): 128–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/0924-3860(200004)38:2;1-f;ft128.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Betancur, Catalina, Pierre J. Neveu, and Michel Le Moal. "Strain and sex differences in the degree of paw preference in mice." Behavioural Brain Research 45, no. 1 (October 1991): 97–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80185-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Wu, He-ming, Chao Wang, Xue-lian Wang, Ling Wang, Chong-wang Chang, Peng Wang, and Guo-dong Gao. "Correlations between angiotensinase activity asymmetries in the brain and paw preference in rats." Neuropeptides 44, no. 3 (June 2010): 253–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2009.12.016.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Nielsen, Darci M., Kathleen E. Visker, Matthew J. Cunningham, Richard W. Keller, Stanley D. Glick, and Jeffrey N. Carlson. "Paw Preference, Rotation, and Dopamine Function in Collins HI and LO Mouse Strains." Physiology & Behavior 61, no. 4 (April 1997): 525–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00496-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Lipp, Hans-Peter, Robert L. Collins, Zafiro Hausheer-Zarmakupi, Marie-Claire Leisinger-Trigona, Wim E. Crusio, Marika Nosten-Bertrand, Pierre Signore, Herbert Schwegler, and David P. Wolfer. "Paw preference and intra-/infrapyramidal mossy fibers in the hippocampus of the mouse." Behavior Genetics 26, no. 4 (July 1996): 379–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02359482.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Manhães, Alex C., Thomas E. Krahe, Egas Caparelli-Dáquer, Anderson Ribeiro-Carvalho, Sergio L. Schmidt, and Cláudio C. Filgueiras. "Neonatal transection of the corpus callosum affects paw preference lateralization of adult Swiss mice." Neuroscience Letters 348, no. 2 (September 2003): 69–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00746-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Ecevitoglu, Alev, Efe Soyman, Resit Canbeyli, and Gunes Unal. "Paw preference is associated with behavioural despair and spatial reference memory in male rats." Behavioural Processes 180 (November 2020): 104254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104254.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Waters, Nicholas S., and Victor H. Denenberg. "Analysis of two measures of paw preference in a large population of inbred mice." Behavioural Brain Research 63, no. 2 (August 1994): 195–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)90091-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Zuo, Zhen-Xing, Yong-Jie Wang, Li Liu, Yiner Wang, Shu-Hao Mei, Zhi-Hui Feng, Maode Wang, and Xiang-Yao Li. "Huperzine A Alleviates Mechanical Allodynia but Not Spontaneous Pain via Muscarinic Acetylcholine Receptors in Mice." Neural Plasticity 2015 (2015): 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/453170.

Full text
Abstract:
Chronic pain is a major health issue and most patients suffer from spontaneous pain. Previous studies suggest that Huperzine A (Hup A), an alkaloid isolated from the Chinese herbHuperzia serrata, is a potent analgesic with few side effects. However, whether it alleviates spontaneous pain is unclear. We evaluated the effects of Hup A on spontaneous pain in mice using the conditioned place preference (CPP) behavioral assay and found that application of Hup A attenuated the mechanical allodynia induced by peripheral nerve injury or inflammation. This effect was blocked by atropine. However, clonidine but not Hup A induced preference for the drug-paired chamber in CPP. The same effects occurred when Hup A was infused into the anterior cingulate cortex. Furthermore, ambenonium chloride, a competitive inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase, also increased the paw-withdrawal threshold but failed to induce place preference in CPP. Therefore, our data suggest that acetylcholinesterase in both the peripheral and central nervous systems is involved in the regulation of mechanical allodynia but not the spontaneous pain.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Tan, Uner, and Necip Kutlu. "Sexual Dimorphism in Body and Brain Weight and Its Association with Paw Preference in Cats." International Journal of Neuroscience 73, no. 1-2 (January 1993): 23–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459308987208.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

GÜVEN, MUSTAFA, DERYA DENIZ ELALMIS, SEÇIL BINOKAY, and ÜNER TAN. "POPULATION-LEVEL RIGHT-PAW PREFERENCE IN RATS ASSESSED BY A NEW COMPUTERIZED FOOD-REACHING TEST." International Journal of Neuroscience 113, no. 12 (January 2003): 1675–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207450390249258.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Konerding, Wiebke S., Hans-Jürgen Hedrich, Eva Bleich, and Elke Zimmermann. "Paw preference is not affected by postural demand in a nonprimate mammal (Felis silvestris catus)." Journal of Comparative Psychology 126, no. 1 (February 2012): 15–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024638.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Sullivan, R. M., S. L. Chehab, M. M. Dufresne, and F. Laplante. "Role of sex in the neurochemical and neuroendocrine correlates of paw preference in the rat." Neuroscience 202 (January 2012): 192–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.12.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Wells, Deborah L., Peter G. Hepper, Adam D. S. Milligan, and Shanis Barnard. "Lack of association between paw preference and behaviour problems in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris." Applied Animal Behaviour Science 210 (January 2019): 81–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.10.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Neveu, Pierre J., Catalina Betancur, Sergio Vitiello, and Michel Le Moal. "Sex-dependent association between immune function and paw preference in two substrains of C3H mice." Brain Research 559, no. 2 (September 1991): 347–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(91)90023-o.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Denenberg, Victor H., Gordon F. Sherman, Lisa M. Schrott, Glenn D. Rosen, and Albert M. Galaburda. "Spatial learning, discrimination learning, paw preference and neocortical ectopias in two autoimmune strains of mice." Brain Research 562, no. 1 (October 1991): 98–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(91)91192-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Manhães, Alex C., Sergio L. Schmidt, and Cláudio C. Filgueiras. "Callosal agenesis affects consistency of laterality in a paw preference task in BALB/cCF mice." Behavioural Brain Research 159, no. 1 (April 2005): 43–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2004.09.023.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Neveu, P. J., P. Barnéoud, S. Vitiello, C. Betancur, and M. Le Moal. "Brain modulation of the immune system: association between lymphocyte responsiveness and paw preference in mice." Brain Research 457, no. 2 (August 1988): 392–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(88)90714-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Vyazovskiy, V. V., and I. Tobler. "Handedness Leads to Interhemispheric EEG Asymmetry During Sleep in the Rat." Journal of Neurophysiology 99, no. 2 (February 2008): 969–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01154.2007.

Full text
Abstract:
Sleep electroencephalographic (EEG) slow-wave activity is increased after wakefulness and decreases during sleep. Regional sleep EEG differences are thought to be a consequence of activation of specific cortical neuronal circuits during waking. We investigated the relationship between handedness and interhemispheric brain asymmetry. Bilateral EEG recordings were obtained from the frontal and occipital cortex in rats with a clear paw preference in a food-reaching task (right, n = 5; left, n = 5). While still naïve to the task, no waking or sleep EEG asymmetry was present. During the food-reaching task, the waking EEG showed significant, substantial power increases in the frontal hemisphere contralateral to the dominant paw in the low theta range (4.5–6.0 Hz). Moreover, the non-REM sleep EEG following feeding bouts was markedly asymmetric, with significantly higher power in the hemisphere contralateral to the preferred paw in frequencies >1.5 Hz. No asymmetry was evident in the occipital EEG. Correlation analyses revealed a positive association between the hemispheric asymmetry during sleep and the degree of preferred use of the contralateral paw during waking in frequencies <9.0 Hz. Our findings show that handedness is reflected in specific, regional EEG asymmetry during sleep. Neuronal activity induced by preferential use of a particular forelimb led to a local enhancement of EEG power in frequencies within the delta and sigma ranges, supporting the hypothesis of use-dependent local sleep regulation. We conclude that inherent laterality is manifested when animals are exposed to complex behavioral tasks, and sleep plays a role in consolidating the hemispheric dominance of the brain.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

César da Silva, Rodrigo, Fabiano Veiga, Fabiana Cardoso Vilela, André Victor Pereira, Thayssa Tavares da Silva Cunha, Roberta Tesch, Claudio Viegas, et al. "Design, Synthesis and Pharmacological Evaluation of Novel Antiinflammatory and Analgesic O-Benzyloxime Compounds Derived From Natural Eugenol." Letters in Drug Design & Discovery 16, no. 10 (September 19, 2019): 1157–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/1570180815666180620145609.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: : A new series of O-benzyloximes derived from eugenol was synthesized and was evaluated for its antinociceptive and anti-inflammatory properties. Methods: : The target compounds were obtained in good global 25-28% yields over 6 steps, which led us to identify compounds (Z)-5,6-dimethoxy-2,2-dimethyl-2,3-dihydro-1H-inden-1-one-O-(4- (methylthio)benzyloxime (8b), (Z)-5,6-dimethoxy-2,2-dimethyl-2,3-dihydro-1H-inden-1-one-O-4- bromobenzyloxime (8d) and (Z)-5,6-dimethoxy-2,2-dimethyl-2,3-dihydro-1H-inden-1-one-O-4- (methylsulfonyl)benzyloxime (8f) as promising bioactive prototypes. Results:: These compounds have significant analgesic and anti-inflammatory effects, as evidenced by formalin-induced mice paw edema and carrageenan-induced mice paw edema tests. In the formalin test, compounds 8b and 8f evidenced both anti-inflammatory and direct analgesic activities and in the carrageenan-induced paw edema, with compounds 8c, 8d, and 8f showing the best inhibitory effects, exceeding the standard drugs indomethacin and celecoxib. Conclusion: : Molecular docking studies have provided additional evidence that the pharmacological profile of these compounds may be related to inhibition of COX enzymes, with slight preference for COX-1. These results led us to identify the new O-benzyloxime ethers 8b, 8d and 8f as orally bioactive prototypes, with a novel structural pattern capable of being explored in further studies aiming at their optimization and development as drug candidates.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Tan, Ü., and S. Çalşikan. "Allometry and Asymmetry in the Dog Brain: The Right Hemisphere is Heavier Regardless of Paw Preference." International Journal of Neuroscience 35, no. 3-4 (January 1987): 189–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207458708987127.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Clark, Mertice M., Rohan K. Robertson, and Bennett G. Galef. "Intrauterine position effects on sexually dimorphic asymmetries of Mongolian gerbils: Testosterone, eye opening, and paw preference." Developmental Psychobiology 26, no. 4 (May 1993): 185–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.420260402.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

çalişkan, Sadettin, and Üner Tan. "The relationship between the degree of paw preference and excitability of motor neurons innervating foreleg flexors in right- and left-preferent cats." International Journal of Neuroscience 53, no. 2-4 (January 1990): 173–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459008986599.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography