Academic literature on the topic 'Orthography'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Orthography.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Orthography"

1

Roberts, David. "A tone orthography typology." Written Language and Literacy 14, no. 1 (February 17, 2011): 82–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/wll.14.1.05rob.

Full text
Abstract:
Discussions about tone orthography have long been hampered by imprecise terminology. This article aims to bring clarity by means of an explicit typology composed of six parameters. Each parameter is defined by a choice: domain, target, symbol, position, density, and depth. The orthographer assesses each typological aspect individually, while always bearing in mind that the six parameters together generate a complex matrix of responses. The result is a precise and informative character profile for any Roman script tone orthography. Keywords: typology; tone; orthography; orthographic depth
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Jacobsen, Henrik Galberg. "Den normative ortografi og udtalen. Skitse til en typologi." Folia Scandinavica Posnaniensia 20, no. 1 (December 1, 2016): 41–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/fsp-2016-0024.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The paper deals with the relations between Danish pro­nunciation and the so-called ‘normative orthography’, i.e. the official Danish writing rules as laid down by the authorities in orthographic directives since 1889. The specific aim of the paper is to suggest a model for defining and describing the different kinds of normative orthography and their mutual relations. The criteria used for placing the orthographic rules in the model are (a) whether the rule is dependent on pronunciation or not, and (b) whether the resulting orthography (i.e. the prescribed letters) are linguistic signs (inflectional endings, morphemes) or non-signs. The model thus consists of four types of normative orthography, i.e. autonomous expression orthography (Danish: autonom udtryksortografi), autonomous content orthography (Danish: autonom indholdsortografi), mirrored expression orthography (Danish: spejlet udtryksortografi) and mirrored content orthography (Danish: spejlet indholdsortografi). Among these, mirrored expression orthography constitutes the core domain, followed by mirrored content orthography, which has been a growing domain after the 1889-directives, and autonomous content orthography, which is a potentially growing domain.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Burkhardt, Jey Lingam, and Jürgen Martin Burkhardt. "Developing a unified orthography for Berawan." Written Language and Literacy 22, no. 2 (December 31, 2019): 280–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/wll.00029.bur.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Berawan is a small ethnic community, numbering fewer than 4,000 members, in the Malaysian state of Sarawak on Borneo Island. There are four language varieties; namely, Long Jegan (LJG), Batu Belah (BB), Long Teru (LTU) and Long Terawan (LTN). This paper describes the development of the preliminary unified Berawan orthography by the authors, in collaboration with representatives from three Berawan communities (BB, LTU and LJG). Smalley’s (1959, 1965) criteria for orthography development are discussed in relation to the authors’ stage framework for orthographic development, which has been adapted from Rempel (1995) within the context of developing the Berawan orthography and the orthographic decisions made by the Berawan. Two additional factors for orthography implementation are proposed. The first stresses the importance of adequate testing, which is essential for both obtaining objective measurements to inform decision making and establishing a well-designed and effective orthography. The second new factor emphasizes the importance of having sufficient time available to learn the developed orthography.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Rustamovna, Abduraxmonova Umida. "Methods Of Orthographic And Grammatic Analysis Of Uzbek Writing." American Journal of Social Science and Education Innovations 03, no. 04 (April 30, 2021): 364–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.37547/tajssei/volume03issue04-55.

Full text
Abstract:
Orthography is a system of rules adopted to correctly express an idea through writing, which ensures that the idea is equally expressed both for the writer and for the reader. In the teaching of orthography, hearing and sight play an important role. In addition, factors such as logical thinking, memory, intelligence also play an important role in the formation of orthographic skills in students. These are those in which orthography creates a certain system in teaching, one complements the other. This article will give an idea of the methods of orthographic analysis.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Georgiou, Georgios P. "How Do Speakers of a Language with a Transparent Orthographic System Perceive the L2 Vowels of a Language with an Opaque Orthographic System? An Analysis through a Battery of Behavioral Tests." Languages 6, no. 3 (July 11, 2021): 118. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/languages6030118.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: The present study aims to investigate the effect of the first language (L1) orthography on the perception of the second language (L2) vowel contrasts and whether orthographic effects occur at the sublexical level. Methods: Fourteen adult Greek learners of English participated in two AXB discrimination tests: one auditory and one orthography test. In the auditory test, participants listened to triads of auditory stimuli that targeted specific English vowel contrasts embedded in nonsense words and were asked to decide if the middle vowel was the same as the first or the third vowel by clicking on the corresponding labels. The orthography test followed the same procedure as the auditory test, but instead, the two labels contained grapheme representations of the target vowel contrasts. Results: All but one vowel contrast could be more accurately discriminated in the auditory than in the orthography test. The use of nonsense words in the elicitation task eradicated the possibility of a lexical effect of orthography on auditory processing, leaving space for the interpretation of this effect on a sublexical basis, primarily prelexical and secondarily postlexical. Conclusions: L2 auditory processing is subject to L1 orthography influence. Speakers of languages with transparent orthographies such as Greek may rely on the grapheme–phoneme correspondence to decode orthographic representations of sounds coming from languages with an opaque orthographic system such as English.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Miller, Kimberly M., and Diane Swick. "Orthography Influences the Perception of Speech in Alexic Patients." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15, no. 7 (October 1, 2003): 981–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892903770007371.

Full text
Abstract:
Interactive models of reading propose that phonological representations directly activate and/or constrain orthographic representations through feedback. These models also predict that spoken words should activate their orthographic forms. The effect of word orthography on auditory lexical access was investigated in two patients with alexia without agraphia. Several theories of alexia suggest that letter-by-letter reading results from impaired access to orthographic representations. Although alexics can often correctly identify orally spelled words and spell to dictation, it is unknown whether they can access the whole orthographic “word-form” as a unit via auditory presentation. The nonobligatory activation of orthography was examined in an auditory lexical decision task, in which the orthographic and phonological similarity between prime and target was manipulated. In controls, the combined effect of phonological and orthographic relatedness (OP) produced greater facilitation than phonological relatedness alone, indicating that orthography can influence auditory lexical decisions. The alexics displayed patterns of facilitation comparable to controls, suggesting they can quickly access whole-word orthographic information via the auditory modality. An alternate account posits that the OP advantage does not require on-line access of orthography, but instead is a developmental by-product of learning to read an orthographically inconsistent language. The results have implications for cognitive theories of alexia and provide support for interactive models of word recognition.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Venckienė, Jurgita. "Orthography of books and their authors at the end of the 19th and the beginning of the 20th century." Lietuvių kalba, no. 15 (December 28, 2020): 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/lk.2020.22451.

Full text
Abstract:
During development of the Standard Lithuanian language at the end of the 19th century, the dialectal basis was chosen first, and the orthography varied yet for another twenty years. This article analyses the dual orthography – of books and personal orthography of their authors. The study is designed to find out whether the books published during that period reflect the orthographic model chosen by their authors; what factors, in addition to the author’s choice, may have influenced the orthography of the books.The influence of printers on the orthography of books during that period was smaller than before, as many authors did the proofreading themselves. Thus, printers were able to change the orthography in cases where books were printed without the author’s knowledge or consent, such as prayer books. If the author chose unusual, rare, or even self-invented characters, a limited inventory of prints could be a serious obstacle to keep their orthography in the book. As the case of Jonas Basanavičius shows, even when the author offered to finance the acquisition of the necessary prints, this was not necessarily done.At the end of the 19th century, books were published as supplements to periodicals. The editors of newspapers Ūkininkas and Tėvynės sargas adapted the orthography of such books to their periodicals. Under the terms of the press ban, it was often important for authors just to print a book, and the spelling model was chosen by the publisher. However, authors such as Basanavičius, who considered themselves the creators of the standard language, took care to present their chosen or created model of orthography in their books as well.As the cases of Liudvika Didžiulienė, Dominykas Tumėnas and Basanavičius show, two orthographic standards emerged during the research period: correspondence was written one way and books were printed another. Hence, it is not always possible to judge the orthographic model chosen by the authors in books published at the end of the 19th century and the early 20th century.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Paulesu, Eraldo, Rolando Bonandrini, Laura Zapparoli, Cristina Rupani, Cristina Mapelli, Fulvia Tassini, Pietro Schenone, Gabriella Bottini, Conrad Perry, and Marco Zorzi. "Effects of Orthographic Consistency on Bilingual Reading: Human and Computer Simulation Data." Brain Sciences 11, no. 7 (June 30, 2021): 878. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11070878.

Full text
Abstract:
English serves as today’s lingua franca, a role not eased by the inconsistency of its orthography. Indeed, monolingual readers of more consistent orthographies such as Italian or German learn to read more quickly than monolingual English readers. Here, we assessed whether long-lasting bilingualism would mitigate orthography-specific differences in reading speed and whether the order in which orthographies with a different regularity are learned matters. We studied high-proficiency Italian-English and English-Italian bilinguals, with at least 20 years of intensive daily exposure to the second language and its orthography and we simulated sequential learning of the two orthographies with the CDP++ connectionist model of reading. We found that group differences in reading speed were comparatively bigger with Italian stimuli than with English stimuli. Furthermore, only Italian bilinguals took advantage of a blocked presentation of Italian stimuli compared to when stimuli from both languages were presented in mixed order, suggesting a greater ability to keep language-specific orthographic representations segregated. These findings demonstrate orthographic constraints on bilingual reading, whereby the level of consistency of the first learned orthography affects later learning and performance on a second orthography. The computer simulations were consistent with these conclusions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Showalter, Catherine E. "Impact of Cyrillic on Native English Speakers’ Phono-lexical Acquisition of Russian." Language and Speech 61, no. 4 (March 26, 2018): 565–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0023830918761489.

Full text
Abstract:
We investigated the influence of grapheme familiarity and native language grapheme–phoneme correspondences during second language lexical learning. Native English speakers learned Russian-like words via auditory presentations containing only familiar first language phones, pictured meanings, and exposure to either Cyrillic orthographic forms (Orthography condition) or the sequence <XXX> (No Orthography condition). Orthography participants saw three types of written forms: familiar-congruent (e.g., <KOM>-[kom]), familiar-incongruent (e.g., <PAT>-[rɑt]), and unfamiliar (e.g., <ФИЛ>-[fil]). At test, participants determined whether pictures and words matched according to what they saw during word learning. All participants performed near ceiling in all stimulus conditions, except for Orthography participants on words containing incongruent grapheme–phoneme correspondences. These results suggest that first language grapheme–phoneme correspondences can cause interference during second language phono-lexical acquisition. In addition, these results suggest that orthographic input effects are robust enough to interfere even when the input does not contain novel phones.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Pletneva, Alexandra. "Советская орфография: предыстория, программы, реализация." Fontes Slaviae Orthodoxae 1, no. 1 (February 12, 2019): 109–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.31648/fso.3047.

Full text
Abstract:
The article is devoted to the orthographic reform of 1917-1918 and the cultural and historical context in which it was prepared. The era between the reign of Alexander II and the revolution is characterized by a broad public debate about a wide range of reforms. During these decades, a mass of various reform projects appeared. It is in this context that projects of simplification of Russian orthography should also be considered. The issue of simplification of orthography was first raised by the teachers, complaining that teaching children orthographic rules took almost the entire academic time. In the course of this discussion, positions of both reform advocates and their opponents were formed. Then public and academic institutions developing the reform project began to arise. The projects of “simplified” orthography, which resulted from the reform”, in practice meant its approach to a variety of "national" orthography systems. In the first years after the revolution, many of the projects discussed before the revolution were realized. The orthographic reform was among them. The new norm was much more rigid and brooked no exceptions, unlike the pre-reform one.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Orthography"

1

Zheltukhin, Alexander. "Orthographic codes and code-switching : a study in 16th century Swedish orthography /." Stockholm : Almqvist & Wiksell, 1996. http://catalogue.bnf.fr/ark:/12148/cb37164838m.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Herring, Jon Russell. "Orthography and the lexicon." Thesis, University of Brighton, 2006. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.439207.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Tadich, Matthew A. "The orthography of mstislavovo evangelie /." The Ohio State University, 1996. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1487940308433493.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Matsumoto-Sturt, Yoko. "Second language acquisition of Japanese orthography." Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 2004. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/24918.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this thesis is to address issues on second language (L2) acquisition of Japanese orthography. In particular, I investigate L2 acquisition of reading and writing in Japanese with special attention paid to how linguistic and perceptual difficulties influence lexical processes involved in reading and writing by English-speaking learners of Japanese. I first establish that L2 spelling problems are found in writing by keyboard. Evidence is presented from error patterns of L2 language used in a small self-constructed L2 error corpus that was gathered according to originally set external criteria. Next, I demonstrate that there are predictable error causes as well as general linguistic and perceptual problems among L2 writers. Real-time observational data illustrates how and when L2 writers make spelling mistakes with the keyboard, and an analysis of verbal protocol data reveals L2 writing strategies. Third, I turn to the domain of L2 perception and L1-specific listening strategies. I show the different patterns of learners’ perceived rhythmic units in terms of L2 Moraic Awareness of Japanese words, and determine the extent of the use of L1-specific listening strategies. Fourth, I provide a general picture of Interlanguage (IL) lexical representations in reading and spelling. Evidence from novel and existing experimental work is presented which shows that L2 writer’s linguistic problems are reflected in their written products. I present an account of a typical learner strategy of sub-lexical reading and writing. Finally, in the domain of visual kanji recognition, visual attention is addressed. An originally defined phenomenon of ‘kanji illusion’ leads to the interesting result that linguistic factors are not solely responsible for failures to notice kanji errors. This represents a new kind of explanation for L2 kanji reading difficulties, from a psycholinguistic perspective.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Schlapp, Ursula. "Phonology and orthography in word recognition." Thesis, University of Nottingham, 1989. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.235989.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Erdener, Vahit Dogu, University of Western Sydney, of Arts Education and Social Sciences College, and School of Psychology. "The effect of auditory, visual and orthographic information on second language acquisition." THESIS_CAESS_PSY_Erdener_V.xml, 2002. http://handle.uws.edu.au:8081/1959.7/685.

Full text
Abstract:
The current study investigates the effect of auditory and visual speech information and orthographic information on second/foreign language (L2) acquisition. To test this, native speakers of Turkish (a language with a transparent orthography) and native speakers of Australian English (a language with an opaque orthography) were exposed to Spanish (transparent orthography) and Irish (opaque orthography) legal non-word items in four experimental conditions: auditory-only, auditory-visual, auditory-orthographic, and auditory-visual-orthographic. On each trial, Turkish and Australian English speakers were asked to produce each Spanish and Irish legal non-words. In terms of phoneme errors it was found that Turkish participants generally made less errors in Spanish than their Australian counterparts, and visual speech information generally facilitated performance. Orthographic information had an overriding effect such that there was no visual advantage once it was provided. In the orthographic conditions, Turkish speakers performed better than their Australian English counterparts with Spanish items and worse with Irish terms. In terms of native speakers' ratings of participants' productions, it was found that orthographic input improved accent. Overall the results confirm findings that visual information enhances speech production in L2 and additionally show the facilitative effects of orthographic input in L2 acquisition as a function of orthographic depth. Inter-rater reliability measures revealed that the native speaker rating procedure may be prone to individual and socio-cultural influences that may stem from internal criteria for native accents. This suggests that native speaker ratings should be treated with caution.
Master of Arts (Hons)
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Hashabeiky, Forogh. "Persian Orthography : Modification or Changeover? (1850-2000)." Doctoral thesis, Uppsala : Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis, : Uppsala University Library [distributör], 2005. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-5784.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Older, Lianne Jannice Elizabeth. "Morphology and orthography in the mental lexicon." Thesis, Birkbeck (University of London), 1998. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.298189.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Raman, Ilhan. "Single-word naming in a transparent alphabetic orthography." Thesis, Middlesex University, 1999. http://eprints.mdx.ac.uk/6754/.

Full text
Abstract:
The cognitive processes involved in single-word naming of the transparent Turkish orthography were examined in a series of nine naming experiments on adult native readers. In Experiment 1, a significant word frequency effect was observed when matched (i.e. on initial phoneme, letter length and number of syllables) high- and low-frequency words were presented for naming. However, no frequency effect was found in Experiment 2, when an equal number of matched (i.e. on initial phoneme, letter length and number of syllables) nonword fillers were mixed with the target words. A null frequency effect was also found in Experiment 3 when conditions were mixed-blocks, i.e. high- and low frequency were words presented in separate blocks mixed with an equal number of matched nonword fillers. Experiment 4 served the purpose of creating and validating nonwords (to be used in Experiments 5 and 6) that could be named as fast as high- and low-frequency words by manipulating the letter length of nonwords. A significant word frequency effect emerged with both the mixed-block design (Experiment 5) and mixed design (Experiment 6) when the nonword fillers matched the target words in speed of naming. Experiment 7, however, found no frequency effect when high- and low-frequency words were mixed with word fillers that were slower to be named (longer in length) than the target words. In Experiment 8, frequency was factorially manipulated with imageability (high vs. low) and level of skill (very skilled vs. skilled) which found significant main effects for word frequency and level of skill, and a significant 2-way interaction of skill by imageability and a significant 3-way interaction of skill by imageability by frequency. In Experiment 9, however, there was only a main effect for frequency when previously skilled readers performed on the same words used in Experiment 8. These findings suggest that whilst a lexical route dominates in naming the transparent Turkish orthography, an explanation that the readers shut down the operation of this route in the presence of nonword fillers is not entertained. Instead, the results suggest that both routes operate in naming, with the inclusion of filler stimuli and their “perceived difficulty” having an impact in the time criterion for articulation. Moreover, there are indications that a semantic route is involved in naming Turkish only when level of skill is taken into account. Implications of these findings for models of single-word naming are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Patel, Tanya Kamroon. "Individual differences in learning to read in English and Dutch children." Thesis, University of York, 2001. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.250620.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Books on the topic "Orthography"

1

Nunn, Anneke M. Dutch orthography. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics, 1998.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

University of Malawi. Centre for Language Studies. Citumbuka orthography. Chileka, Malawi: University of Malawi, Centre for Language Studies, 2005.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kiraz, George Anton. Syriac orthography. Piscataway, NJ: Gorgias Press, 2012.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Luelsdorff, Philip. Developmental orthography. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins Pub. Co., 1991.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Philip, Luelsdorff, ed. Orthography and phonology. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins Pub. Co., 1987.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Luelsdorff, Philip A., ed. Orthography and Phonology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1987. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/z.29.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Aye, Efiong U. Modern Efịk orthography. Calabar, Cross River State, Nigeria: Wusen Publishers, 2004.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Board, Chichewa, ed. Chicheŵa orthography rules. [Zomba, Malawi]: The Board, 1990.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Luelsdorff, Philip A. Principles of orthography. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 1990.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Luelsdorff, Philip A. Parameters of orthography. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 1991.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Orthography"

1

Luelsdorff, Philip A. "Orthographic Complexity and Orthography Acquisition." In Learning, Keeping and Using Language, 353–78. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/z.lkul1.25lue.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Cantino, Philip D., and Kevin de Queiroz**. "Orthography." In International Code of Phylogenetic Nomenclature (PhyloCode), 83–88. Version 6. | Boca Raton : CRC Press, 2020. | Ratified on January 20, 2019, by the Committee on Phylogenetic Nomenclature, of the International Society for Phylogenetic Nomenclature: CRC Press, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/9780429446320-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Hinchliffe, Ian, and Philip Holmes. "Orthography." In Swedish, 190–92. Third edition. | New York, NY : Routledge, 2020. | Series: Routledge essential grammars: Routledge, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315559131-15.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Nwachukwu, P. Akụjụọobi. "Orthography." In Towards an Igbo Literary Standard, 16–35. London: Routledge, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003209553-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Buszkowski, Wojciech, and Philip A. Luelsdorff. "2. A formal approach to error taxonomy." In Developmental Orthography, 19. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/z.51.05bus.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Luelsdorff, Philip A., and E. Ann Eyland. "5. The complexity hypothesis and graphemic ambiguity." In Developmental Orthography, 69. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/z.51.08lue.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Luelsdorff, Philip A., and E. Ann Eyland. "7. Psycholinguistic determinants of orthography acquisition." In Developmental Orthography, 109. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/z.51.10lue.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Luelsdorff, Philip A., and E. Ann Eyland. "8. Developmental morphographemics." In Developmental Orthography, 135. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/z.51.11lue.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Luelsdorff, Philip A., and E. Ann Eyland. "10. A psycholinguistic model of the bilingual speller." In Developmental Orthography, 187. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/z.51.13lue.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Nuessel, Frank. "Deviant Orthography." In International Handbook of Semiotics, 291–301. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9404-6_12.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Orthography"

1

Zeljić, Goran. "Kritički pogled na vrste pravopisnih vežbi u nastavi srpskog jezika." In Nauka, nastava, učenje u izmenjenom društvenom kontekstu. University of Kragujevac, Faculty of Education in Uzice, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.46793/nnu21.343z.

Full text
Abstract:
The article analyzes orthographic tasks that are part of teaching content in Serbian orthography at the elementary school age. These contents, especially at the younger school age, are an essential part of teaching Serbian. The plan and program cover all major orhographic topics, starting with the use of capital letters, through merged and conquered spelling, punctuation, and abbreviations. The orthographic aspect of voice alternatives is given in the older classes, although the basics of these rules are given at a younger age in changing the form of words (in the writing of nouns of the vrabac – vrapca type, mn. vrapci, zadatak, zadatka, zadaci, etc.) in word formation (eg. in the construction of deminitives such as sveska – sveščica, noga – nožica, etc.), and in highlighting exceptions such as the absence of alternation in loudness in the contact of sound consonants d and đ with silent consonants s and š (e.g., predsednik, predškolski, etc.). The aim of this paper is to examine the types and quality of exercises and tasks in Serbian orthography at primary school age. The analysis covers orthography units presented in textbooks and orthography literature and additional material used in teaching practice (task collections, etc.). Also, the orthographic tasks given in the Serbian language tests at different levels of competitions were examined.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Angelov, Krasimir. "Orthography Engineering in Grammatical Framework." In Proceedings of the Grammar Engineering Across Frameworks (GEAF) 2015 Workshop. Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/w15-3305.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Adam, Gabriela‑Violeta. "Romanian toponymy in Szabó T. Attila’s works. Solnoc-Dăbâca county. I. Vocalism." In International Conference on Onomastics “Name and Naming”. Editura Mega, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.30816/iconn5/2019/29.

Full text
Abstract:
The Magyarization of Romanian toponymy in historical Transylvania was achieved in three different ways: 1) adapting the onomastic material to the Hungarian orthographic and phonetic system; 2) translating the toponymic items; 3) adopting the specific Hungarian morphosyntactic rules. The Magyarization of microtoponymy did not have repercussions on the morphosyntactic and lexical levels; the adoption of Hungarian orthography ensured only the formal assimilation of the toponyms. The Hungarian orthographic principles and norms, used inconsistently, reflect numerous oscillating contexts in which the sounds ă, î, u have as graphic correspondents both labial and non-labial vowels. The Magyarization of Romanian toponymy in historical Transylvania did not obscure specific dialectal features, which highlight important information on the age and strata of populations and the relationships among them
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Smirnov, Yury V. "Typing circumflexes in search box in multilingual digital dictionaries (as exemplified by the digital abbreviation dictionary and 25 European languages)2." In Twenty Fourth International Conference "Information technologies, computer systems and publications for libraries". Russian National Public Library for Science and Technology, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.33186/978-5-85638-231-9-2020-101-104.

Full text
Abstract:
Entering circumflexes which are lacking on Russian and English language keyboard traditionally used in Russia is rather incovenient. As the example, the interface of digital abbreviation dictionary comprising 25 languages is described; the additional symbol bar is used for the purpose. Special attention is given to the features of the Modern Greek language where, along with circumflex input problem, the problems of orthography (two literary forms in use, orthographic complexities) are faced. The solution as appears in the abbreviations digital dictionary is presented. The author concludes that user-friendly solution for entering lacking symbols has to be found.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Kozmács, István, and Ildikó Vančo. "PROBLEMS OF ORTHOGRAPHY AND READING COMPREHENSION." In 13th International Conference on Education and New Learning Technologies. IATED, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.21125/edulearn.2021.0584.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Saadane, Houda, and Nizar Habash. "A Conventional Orthography for Algerian Arabic." In Proceedings of the Second Workshop on Arabic Natural Language Processing. Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/w15-3208.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Tseng, Yu-Hsiang, and Shu-Kai Hsieh. "Eigencharacter: An Embedding of Chinese Character Orthography." In Proceedings of the Beyond Vision and LANguage: inTEgrating Real-world kNowledge (LANTERN). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.18653/v1/d19-6404.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Buckwalter, Tim. "Issues in Arabic orthography and morphology analysis." In the Workshop. Morristown, NJ, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3115/1621804.1621813.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Nicolai, Garrett, and Grzegorz Kondrak. "English orthography is not "close to optimal"." In Proceedings of the 2015 Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies. Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.3115/v1/n15-1056.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Bailly, Gérard, and Will Barbour. "Synchronous reading: learning French orthography by audiovisual training." In Interspeech 2011. ISCA: ISCA, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2011-342.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Reports on the topic "Orthography"

1

Kanatani, Ken-Ichi. Analysis of Structure and Motion from Optical Flow. Part 1. Orthographic Projection. Revision. Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, June 1985. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada171612.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Dukart, Carla. A Comparison of Speech Intelligibility Measures between Unsophisticated Listener Judgements and Orthographic Transcription. Portland State University Library, January 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.15760/etd.7130.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Mowe, Kristi. Comparison of Intelligibility Estimation and Orthographic Transcription Methods by Preprofessional Speech-Language Pathologists. Portland State University Library, January 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.15760/etd.7262.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Sugarman, Nancy. A Comparison Between Trained Ear Estimation and Orthographic Transcription When Measuring Speech Intelligibility of Young Children. Portland State University Library, January 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.15760/etd.6659.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography