To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Neuronal discrimination.

Journal articles on the topic 'Neuronal discrimination'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Neuronal discrimination.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Deng, Yingchun, Peter Williams, Feng Liu, and Jianfeng Feng. "Neuronal discrimination capacity." Journal of Physics A: Mathematical and General 36, no. 50 (December 1, 2003): 12379–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/0305-4470/36/50/003.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Boynton, Geoffrey M., Jonathan B. Demb, Gary H. Glover, and David J. Heeger. "Neuronal basis of contrast discrimination." Vision Research 39, no. 2 (January 1999): 257–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00113-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Heeger, D. J. "Neuronal correlates of contrast detection and discrimination." Journal of Vision 2, no. 10 (December 1, 2002): 13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/2.10.13.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Spitzer, H., R. Desimone, and J. Moran. "Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance." Science 240, no. 4850 (April 15, 1988): 338–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.3353728.

Full text
Abstract:
Single cells were recorded from cortical area V4 of two rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) trained on a visual discrimination task with two levels of difficulty. Behavioral evidence indicated that the monkeys' discriminative abilities improved when the task was made more difficult. Correspondingly, neuronal responses to stimuli became larger and more selective in the difficult task. A control experiment demonstrated that changes in general arousal could not account for the effects of task difficulty on neuronal responses. It is concluded that increasing the amount of attention directed toward a stimulus can enhance the responsiveness and selectivity of the neurons that process it.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Li, Wu, Peter Thier, and Christian Wehrhahn. "Contextual Influence on Orientation Discrimination of Humans and Responses of Neurons in V1 of Alert Monkeys." Journal of Neurophysiology 83, no. 2 (February 1, 2000): 941–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.2000.83.2.941.

Full text
Abstract:
We studied the effects of various patterns as contextual stimuli on human orientation discrimination, and on responses of neurons in V1 of alert monkeys. When a target line is presented along with various contextual stimuli (masks), human orientation discrimination is impaired. For most V1 neurons, responses elicited by a line in the receptive field (RF) center are suppressed by these contextual patterns. Orientation discrimination thresholds of human observers are elevated slightly when the target line is surrounded by orthogonal lines. For randomly oriented lines, thresholds are elevated further and even more so for lines parallel to the target. Correspondingly, responses of most V1 neurons to a line are suppressed. Although contextual lines inhibit the amplitude of orientation tuning functions of most V1 neurons, they do not systematically alter the tuning width. Elevation of human orientation discrimination thresholds decreases with increasing curvature of masking lines, so does the inhibition of V1 neuronal responses. A mask made of straight lines yields the strongest interference with human orientation discrimination and produces the strongest suppression of neuronal responses. Elevation of human orientation discrimination thresholds is highest when a mask covers only the immediate vicinity of the target line. Increasing the masking area results in less interference. On the contrary, suppression of neuronal responses in V1 increases with increasing mask size. Our data imply that contextual interference observed in human orientation discrimination is in part directly related to contextual inhibition of neuronal activity in V1. However, the finding that interference with orientation discrimination is weaker for larger masks suggests a figure-ground segregation process that is not located in V1.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Adibi, Mehdi, and Ehsan Arabzadeh. "A Comparison of Neuronal and Behavioral Detection and Discrimination Performances in Rat Whisker System." Journal of Neurophysiology 105, no. 1 (January 2011): 356–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00794.2010.

Full text
Abstract:
We used the rat whisker touch as a model system to investigate the correlation between the response function of cortical neurons and the behavior of rats in a sensory detection versus discrimination task. The rat whisker–barrel system is structurally well characterized and represents one of the main channels through which rodents collect information about the environment. In experiment 1, we recorded neuronal activity ( n = 235) in the whisker area of the rat somatosensory cortex in anesthetized rats while applying vibrotactile stimuli of varying amplitudes to the whiskers. Neurons showed a characteristic sigmoidal input–output function, with an accelerating nonlinearity at low stimulus amplitudes and a compressive nonlinearity at high stimulus amplitudes. We further quantified the performance of individual neurons for stimulus detection and for discrimination across different stimulus pairs with identical amplitude differences. For near-threshold stimuli, the neuronal discrimination performance surpassed the detection performance despite the fact that detection and discrimination represented identical amplitude differences. This is consistent with the accelerating nonlinearity observed at low stimulus intensities. In the second stage of the experiment, four rats were trained to select the higher-amplitude stimulus between two vibrations applied to their whiskers. Similar to neuronal results, the rats' performance was better for the discrimination task compared with the detection task. The behavioral performance followed the same trend as that of the population of individual neurons. Both behavioral and neuronal data are consistent with the “pedestal effect” previously reported in human psychophysics.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Matsumora, Takehiro, Kowa Koida, and Hidehiko Komatsu. "Relationship Between Color Discrimination and Neural Responses in the Inferior Temporal Cortex of the Monkey." Journal of Neurophysiology 100, no. 6 (December 2008): 3361–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.90551.2008.

Full text
Abstract:
Earlier studies suggest that the inferior temporal (IT) cortex of the monkey plays a key role in color discrimination. Here, we examined the quantitative relationship between color judgment in monkeys and the responses of color-selective neurons in the anterior part of the IT cortex (area TE) by comparing neuronal activity and behavior recorded simultaneously while the monkeys performed a color-judgment task. We first compared the abilities of single neurons and monkeys to discriminate color. To calculate a neuron's ability to discriminate color, we computed a neurometric function using receiver-operating-characteristics analysis. We then compared the neural and behavioral thresholds for color discrimination and found that, in general, the neural threshold was higher than the behavioral threshold, although occasionally the reverse was true. Variation in the neural threshold across the color space corresponded well with that of the behavioral threshold. We then calculated the choice probability (CP), which is a measure of the correlation between the trial-to-trial fluctuations in neuronal responses and the monkeys' color judgment. On average, CPs were slightly but significantly greater than 0.5, indicating the activities of these TE neurons correlate positively with the monkeys' color judgment. This suggests that individual color-selective TE neurons only weakly contribute to color discrimination and that a large population of color-selective TE neurons contribute to the performance of color discrimination.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Orban, Guy A., and Rufin Vogels. "The neuronal machinery involved in successive orientation discrimination." Progress in Neurobiology 55, no. 2 (June 1998): 117–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00010-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Arabzadeh, Ehsan, Colin W. G. Clifford, Justin A. Harris, David A. Mahns, Vaughan G. Macefield, and Ingvars Birznieks. "Single tactile afferents outperform human subjects in a vibrotactile intensity discrimination task." Journal of Neurophysiology 112, no. 10 (November 15, 2014): 2382–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00482.2014.

Full text
Abstract:
We simultaneously compared the sensitivity of single primary afferent neurons supplying the glabrous skin of the hand and the psychophysical amplitude discrimination thresholds in human subjects for a set of vibrotactile stimuli delivered to the receptive field. All recorded afferents had a dynamic range narrower than the range of amplitudes across which the subjects could discriminate. However, when the vibration amplitude was chosen to be within the steepest part of the afferent's stimulus-response function the response of single afferents, defined as the spike count over the vibration duration (500 ms), was often more sensitive in discriminating vibration amplitude than the perceptual judgment of the participants. We quantified how the neuronal performance depended on the integration window: for short windows the neuronal performance was inferior to the performance of the subject. The neuronal performance progressively improved with increasing spike count duration and reached a level significantly above that of the subjects when the integration window was 250 ms or longer. The superiority in performance of individual neurons over observers could reflect a nonoptimal integration window or be due to the presence of noise between the sensory periphery and the cortical decision stage. Additionally, it could indicate that the range of perceptual sensitivity comes at the cost of discrimination through pooling across neurons with different response functions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Smith, Jackson E. T., and Andrew J. Parker. "Correlated structure of neuronal firing in macaque visual cortex limits information for binocular depth discrimination." Journal of Neurophysiology 126, no. 1 (July 1, 2021): 275–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00667.2020.

Full text
Abstract:
Correlated noise reduces the stimulus information in visual cortical neurons during experimental performance of binocular depth discriminations. The temporal scale of these correlations is important. Rapid (20–30 ms) correlations reduce information within and between areas V1 and V4, whereas slow (>100 ms) correlations between areas do not. Separate cortical areas appear to act together to maintain signal fidelity. Rapid correlations reduce the neuronal signal difference between stimuli and adversely affect perceptual discrimination.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Sanders, Teresa H., Mark A. Clements, and Thomas Wichmann. "Parkinsonism-related features of neuronal discharge in primates." Journal of Neurophysiology 110, no. 3 (August 1, 2013): 720–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00672.2012.

Full text
Abstract:
Parkinson's disease is known to be associated with abnormal electrical spiking activities of basal ganglia neurons, including changes in firing rate, bursting activities and oscillatory firing patterns and changes in entropy. We explored the relative importance of these measures through optimal feature selection and discrimination analysis methods. We identified key characteristics of basal ganglia activity that predicted whether the neurons were recorded in the normal or parkinsonian state. Starting with 29 features extracted from the spike timing of neurons recorded in normal and parkinsonian monkeys in the internal or external segment of the globus pallidus or the subthalamic nucleus (STN), we used a method that incorporates a support vector machine algorithm to find feature combinations that optimally discriminate between the normal and parkinsonian states. Our results demonstrate that the discrimination power of combinations of specific features is higher than that of single features, or of all features combined, and that the most discriminative feature sets differ substantially between basal ganglia structures. Each nucleus or class of neurons in the basal ganglia may react differently to the parkinsonian condition, and the features used to describe this state should be adapted to the neuron type under study. The feature that was overall most predictive of the parkinsonian state in our data set was a high STN intraburst frequency. Interestingly, this feature was not correlated with parameters describing oscillatory firing properties in recordings made in the normal condition but was significantly correlated with spectral power in specific frequency bands in recordings from the parkinsonian state (specifically with power in the 8–13 Hz band).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Brown, M. W., and Z. I. Bashir. "Evidence concerning how neurons of the perirhinal cortex may effect familiarity discrimination." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 357, no. 1424 (August 29, 2002): 1083–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1097.

Full text
Abstract:
Many studies indicate that recognition memory involves at least two separable processes, familiarity discrimination and recollection. Aspects of what is known of potential neuronal substrates of familiarity discrimination are reviewed. Lesion studies have established that familiarity discrimination for individual visual stimuli is effected by a system centred on the perirhinal cortex of the temporal lobe. The fundamental change that encodes prior occurrence of such stimuli appears to be a reduction in the response of neurons in anterior inferior temporal (including perirhinal) cortex when a stimulus is repeated. The neuronal responses rapidly signal the presence of a novel stimulus, and are evidence of long–lasting learning after a single exposure. Computational modelling indicates that a neuronal network based on such a change in responsiveness is potentially highly efficient in information theoretic terms. Processes that occur in long–term depression within the perirhinal cortex provide candidate synaptic plastic mechanisms for that underlying the change, but such linkage remains to be experimentally established.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Marshel, James H., Yoon Seok Kim, Timothy A. Machado, Sean Quirin, Brandon Benson, Jonathan Kadmon, Cephra Raja, et al. "Cortical layer–specific critical dynamics triggering perception." Science 365, no. 6453 (July 18, 2019): eaaw5202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.aaw5202.

Full text
Abstract:
Perceptual experiences may arise from neuronal activity patterns in mammalian neocortex. We probed mouse neocortex during visual discrimination using a red-shifted channelrhodopsin (ChRmine, discovered through structure-guided genome mining) alongside multiplexed multiphoton-holography (MultiSLM), achieving control of individually specified neurons spanning large cortical volumes with millisecond precision. Stimulating a critical number of stimulus-orientation-selective neurons drove widespread recruitment of functionally related neurons, a process enhanced by (but not requiring) orientation-discrimination task learning. Optogenetic targeting of orientation-selective ensembles elicited correct behavioral discrimination. Cortical layer–specific dynamics were apparent, as emergent neuronal activity asymmetrically propagated from layer 2/3 to layer 5, and smaller layer 5 ensembles were as effective as larger layer 2/3 ensembles in eliciting orientation discrimination behavior. Population dynamics emerging after optogenetic stimulation both correctly predicted behavior and resembled natural internal representations of visual stimuli at cellular resolution over volumes of cortex.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Tremblay, N., M. C. Bushnell, and G. H. Duncan. "Thalamic VPM nucleus in the behaving monkey. II. Response to air-puff stimulation during discrimination and attention tasks." Journal of Neurophysiology 69, no. 3 (March 1, 1993): 753–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1993.69.3.753.

Full text
Abstract:
1. Single-unit activity was recorded in the ventral posterior medial (VPM) thalamic nucleus of awake monkeys while they performed detection and discrimination tasks involving tactile air-puff stimuli presented to the face. Neuronal responsiveness was compared directly with the monkey's discriminative performance. In addition, neuronal activity was compared when the monkey's attention was directed to the air-puff stimulus and when it was directed to a concurrent visual stimulus. 2. Neurons responding to the air-puff stimuli were classified as slowly adapting (SA), rapidly adapting (RA), inhibitory (IN), or multimodal (MM), according to their responses to manual and thermal stimulation, as well as their adaption rates to the air puff. Of 47 neurons responsive to air-puff stimulation and studied extensively in the behavioral task, 14 were classified as RA, 15 as SA, 6 as IN, and 12 as MM. The 12 MM neurons were so classified because, in addition to air puff, they responded to noxious heat, innocuous cooling, or noxious pinch. 3. Neurons from each class had restricted contralateral receptive fields localized within one division of the trigeminal nerve. There was no systematic difference in receptive-field size among groups. 4. A prominent difference in tactile responsiveness of MM neurons was response latency. Although the mean latency for RA, SA, and IN neurons was not significantly different (6.1, 9.1, and 12.2 ms, respectively), the mean latency for MM neurons was significantly longer than that for each of the other neuronal categories (28.8 ms; Ps < 0.001). These data suggest that the excitatory tactile afferent input to MM neurons is different from that to low-threshold neurons. 5. For RA, SA, and MM neurons the frequency of the neuronal discharge evoked by the air-puff stimulation was proportional to the intensity of the air puff. Thus responses of each neuronal class coded air-puff stimulus intensity. 6. The monkeys' ability to detect air-puff stimuli of various intensities was compared with the frequency of neuronal responses to those stimuli. Both the percent success in detecting differences in air-puff intensity and the detection latency were highly correlated with neuronal response frequency. The responses of all three excitatory neuronal categories corresponded well with the monkey's performance. Thus any or all of RA, SA, and MM neurons could play a role in the discrimination of air-puff intensities.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

von Heimendahl, Moritz, Pavel M. Itskov, Ehsan Arabzadeh, and Mathew E. Diamond. "Neuronal Activity in Rat Barrel Cortex Underlying Texture Discrimination." PLoS Biology 5, no. 11 (November 13, 2007): e305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050305.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Rodríguez-Sanchez, Antonio J., John K. Tsotsos, Stefan Treue, and Julio C. Martinez-Trujillo. "Comparing neuronal and behavioral thresholds for spiral motion discrimination." NeuroReport 20, no. 18 (December 2009): 1619–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0b013e32833312c7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Schiff, Steven J., Tim Sauer, Rohit Kumar, and Steven L. Weinstein. "Neuronal spatiotemporal pattern discrimination: The dynamical evolution of seizures." NeuroImage 28, no. 4 (December 2005): 1043–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.059.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Zheng, Yi, Jianbo Gao, Justin C. Sanchez, Jose C. Principe, and Michael S. Okun. "Multiplicative multifractal modeling and discrimination of human neuronal activity." Physics Letters A 344, no. 2-4 (September 2005): 253–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physleta.2005.06.092.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Hernandez, A., A. Zainos, and R. Romo. "Neuronal correlates of sensory discrimination in the somatosensory cortex." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97, no. 11 (May 16, 2000): 6191–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.120018597.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Chiba, Atsushi, Ken-ichi Oshio, and Masahiko Inase. "Neuronal representation of duration discrimination in the monkey striatum." Physiological Reports 3, no. 2 (February 2015): e12283. http://dx.doi.org/10.14814/phy2.12283.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Nakao, M., K. Ohmura, and R. Sato. "Hardware-based adaptive system for discrimination of neuronal spikes." Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing 26, no. 4 (July 1988): 360–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02442292.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Zhou, Jingyang, and Chanwoo Chung. "How does perceptual discrimination relate to neuronal receptive fields?" Journal of Vision 23, no. 9 (August 1, 2023): 5708. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.23.9.5708.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

MERIGAN, WILLIAM H. "Cortical area V4 is critical for certain texture discriminations, but this effect is not dependent on attention." Visual Neuroscience 17, no. 6 (November 2000): 949–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s095252380017614x.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examined the question of which features of a complex grouping discrimination make it vulnerable to permanent elimination by V4 lesions. We first verified that the line element grouping discrimination, which we previously reported to be devastated by V4 lesions, was similarly affected in the monkeys of this study. The permanence of the deficit was established by mapping its visual field distribution and then testing this discrimination for an extended period at a locus on the border of the deficit. Also, a staircase procedure was used to provide the monkey with within session instruction in the grouping discrimination, but this did not improve V4 lesion performance. Grouping was then compared with several discriminations that shared some features with it, but which were found not to be permanently eliminated by V4 lesions. This comparison suggested that grouping (rather than segmentation or response to a single element) was one feature that made the discrimination vulnerable, a second was the similarity in shape of the texture elements to be grouped. Finally, we tested visual crowding, a property of peripheral vision that is thought to reflect neuronal interactions early in visual cortex, possibly in area V1, and found no effect of V4 lesions. A control experiment with human observers tested whether the elimination of grouping by V4 lesions might be due to an alteration of attention, but found no evidence to support this hypothesis. These results show that severe disruption of texture discriminations by V4 lesions depends on both the nature of the discrimination and the type of texture elements involved, but does not necessarily involve the disruption of attention.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Berberian, Nareg, Amanda MacPherson, Eloïse Giraud, Lydia Richardson, and J. P. Thivierge. "Neuronal pattern separation of motion-relevant input in LIP activity." Journal of Neurophysiology 117, no. 2 (February 1, 2017): 738–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00145.2016.

Full text
Abstract:
In various regions of the brain, neurons discriminate sensory stimuli by decreasing the similarity between ambiguous input patterns. Here, we examine whether this process of pattern separation may drive the rapid discrimination of visual motion stimuli in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP). Starting with a simple mean-rate population model that captures neuronal activity in LIP, we show that overlapping input patterns can be reformatted dynamically to give rise to separated patterns of neuronal activity. The population model predicts that a key ingredient of pattern separation is the presence of heterogeneity in the response of individual units. Furthermore, the model proposes that pattern separation relies on heterogeneity in the temporal dynamics of neural activity and not merely in the mean firing rates of individual neurons over time. We confirm these predictions in recordings of macaque LIP neurons and show that the accuracy of pattern separation is a strong predictor of behavioral performance. Overall, results propose that LIP relies on neuronal pattern separation to facilitate decision-relevant discrimination of sensory stimuli. NEW & NOTEWORTHY A new hypothesis is proposed on the role of the lateral intraparietal (LIP) region of cortex during rapid decision making. This hypothesis suggests that LIP alters the representation of ambiguous inputs to reduce their overlap, thus improving sensory discrimination. A combination of computational modeling, theoretical analysis, and electrophysiological data shows that the pattern separation hypothesis links neural activity to behavior and offers novel predictions on the role of LIP during sensory discrimination.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Hashimoto, Kosuke, Bibin B. Andriana, Hiroko Matsuyoshi, and Hidetoshi Sato. "Discrimination analysis of excitatory and inhibitory neurons using Raman spectroscopy." Analyst 143, no. 12 (2018): 2889–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1039/c8an00051d.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Heuer, Hilary W., and Kenneth H. Britten. "Optic Flow Signals in Extrastriate Area MST: Comparison of Perceptual and Neuronal Sensitivity." Journal of Neurophysiology 91, no. 3 (March 2004): 1314–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00637.2003.

Full text
Abstract:
The medial superior temporal area of extrastriate cortex (MST) contains signals selective for nonuniform patterns of motion often termed “optic flow.” The presence of such tuning, however, does not necessarily imply involvement in perception. To quantify the relationship between these selective neuronal signals and the perception of optic flow, we designed a discrimination task that allowed us to simultaneously record neuronal and behavioral sensitivities to near-threshold optic flow stimuli tailored to MST cells' preferences. In this two-alternative forced-choice task, we controlled the salience of globally opposite patterns (e.g., expansion and contraction) by varying the coherence of the motion. Using these stimuli, we could both relate the sensitivity of neuronal signals in MST to the animal's behavioral sensitivity and also measure trial-by-trial correlation between neuronal signals and behavioral choices. Neurons in MST showed a wide range of sensitivities to these complex motion stimuli. Many neurons had sensitivities equal or superior to the monkey's threshold. On the other hand, trial-by-trial correlation between neuronal discharge and choice (“choice probability”) was weak or nonexistent in our data. Together, these results lead us to conclude that MST contains sufficient information for threshold judgments of optic flow; however, the role of MST activity in optic flow discriminations may be less direct than in other visual motion tasks previously described by other laboratories.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Zhivago, Kalathupiriyan A., and Sripati P. Arun. "Texture discriminability in monkey inferotemporal cortex predicts human texture perception." Journal of Neurophysiology 112, no. 11 (December 1, 2014): 2745–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00532.2014.

Full text
Abstract:
Shape and texture are both important properties of visual objects, but texture is relatively less understood. Here, we characterized neuronal responses to discrete textures in monkey inferotemporal (IT) cortex and asked whether they can explain classic findings in human texture perception. We focused on three classic findings on texture discrimination: 1) it can be easy or hard depending on the constituent elements; 2) it can have asymmetries, and 3) it is reduced for textures with randomly oriented elements. We recorded neuronal activity from monkey inferotemporal (IT) cortex and measured texture perception in humans for a variety of textures. Our main findings are as follows: 1) IT neurons show congruent selectivity for textures across array size; 2) textures that were easy for humans to discriminate also elicited distinct patterns of neuronal activity in monkey IT; 3) texture pairs with asymmetries in humans also exhibited asymmetric variation in firing rate across monkey IT; and 4) neuronal responses to randomly oriented textures were explained by an average of responses to homogeneous textures, which rendered them less discriminable. The reduction in discriminability of monkey IT neurons predicted the reduced discriminability in humans during texture discrimination. Taken together, our results suggest that texture perception in humans is likely based on neuronal representations similar to those in monkey IT.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Brody, Carlos D., Adrián Hernández, Antonio Zainos, Luis Lemus, and Ranulfo Romo. "Analysing neuronal correlates of the comparison of two sequentially presented sensory stimuli." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 357, no. 1428 (December 29, 2002): 1843–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1167.

Full text
Abstract:
In a typical sequential sensory discrimination task, subjects are required to make a decision based on comparing a sensory stimulus against the memory trace left by a previous stimulus. What is the neuronal substrate for such comparisons and the resulting decisions? This question was studied by recording neuronal responses in a variety of cortical areas of awake monkeys ( Macaca mulatta ), trained to carry out a vibrotactile sequential discrimination task. We describe methods to analyse responses obtained during the comparison and decision phases of the task, and describe the resulting findings from recordings in secondary somatosensory cortical area (S2). A subset of neurons in S2 become highly correlated with the monkey's decision in the task.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Yu, Bo, Terrence Mak, Xiangyu Li, Leslie Smith, Yihe Sun, and Chi-Sang Poon. "Stream-based Hebbian Eigenfilter for real-time neuronal spike discrimination." BioMedical Engineering OnLine 11, no. 1 (2012): 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1475-925x-11-18.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Seiferth, Nina Y., Katharina Pauly, Thilo Kellermann, N. Jon Shah, Gudrun Ott, Beate Herpertz-Dahlmann, Tilo Kircher, Frank Schneider, and Ute Habel. "Neuronal Correlates of Facial Emotion Discrimination in Early Onset Schizophrenia." Neuropsychopharmacology 34, no. 2 (June 25, 2008): 477–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2008.93.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Romo, Ranulfo, Adrián Hernández, Antonio Zainos, and Emilio Salinas. "Correlated Neuronal Discharges that Increase Coding Efficiency during Perceptual Discrimination." Neuron 38, no. 4 (May 2003): 649–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00287-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Seiferth, N. Y., K. Pauly, V. Backes, T. Kellermann, U. Habel, N. J. Shah, S. Ruhrmann, B. Herpertz-Dahlmann, F. Schneider, and T. Kircher. "0356 EMOTION DISCRIMINATION AND ITS NEURONAL CORRELATES IN EARLY PSYCHOSIS." Schizophrenia Research 86 (October 2006): S72—S73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(06)70216-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Schaefer, Andreas T., Kamilla Angelo, Hartwig Spors, and Troy W. Margrie. "Neuronal Oscillations Enhance Stimulus Discrimination by Ensuring Action Potential Precision." PLoS Biology 4, no. 6 (May 16, 2006): e163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040163.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Altmann, L., H. J. Luhmann, J. M. Greuel, and W. Singer. "Functional and neuronal binocularity in kittens raised with rapidly alternating monocular occlusion." Journal of Neurophysiology 58, no. 5 (November 1, 1987): 965–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1987.58.5.965.

Full text
Abstract:
1. In order to determine the degree of synchrony of binocular activation required for the development of binocularity we reared 11 kittens with rapidly alternating monocular occlusion. Alternating occlusion was achieved with microprocessor-controlled electrooptic solid-state shutters, which were fitted to individually moulded goggles. The intervals of alternating occlusion were varied from 50 to 1,000 ms. Two normally reared kittens and three kittens that were reared with the shutters operating synchronously with open/close intervals of 50/50 ms, 200/200 ms, and 400/100 ms, respectively, were used as controls. Toward the end of the critical period we examined the kittens' ability for binocular depth discrimination and tested binocular luminance summation of the pupillary light reflex. Single-cell recordings were made from the visual cortex in order to determine the percentages of binocularly excitable neurons. 2. There was a good correlation between the degree of asynchrony of binocular experience, the impairment of depth discrimination, and the percentage of binocular neurons. Kittens reared with alternation rates of 200, 330, and 400 ms, respectively, had developed normal binocularity and were indistinguishable from the controls. Alternation rates of 500 ms or longer prevented the development of normal depth discrimination and luminance summation and resulted in reduced cortical binocularity. 3. A linear relationship between depth discrimination, binocular luminance summation, and percentages of binocular neurons was found. 4. Our findings indicate that an asynchrony of binocular activation of several hundred milliseconds is compatible with the development of normal binocularity in the kitten visual system.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Zorick, Todd, and Jason Smith. "Generalized Information Equilibrium Approaches to EEG Sleep Stage Discrimination." Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine 2016 (2016): 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/6450126.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent advances in neuroscience have raised the hypothesis that the underlying pattern of neuronal activation which results in electroencephalography (EEG) signals is via power-law distributed neuronal avalanches, while EEG signals are nonstationary. Therefore, spectral analysis of EEG may miss many properties inherent in such signals. A complete understanding of such dynamical systems requires knowledge of the underlying nonequilibrium thermodynamics. In recent work by Fielitz and Borchardt (2011, 2014), the concept of information equilibrium (IE) in information transfer processes has successfully characterized many different systems far from thermodynamic equilibrium. We utilized a publicly available database of polysomnogram EEG data from fourteen subjects with eight different one-minute tracings of sleep stage 2 and waking and an overlapping set of eleven subjects with eight different one-minute tracings of sleep stage 3. We applied principles of IE to model EEG as a system that transfers (equilibrates) information from the time domain to scalp-recorded voltages. We find that waking consciousness is readily distinguished from sleep stages 2 and 3 by several differences in mean information transfer constants. Principles of IE applied to EEG may therefore prove to be useful in the study of changes in brain function more generally.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Tramo, Mark Jude, Gaurav D. Shah, and Louis D. Braida. "Functional Role of Auditory Cortex in Frequency Processing and Pitch Perception." Journal of Neurophysiology 87, no. 1 (January 1, 2002): 122–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00104.1999.

Full text
Abstract:
Microelectrode studies in nonhuman primates and other mammals have demonstrated that many neurons in auditory cortex are excited by pure tone stimulation only when the tone's frequency lies within a narrow range of the audible spectrum. However, the effects of auditory cortex lesions in animals and humans have been interpreted as evidence against the notion that neuronal frequency selectivity is functionally relevant to frequency discrimination. Here we report psychophysical and anatomical evidence in favor of the hypothesis that fine-grained frequency resolution at the perceptual level relies on neuronal frequency selectivity in auditory cortex. An adaptive procedure was used to measure difference thresholds for pure tone frequency discrimination in five humans with focal brain lesions and eight normal controls. Only the patient with bilateral lesions of primary auditory cortex and surrounding areas showed markedly elevated frequency difference thresholds: Weber fractions for frequency direction discrimination (“higher”—“lower” pitch judgments) were about eightfold higher than Weber fractions measured in patients with unilateral lesions of auditory cortex, auditory midbrain, or dorsolateral frontal cortex; Weber fractions for frequency change discrimination (“same”—“different” pitch judgments) were about seven times higher. In contrast, pure-tone detection thresholds, difference thresholds for pure tone duration discrimination centered at 500 ms, difference thresholds for vibrotactile intensity discrimination, and judgments of visual line orientation were within normal limits or only mildly impaired following bilateral auditory cortex lesions. In light of current knowledge about the physiology and anatomy of primate auditory cortex and a review of previous lesion studies, we interpret the present results as evidence that fine-grained frequency processing at the perceptual level relies on the integrity of finely tuned neurons in auditory cortex.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Gschwend, Olivier, Nixon M. Abraham, Samuel Lagier, Frédéric Begnaud, Ivan Rodriguez, and Alan Carleton. "Neuronal pattern separation in the olfactory bulb improves odor discrimination learning." Nature Neuroscience 18, no. 10 (August 24, 2015): 1474–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.4089.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Mountcastle, V. B., and M. A. Steinmetz. "Cortical Neuronal Periodicities and Frequency Discrimination in the Sense of Flutter." Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology 55 (January 1, 1990): 861–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.081.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Tsunada, Joji, and Toshiyuki Sawaguchi. "Neuronal Categorization and Discrimination of Social Behaviors in Primate Prefrontal Cortex." PLoS ONE 7, no. 12 (December 28, 2012): e52610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052610.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Dragoi, Valentin, Jitendra Sharma, Earl K. Miller, and Mriganka Sur. "Dynamics of neuronal sensitivity in visual cortex and local feature discrimination." Nature Neuroscience 5, no. 9 (August 5, 2002): 883–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn900.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Li Hegner, Yiwen, Ralf Saur, Ralf Veit, Raymond Butts, Susanne Leiberg, Wolfgang Grodd, and Christoph Braun. "BOLD Adaptation in Vibrotactile Stimulation: Neuronal Networks Involved in Frequency Discrimination." Journal of Neurophysiology 97, no. 1 (January 2007): 264–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00617.2006.

Full text
Abstract:
The present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study investigated human brain regions subserving the discrimination of vibrotactile frequency. An event-related adaptation paradigm was used in which blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) responses are lower to same compared with different pairs of stimuli (BOLD adaptation). This adaptation effect serves as an indicator for feature-specific responding of neuronal subpopulations. Subjects had to discriminate two vibrotactile stimuli sequentially applied with a delay of 600 ms to their left middle fingertip. The stimulus frequency was in the flutter range of 18–26 Hz. In half of the trials, the two stimuli possessed identical frequency (same), whereas in the other half, a frequency difference of ±2 Hz was used (diff). As a result, BOLD adaptation was observed in the contralateral primary somatosensory cortex (S1), precentral gyrus, superior temporal gyrus (STG); ipsilateral insula as well as bilateral secondary somatosensory cortex and supplementary motor area. When statistically comparing the BOLD time courses between same and diff trials in these cortical areas, it was found that the vibrotactile BOLD adaptation is initiated in the contralateral S1 and STG simultaneously. These findings suggest that the cortical areas responsive to the frequency difference between two serially presented stimuli sequentially process the frequency of a vibrotactile stimulus and constitute a putative neuronal network underlying human vibrotactile frequency discrimination.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Dragoi MIT, V., J. Sharma, E. K. Miller, and M. Sur. "Dynamics of neuronal sensitivity in primate V1 underlying local feature discrimination." Journal of Vision 2, no. 7 (March 15, 2010): 126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/2.7.126.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Allegra, Manuela, Lorenzo Posani, Ruy Gómez-Ocádiz, and Christoph Schmidt-Hieber. "Differential Relation between Neuronal and Behavioral Discrimination during Hippocampal Memory Encoding." Neuron 108, no. 6 (December 2020): 1103–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.09.032.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

van Veelen, C. W. M., G. Rijksen, and G. E. J. Staal. "Discrimination between neuronal and glial cell tumours by pyruvate kinase electrophoresis." Acta Neurochirurgica 91, no. 3-4 (September 1988): 126–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf01424567.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Spengler, Friederike, Timothy P.L. Roberts, David Poeppel, Nancy Byl, Xiaoqin Wang, Howard A. Rowley, and Mike M. Merzenich. "Learning transfer and neuronal plasticity in humans trained in tactile discrimination." Neuroscience Letters 232, no. 3 (September 1997): 151–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3940(97)00602-2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Schul, J. "Neuronal basis for spectral song discrimination in the bushcricket Tettigonia cantans." Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 184, no. 4 (May 10, 1999): 457–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s003590050345.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Folarin, R., S. John, O. Oyenuga, N. Tijani, O. Otulana, and E. Mbonu. "Olfacto-protective roles of Nigella sativa oil in Harmaline-induced essential tremor modelling." Annals of Health Research 6, no. 2 (May 17, 2020): 218–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.30442/ahr.0602-11-84.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: Harmaline is a tremorgenic beta-carboline, reported to induce acute postural and kinetic tremor. Essential Tremor (ET) is an idiopathic slowly neurodegenerative tremor disorder which also compromises olfactory acuity. Nigella sativa (NS) is a therapeutic agent widely used in the treatment of various ailments. Objective: To determine the effect of NSon olfactory functions of mice treated with harmaline. Methods: Seventy-five BALB/c male mice weighing 20g-25g, were equally divided into five groups, namely CNTRL (received only Normal saline), NS (received NS oil1ml/kg), HML(received Harmaline 20mg/kg), HNS (received Harmaline and Nigella sativa concurrently), and NSH (received NSfollowed by Harmaline). Olfactory sensitivity and discrimination were assayed through buried food test. The olfactory bulb was assayed neurochemically for glutamate and dopamine, and histologically for neuronal architecture using haematoxylin and eosin stain. Differences in neurochemical and histological data, body weight, appetite, relative brain weight, sensitivity and discrimination indices were statistically analysed. Results: NS was significantly protective against the negative effects of Harmaline. It also effected quick olfactory discrimination, increased dopamine level, decrease in weight difference and increased food consumption in the animals. However, Harmaline increased relative brain weight and GPX levels. The concurrent administration aided in the reduction of neuronal density while neuronal average size reduced on pre-treatment with NS. Conclusion: Harmaline did not induce tremor in the animals, though it resulted in histological and neurochemical deficits. However, it resulted in olfactory insensitivity and indiscrimination, both of which were prevented and ameliorated by Nigella sativa oil.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Wiest, Michael C., Eric Thomson, Janaina Pantoja, and Miguel A. L. Nicolelis. "Changes in S1 Neural Responses During Tactile Discrimination Learning." Journal of Neurophysiology 104, no. 1 (July 2010): 300–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00194.2010.

Full text
Abstract:
In freely moving rats that are actively performing a discrimination task, single-unit responses in primary somatosensory cortex (S1) are strikingly different from responses to comparable tactile stimuli in immobile rats. For example, in the active discrimination context prestimulus response modulations are common, responses are longer in duration and more likely to be inhibited. To determine whether these differences emerge as rats learned a whisker-dependent discrimination task, we recorded single-unit S1 activity while rats learned to discriminate aperture-widths using their whiskers. Even before discrimination training began, S1 responses in freely moving rats showed many of the signatures of active responses, such as increased duration of response and prestimulus response modulations. As rats subsequently learned the discrimination task, single unit responses changed: more cortical units responded to the stimuli, neuronal sensory responses grew in duration, and individual neurons better predicted aperture-width. In summary, the operant behavioral context changes S1 tactile responses even in the absence of tactile discrimination, whereas subsequent width discrimination learning refines the S1 representation of aperture-width.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Komoltsev, Ilia, Olga Salyp, Aleksandra Volkova, Daria Bashkatova, Natalia Shirobokova, Stepan Frankevich, Daria Shalneva, et al. "Posttraumatic and Idiopathic Spike–Wave Discharges in Rats: Discrimination by Morphology and Thalamus Involvement." Neurology International 15, no. 2 (April 27, 2023): 609–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/neurolint15020038.

Full text
Abstract:
The possibility of epileptiform activity generation by the thalamocortical neuronal network after focal brain injuries, including traumatic brain injury (TBI), is actively debated. Presumably, posttraumatic spike–wave discharges (SWDs) involve a cortico-thalamocortical neuronal network. Differentiation of posttraumatic and idiopathic (i.e., spontaneously generated) SWDs is imperative for understanding posttraumatic epileptogenic mechanisms. Experiments were performed on male Sprague-Dawley rats with electrodes implanted into the somatosensory cortex and the thalamic ventral posterolateral nucleus. Local field potentials were recorded for 7 days before and 7 days after TBI (lateral fluid percussion injury, 2.5 atm). The morphology of 365 SWDs (89 idiopathic before craniotomy, and 262 posttraumatic that appeared only after TBI) and their appearance in the thalamus were analyzed. The occurrence of SWDs in the thalamus determined their spike–wave form and bilateral lateralization in the neocortex. Posttraumatic discharges were characterized by more “mature” characteristics as compared to spontaneously generated discharges: higher proportions of bilateral spreading, well-defined spike–wave form, and thalamus involvement. Based on SWD parameters, the etiology could be established with an accuracy of 75% (AUC 0.79). Our results support the hypothesis that the formation of posttraumatic SWDs involves a cortico-thalamocortical neuronal network. The results form a basis for further research of mechanisms associated with posttraumatic epileptiform activity and epileptogenesis.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Groschner, Lukas N., and Gero Miesenböck. "Mechanisms of Sensory Discrimination: Insights from Drosophila Olfaction." Annual Review of Biophysics 48, no. 1 (May 6, 2019): 209–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-biophys-052118-115655.

Full text
Abstract:
All an animal can do to infer the state of its environment is to observe the sensory-evoked activity of its own neurons. These inferences about the presence, quality, or similarity of objects are probabilistic and inform behavioral decisions that are often made in close to real time. Neural systems employ several strategies to facilitate sensory discrimination: Biophysical mechanisms separate the neuronal response distributions in coding space, compress their variances, and combine information from sequential observations. We review how these strategies are implemented in the olfactory system of the fruit fly. The emerging principles of odor discrimination likely apply to other neural circuits of similar architecture.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography