Academic literature on the topic 'Motor theory of speech perception'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Motor theory of speech perception.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Motor theory of speech perception"

1

Massaro, Dominic W., and Trevor H. Chen. "The motor theory of speech perception revisited." Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15, no. 2 (April 2008): 453–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.2.453.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Galantucci, Bruno, Carol A. Fowler, and M. T. Turvey. "The motor theory of speech perception reviewed." Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 13, no. 3 (June 2006): 361–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193857.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Liberman, Alvin M., and Ignatius G. Mattingly. "The motor theory of speech perception revised." Cognition 21, no. 1 (October 1985): 1–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90021-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Stasenko, Alena, Frank E. Garcea, and Bradford Z. Mahon. "What happens to the motor theory of perception when the motor system is damaged?" Language and Cognition 5, no. 2-3 (September 2013): 225–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/langcog-2013-0016.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractMotor theories of perception posit that motor information is necessary for successful recognition of actions. Perhaps the most well known of this class of proposals is the motor theory of speech perception, which argues that speech recognition is fundamentally a process of identifying the articulatory gestures (i.e. motor representations) that were used to produce the speech signal. Here we review neuropsychological evidence from patients with damage to the motor system, in the context of motor theories of perception applied to both manual actions and speech. Motor theories of perception predict that patients with motor impairments will have impairments for action recognition. Contrary to that prediction, the available neuropsychological evidence indicates that recognition can be spared despite profound impairments to production. These data falsify strong forms of the motor theory of perception, and frame new questions about the dynamical interactions that govern how information is exchanged between input and output systems.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Fox, Robert Allen. "Modularity and the Motor Theory of Speech Perception." Journal of Phonetics 22, no. 1 (January 1994): 104–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0095-4470(19)30270-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Devlin, Joseph T., and Jennifer Aydelott. "Speech Perception: Motoric Contributions versus the Motor Theory." Current Biology 19, no. 5 (March 2009): R198—R200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Galantucci, B., C. A. Fowler, and M. T. Turvey. "Erratum to: The motor theory of speech perception reviewed." Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 13, no. 4 (August 2006): 742. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193990.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Schwartz, Jean-Luc, Anahita Basirat, Lucie Ménard, and Marc Sato. "The Perception-for-Action-Control Theory (PACT): A perceptuo-motor theory of speech perception." Journal of Neurolinguistics 25, no. 5 (September 2012): 336–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.12.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Ivry, Richard B., and Timothy C. Justus. "A neural instantiation of the motor theory of speech perception." Trends in Neurosciences 24, no. 9 (September 2001): 513–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01897-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Moulin-Frier, Clément, and Michael A. Arbib. "Recognizing speech in a novel accent: the motor theory of speech perception reframed." Biological Cybernetics 107, no. 4 (June 11, 2013): 421–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00422-013-0557-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Motor theory of speech perception"

1

Yeung, Ho Henny. "The origins of articulatory-motor influences on speech perception." Thesis, University of British Columbia, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/2429/27243.

Full text
Abstract:
Myriad factors influence perceptual processing, but “embodied” approaches assert that sensorimotor information about bodily movements plays an especially critical role. This view has precedence in speech research, where it has often been assumed that the movements of one’s articulators (i.e., the tongue, lips, jaw, etc.) are closely related to perceiving speech. Indeed, previous work has shown that speech perception is influenced by concurrent stimulation of speech motor cortex or by silently making articulatory motions (e.g., mouthing “pa”) when hearing speech sounds. Critics of embodied approaches claim instead that so-called articulatory influences are attributed to other processes (e.g., auditory imagery or feedback from phonological categories), which are also activated when making speech articulations. This dissertation explores the embodied basis of speech perception, and further investigates its ontogenetic development. Chapter 2 reports a study where adults made silent and synchronous speech-like articulations while listening to and identifying speech sounds. Results show that sensorimotor aspects of these movements (i.e., articulatory-motor information) are a robust source of perceptual modulation, independent from auditory imagery or phonological activation. Chapter 3 reports that even low-level, non-speech articulatory-motor information (i.e., holding one’s breath at a particular position in the vocal tract) can exert a subtle influence on adults’ perception of related speech sounds. Chapter 4 investigates the developmental origins of these influences, showing that low-level articulatory information can influence 4.5-month-old infants’ audiovisual speech perception. Specifically, achieving lip-shapes related to /i/ and /u/ vowels (while chewing or sucking, respectively) is shown to disrupt infants’ ability to match auditory speech information about these vowels to visual displays of talking faces. Together, these chapters show that aspects of speech processing are embodied and follow a pattern of differentiation in development. Before infants produce clear speech, links between low-level articulatory representations and speech perception are already in place. As adults, these links become more specific to sensorimotor information in dynamically coordinated articulations, but vestigial links to low-level articulatory-motor information remain from infancy.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Grobler, Isabella Johanna. "Speech motor development of Afrikaans speaking children aged four to seven years." Diss., Pretoria : [s.n.], 2000. http://upetd.up.ac.za/thesis/available/etd-01112007-154045.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Stringer, Paul David. "Binaural signal processing for the enhancement of speech perception." Thesis, University of York, 1995. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.282296.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Schaefer, Martina Christina Marion. "The interaction between speech perception and speech production: implications for speakers with dysarthria." Thesis, University of Canterbury. Communication Disorders, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/10092/8610.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of the research presented here was to systematically investigate the role of speech perception on speech production in speakers of different ages and those with PD and hypokinetic dysarthria. For this, the experimental designs of auditory perturbation and mimicry were chosen. The initial research phase established that the magnitude of compensation to auditory vowel perturbation was reduced in 54 speakers of New Zealand English (NZE) when compared to previous studies conducted with speakers of American (AE) and Canadian English (CE). A number of factors were studied to determine possible predictors of compensation and distinguish between potential changes associated with ageing. However, no predictors of compensation were found for the overall group. Post-hoc analyses established an increased variability in response patterns in NZE when compared to previous studies of AE and CE. Subsequent follow-up analyses focused on the response-dependent categories of (1) big compensators, (2) compensators, (3) big followers, and (4) followers. Linear mixed-effect modelling revealed that in big compensators, the magnitude of compensation was greater in speakers who exhibited larger F1 baseline standard deviation and greater F1 vowel distances of HEAD relative to HEED and HAD. F1 baseline standard deviation was found to have a similar predictive value for the group of compensators. No predictors of compensation were found for the other two subgroups. Phase two was set up as a continuation of phase one and examined whether a subset of 16 speakers classified as big compensators adapted to auditory vowel perturbation. Linear mixed-effect modelling revealed that in the absence of auditory feedback alterations, big compensators maintained their revised speech motor commands for a short period of time until a process of de-adaptation was initiated. No predictors of adaptation were found for the group. Due to the unexpected results from the first two research phases indicating a dominant weighting of somatosensory feedback in NZE compared to auditory-perceptual influences, a different experimental paradigm was selected for phase three - mimicry. The purpose of this study was to determine whether eight speakers with PD and dysarthria and eight age-matched healthy controls (HC) are able to effectively integrate speech perception and speech production when attempting to match an acoustic target. Results revealed that all speakers were able to modify their speech production to approximate the model speaker but the acoustic dimensions of their speech did not move significantly closer to the target over the three mimicry attempts. Although speakers with moderate levels of dysarthria exhibited greater acoustic distances (except for the dimension of pitch variation), neither the perceptual nor the acoustic analyses found significant differences in mimicry behaviour across the two groups. Overall, these findings were considered preliminary evidence that speech perception and speech production can at least to some extent be effectively integrated to induce error-correction mechanisms and subsequent speech motor learning in these speakers with PD and dysarthria.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Pinkerton, A. Louise. "The influence of motor production experience on voice perception." Thesis, University of Iowa, 2016. https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/5825.

Full text
Abstract:
Perceptual speech and voice analysis is an essential skill for all speech-language pathologists, but it is a difficult skill to teach. Even the reliability for experienced experts is variable. Some training literature and practices in speech-language pathology suggest that imitating pathological voices may be useful for developing perceptual judgment. Evidence from other fields suggests that motor experience influences perception. Until now the link between production and perception of voice quality has not been addressed. The purpose of this pilot study is to test the hypothesis that imitating pathological voice samples would improve the perceptual discrimination abilities of naïve, inexperienced listeners. Three expert listeners rated 25 voice samples using a perceptual voice evaluation scale, the Grade, Instability, Roughness, Breathiness, Asthenia, Strain Scale (GIRBAS) (Dejonckere et al., 1996), and identified anchor samples for the training protocol. These expert ratings were used to develop summary expert ratings that served as a comparison for the naïve listener ratings. Two groups of naïve undergraduate listeners received training in evaluating voice quality and in administering the GIRBAS. They completed a pretest, a training session, a homework session, and a post-test. During each activity, they rated 6 voices and provided a confidence rating for their scores. The experimental group imitated the voice samples during the study, and the control group completed the training without supplemental motor experience. It was hypothesized that both listener groups would have improved accuracy and confidence levels between the pretest and post-test, with a larger improvement for the experimental group. Data suggested that training improved naïve listener accuracy and confidence levels and that this improvement was maintained for at least seven days after the initial training. Post-test accuracy for both groups was approximately the same. Imitation did not improve the accuracy of ratings, although those subjects had higher confidence levels. The data supported previous research that found that training improved the accuracy of perceptual voice evaluations. However, the hypothesis that imitation could improve perceptual ratings was not supported by this study and bears further investigation due to the small sample size.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Hamblin, Erin. "The Effects of Divided Attention on Speech Motor, Verbal Fluency and Manual Motor Task Performance." Diss., CLICK HERE for online access, 2005. http://contentdm.lib.byu.edu/ETD/image/etd905.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Schmulian, Dunay Liezel. "The development of a universal speech facilitation program as an extension of the speech motor learning program and its application in an experimental alternating treatment study." Pretoria : [s.n.], 2000. http://upetd.up.ac.za/thesis/available/etd-07172006-130724.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Mubenga, K.-S. "Teaching listening comprehension to Zairean students : The effects of training on the performance of EFL listening tasks." Thesis, University of East Anglia, 1988. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.235191.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Parveen, Sabiha. "Perception of Speech and Non-Speech Motor Performance by Individuals with Parkinson Disease and Their Communication Partners: Comparison of Perceptual Ratings, Quality of Life Ratings and Objective Measures." Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1375717130.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Treille, Avril. "Percevoir et agir : la nature sensorimotrice, multisensorielle et prédictive de la perception de la parole." Thesis, Université Grenoble Alpes (ComUE), 2017. http://www.theses.fr/2017GREAS015/document.

Full text
Abstract:
Voir les gestes articulatoires de son interlocuteur permet d’améliorer significativement le décodage et la compréhension du signal acoustique de parole émis. Un premier objectif de cette thèse était de déterminer si les interactions multimodales lors de la perception de parole, en plus d’impliquer classiquement les informations auditives et visuelles transmises par le son et le visage du locuteur, pouvaient être déclenchées par d’autres sources sensorielles moins communément utilisées dans la communication parlée, comme la perception tactile de la parole ou encore la perception visuelle des mouvements de la langue. Parallèlement, nos travaux avaient également pour but de déterminer l’implication possible du système moteur dans ces mécanismes de perception multisensorielle. Enfin, un autre enjeu de nos recherches était de déterminer plus avant le décours temporel et l’organisation neuroanatomique fonctionnelle de ces mécanismes d’intégration à l’aide de différentes techniques comme l’électro-encéphalographie, l’imagerie par résonance magnétique fonctionnelle ou encore la stimulation magnétique transcrânienne. Nos travaux ont permis d’élargir la notion de « multisensorialité de la parole » en mettant en évidence une facilitation des traitements temporels auditifs lors de la perception audio-tactile de la parole et lors de l’observation de nos propres mouvements articulatoires. D’autre part, nos études ont fourni de nouveaux arguments en faveur d’un rôle fonctionnel du système moteur lors de la perception de parole en montrant une activation plus importante des régions motrices lors de l’observation de mouvements de la langue ainsi qu’un recrutement plus bilatéral du cortex prémoteur ventral au cours du vieillissement. Pris ensemble, nos résultats renforcent l’idée d’un couplage fonctionnel, d’une co-structuration des systèmes de perception et de production de la parole. Les études présentées dans cette thèse appuient ainsi l’existence de connexions entre régions sensorielles, intégratives et motrices permettant la mise en œuvre de processus et traitements multisensoriels, sensorimoteurs et prédictifs lors de la perception et compréhension des actions de parole
Seeing the speaker’s articulatory gestures significantly enhances auditory speech perception. A key issue is whether cross-modal speech interactions only depend on well-known auditory and visual inputs from the speaker’s voice and face or, rather, might also be triggered by other sensory sources less common in speech communication, such as tactile information or vision of the tongue movements. Another goal of the present research was to determine the possible role of the motor system in these multisensory processes. Finally, we used electro-encephalographic, functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation techniques in order to better understand the time course and the functional neuroanatomical organization of these integration mechanisms. Our results extent the concept of “multisensory speech perception” by highlighting a facilitation of auditory processes during audio-haptic speech perception as well as during the observation of our own articulatory movements. They also provide new evidence in favor of a functional role of the motor system in speech perception by demonstrating an increase of motor activity during visuo-lingual speech perception and a more bilateral ventral premotor cortex recruitment during speech perception across aging. Taken together, our results reinforce the idea of a functional coupling and a co-structuring of speech perception and production systems. Our work support the existence of connections between sensory, integrative and motor regions allowing the implementation of multisensory, sensorimotor and predictive processes in the perception and understanding of speech actions
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Motor theory of speech perception"

1

The acoustics of speech communication: Fundamentals, speech perception theory, and technology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1999.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Speech science: An integrated approach to theory and clinical practice. 2nd ed. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon, 2007.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Ferrand, Carole T. Speech science: An integrated approach to theory and clinical practice. 2nd ed. Boston: Pearson/Allyn&Bacon, 2007.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Prosodic phonology: The theory and its application to language acquisition and speech processing. Newcastle upon Tyne: Grevatt & Grevatt, 1987.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

13 ways of looking at images: The logic of visualization in literature and society. Beverly Hills, Calif: Red Heifer Press, 2003.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

The life of the mind: Selected papers. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates, 1988.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Studdert-Kennedy, Michael, and Ignatius G. Mattingly, eds. Modularity and the Motor theory of Speech Perception. Psychology Press, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315807942.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

(Editor), Michael Studdert-Kennedy, and Ignatius G. Mattingly (Editor), eds. Modularity and the Motor theory of Speech Perception: Proceedings of A Conference To Honor Alvin M. Liberman. Lawrence Erlbaum, 1990.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

M, Liberman Alvin, Mattingly Ignatius G, and Studdert-Kennedy Michael, eds. Modularity and the motor theory of speech perception: Proceedings of a conference to honor Alvin M. Liberman. Hillsdale, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Pickett, J. M. Acoustics of Speech Communication, The: Fundamentals, Speech Perception Theory, and Technology. Allyn & Bacon, 1998.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Book chapters on the topic "Motor theory of speech perception"

1

Tatham, Mark, and Katherine Morton. "Speech Motor Control." In Speech Production and Perception, 99–120. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/9780230513969_4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Tatham, Mark, and Katherine Morton. "Coarticulation Theory." In Speech Production and Perception, 40–98. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/9780230513969_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

He, Kekang. "Innateness and Perceptibility of Speech." In Semantic Perception Theory, 83–109. Singapore: Springer Singapore, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-1104-2_5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Harris, Katherine S. "Action Theory as a Description of the Speech Process." In Speech Motor Dynamics in Stuttering, 25–39. Vienna: Springer Vienna, 1987. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-7091-6969-8_2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Gibet, Sylvie, Pierre-François Marteau, and Kyle Duarte. "Toward a Motor Theory of Sign Language Perception." In Gesture and Sign Language in Human-Computer Interaction and Embodied Communication, 161–72. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-34182-3_15.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Giese, Martin A. "Neural field model for the motor planning of eye movements." In Dynamic Neural Field Theory for Motion Perception, 157–72. Boston, MA: Springer US, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-5581-0_10.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Thórisson, Kristinn R. "Natural Turn-Taking Needs No Manual: Computational Theory and Model, from Perception to Action." In Text, Speech and Language Technology, 173–207. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-2367-1_8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Kuhl, Patricia K. "Innate Predispositions and the Effects of Experience in Speech Perception: The Native Language Magnet Theory." In Developmental Neurocognition: Speech and Face Processing in the First Year of Life, 259–74. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 1993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8234-6_22.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Kent, R. D. "Sonority Theory and Syllable Pattern as Keys to Sensory-Motor-Cognitive Interactions in Infant Vocal Development." In Developmental Neurocognition: Speech and Face Processing in the First Year of Life, 329–39. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 1993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8234-6_27.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Mole, Christopher. "The Motor Theory of Speech Perception1." In Sounds and Perception, 211–33. Oxford University Press, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199282968.003.0010.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Motor theory of speech perception"

1

Zhao, Bin, Jianwu Dang, and Gaoyan Zhang. "A Neuro-Experimental Evidence for the Motor Theory of Speech Perception." In Interspeech 2017. ISCA: ISCA, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2017-1741.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Wolniak, Radoslaw. "THE PERCEPTION OF ARCHITECTURAL BARRIERS IN SOSNOWIEC MUNICIPIAL OFFICE FROM DISABLE PERSON POINT OF VIEW." In GEOLINKS International Conference. SAIMA Consult Ltd, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.32008/geolinks2020/b2/v2/37.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper concentrate on problems connected with problems of peoples with disability. The main aims of the paper is to measure the level of quality of service in the case of architectural barriers in municipal offices by peoples in disability. We made following hypothesis: the types of disability significantly affects the perception of quality of services in municipal office regarding architectural barrier. The problem of satisfaction of people with disability in the case of architectural barriers in municipal office in Sosnowiec was analyzed from type of disability point of view. We distinguished five main types of disability in the paper: sensory impairment – a lack, damage or disorder of sensory analysers’ function (this category includes the blind, the visually impaired, the deaf, hard of hearing persons and people with visual and auditory perception disorders); intellectual impairment – mental retardation; social functioning impairment – disorders of neural and emotional balance; communication impairment – hindered verbal contact (speech impediments, autism, stammering); motor impairment – people with motor organ dysfunction. On the basis of that are discussed in this publication the research, we can conclude that the overall assessment of architectural barriers for people with disabilities is as in the case of the Municipal Office in Sosnowiec at an average level. The problems focus mainly on matters of specialized service selected groups of customers with disabilities who require further elaboration. Another type of problem is to issue a limited number of parking spaces for the disabled, but for objective reasons, it will be difficult to solve. Also we can say that the assessment of the architectural barriers by peoples with various types of disability vary significantly. The architectural barriers are the problem especially for people with motor disabilities – those persons are going to municipal office often and because of type of their disability barriers within the office and near the office is the big problem for them. The results are supporting the hypothesis that the type of disability affects perception of architectural barriers by peoples with disabilities.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Barnaud, Marie-Lou, Julien Diard, Pierre Bessiere, and Jean-Luc Schwartz. "COSMO, a Bayesian computational model of speech communication: Assessing the role of sensory vs. motor knowledge in speech perception." In 2015 Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-EpiRob). IEEE, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/devlrn.2015.7346149.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Nishimura, Yoshitaka, Mitsuru Ishizuka, Kazuhiro Nakadai, Mikio Nakano, and Hiroshi Tsujino. "Speech Recognition for a Humanoid with Motor Noise Utilizing Missing Feature Theory." In 2006 6th IEEE-RAS International Conference on Humanoid Robots. IEEE, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ichr.2006.321359.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Laurent, Raphaël, Jean-Luc Schwartz, Pierre Bessière, and Julien Diard. "A computational model of perceptuo-motor processing in speech perception: learning to imitate and categorize synthetic CV syllables." In Interspeech 2013. ISCA: ISCA, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2013-640.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Kirshenbaum, Ari, Chris Lewis, and Andy Kaplan. "Mobile app detection of THC-related cognitive impairment in heavy users." In 2021 Virtual Scientific Meeting of the Research Society on Marijuana. Research Society on Marijuana, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.26828/cannabis.2022.01.000.10.

Full text
Abstract:
The degree to which frequent users of cannabis experience cognitive impairment from acute self-administration has been questioned on the basis of behavioral tolerance to THC. "Indicator" is a downloadable mobile software app that assesses cognitive, perceptual, and motor skills using a variety of brief videogames. In the course of one month (April 2021), the app was used by 199 adult users who self-identified as either use cannabis "frequently" or "continuously." Sixty-one of these heavy users played at least two of the videogames available in the app while sober, and this was on the first occasion of using the app. Ninety-six used the app while intoxicated by cannabis, and also on their first instance of using the app. Independent-samples t-test was performed to compare sober-versus-intoxicated performance on each of two separate videogames, and these videogames specifically assessed (a) time perception and (b) reaction speed and accuracy. Clear evidence of cannabis-related impairment was evident for both videogames (p < 0.05) for this heavy-using population. This evidence suggests that neurocognitive performance-related deficits are apparent in a population of users who are well-accustomed to the psychopharmacological influence of THC.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Krasaki, Eirini. "Design as semiosis: A design mechanism for place branding." In International Conference on the 4th Game Set and Match (GSM4Q-2019). Qatar University Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.29117/gsm4q.2019.0035.

Full text
Abstract:
The described design methodology combines parametric design, data analysis, algorithmic design and semiotics theory to systematically analyze urban reality. The analysis leads to a creation of a nebula of data which corresponds to the place of interest. The nebula of data consists of networks of semiotics spatially defined. Through the proposed methodology, semiotics are used to enhance the perception that we have for a place and create a strategy for its' branding. Space is not approached as an empty container but as a complex system that consists of material and immaterial elements. The characteristics of these elements are quantified by their context and the logics of description to which they correspond. Logics of description are constantly changing following the multiplicity and the expansion of concepts. Therefore, space is constantly redefined following the transformation of the corresponding virtual data. Considering that each framework draws up an ideology following the change of context and the logics of description, a tool (machine) for analyzing written speech is developed, combining data visualization techniques, linguistics and design methodologies to configure logics of description. Written speech is transformed into a series of networks, visualizing their ontological relationships and disregarding the factor of time. A nebula of data corresponding to the mental reality of space is formed. Following a methodological procedure, the nebula of space is transformed to a nebula of place. The nebula of place contains its' key characteristics parametrized. A selection of these characteristics is combined to create the brand of the place concerning its' context and logics of description. The before mentioned methodological tool connects people, spaces, and machines enabling the connection of spatial data to create the impression (brand) of a place.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography