Academic literature on the topic 'Monkeys motor cortex'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Monkeys motor cortex.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Monkeys motor cortex"
Allison, T., C. C. Wood, G. McCarthy, and D. D. Spencer. "Cortical somatosensory evoked potentials. II. Effects of excision of somatosensory or motor cortex in humans and monkeys." Journal of Neurophysiology 66, no. 1 (July 1, 1991): 64–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1991.66.1.64.
Full textLawrence, Donald G. "Central Neural Mechanisms of Prehension." Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology 72, no. 5 (May 1, 1994): 580–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/y94-082.
Full textMurray, G. M., L. D. Lin, E. M. Moustafa, and B. J. Sessle. "Effects of reversible inactivation by cooling of the primate face motor cortex on the performance of a trained tongue-protrusion task and a trained biting task." Journal of Neurophysiology 65, no. 3 (March 1, 1991): 511–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1991.65.3.511.
Full textQi, Hui-Xin, Iwona Stepniewska, and Jon H. Kaas. "Reorganization of Primary Motor Cortex in Adult Macaque Monkeys With Long-Standing Amputations." Journal of Neurophysiology 84, no. 4 (October 1, 2000): 2133–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.2000.84.4.2133.
Full textWidener, Gail L., and Paul D. Cheney. "Effects on Muscle Activity From Microstimuli Applied to Somatosensory and Motor Cortex During Voluntary Movement in the Monkey." Journal of Neurophysiology 77, no. 5 (May 1, 1997): 2446–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.77.5.2446.
Full textMoore, T., H. R. Rodman, A. B. Repp, C. G. Gross, and R. S. Mezrich. "Greater residual vision in monkeys after striate cortex damage in infancy." Journal of Neurophysiology 76, no. 6 (December 1, 1996): 3928–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.3928.
Full textSimon, Stéphane R., Martine Meunier, Loÿs Piettre, Anna M. Berardi, Christoph M. Segebarth, and Driss Boussaoud. "Spatial Attention and Memory Versus Motor Preparation: Premotor Cortex Involvement as Revealed by fMRI." Journal of Neurophysiology 88, no. 4 (October 1, 2002): 2047–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.2002.88.4.2047.
Full textBracewell, R. M., P. Mazzoni, S. Barash, and R. A. Andersen. "Motor intention activity in the macaque's lateral intraparietal area. II. Changes of motor plan." Journal of Neurophysiology 76, no. 3 (September 1, 1996): 1457–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.1457.
Full textKurata, Kiyoshi, and Eiji Hoshi. "Reacquisition Deficits in Prism Adaptation After Muscimol Microinjection Into the Ventral Premotor Cortex of Monkeys." Journal of Neurophysiology 81, no. 4 (April 1, 1999): 1927–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1927.
Full textPavlides, C., E. Miyashita, and H. Asanuma. "Projection from the sensory to the motor cortex is important in learning motor skills in the monkey." Journal of Neurophysiology 70, no. 2 (August 1, 1993): 733–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1993.70.2.733.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Monkeys motor cortex"
Thaler, D. E. "Supplementary motor cortex and the control of action." Thesis, University of Oxford, 1988. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.235063.
Full textBenda, Brian J. "Neural correlates of motor learning/memory in primary motor cortex of macaque monkeys." Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/9920.
Full textSpinks, Rachel Lucy. "Premotor and motor cortex and visually guided grasp : a methodological and experimental study of local field potentials in the cortex of the awake, behaving macaque monkey." Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2005. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1446483/.
Full textConfais, Joachim. "Timing dans le cortex moteur : de l'anticipation d'un indice spatial à la préparation du mouvement : =Timing in motor cortex : from cue anticipation to movement preparation." Thesis, Aix-Marseille, 2013. http://www.theses.fr/2013AIXM5015/document.
Full textThe temporal context deeply shapes the motor cortical activity (spikes and LFPs), during movement preparation but also outside movement preparation
Zimnik, Andrew James. "The Generation of Complex Reaches." Thesis, 2021. https://doi.org/10.7916/d8-p5ca-zv88.
Full textBittner, Sean Robert. "Building theories of neural circuits with machine learning." Thesis, 2021. https://doi.org/10.7916/d8-qkrz-sv89.
Full textBuchwald, Daniela. "Monkey see, monkey touch, monkey do: Influence of visual and tactile input on the fronto-parietal grasping network." Doctoral thesis, 2020. http://hdl.handle.net/21.11130/00-1735-0000-0005-13DC-E.
Full textAddou, Touria. "Mécanismes psychophysiques et neuronaux de la compensation dynamique de multiples champs de force : facilitation et anticipation liée à des indices de couleur." Thèse, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/1866/15996.
Full textIn this thesis, we addressed motor control by two experimental approaches: psychophysical studies in human subjects and neurophysiological recordings in non-human primates. We identified unresolved issues concerning interference in motor learning during adaptation of subjects to two or more anti-correlated force fields. We designed paradigms in which arbitrary color stimuli provided contextual cues that allowed subjects to predict the nature of impending external force fields before encountering them physically during arm movements. This contextual knowledge helped to facilitate adaptation to the force fields by reducing this interference. According to one computational model of motor learning (MOdular Selection And Identification model for Control; MOSAIC), the color context cues made it easier for subjects to build “internal models” of each force field, to recall them and to switch between them with minimal interference. In our first experiment, four groups of human subjects performed elbow flexion/extension movements against two anti-correlated viscous force fields. We combined two different colors for the computer monitor background with two forces: resistive (Vr) and assistive (Va). The first two groups were control subjects. In those subjects, the color of the computer monitor changed at regular intervals but the force field remained constant; Vr was presented to the first group while the second group only experienced Va. As a result, the color cues were irrelevant in the two control groups. All control subjects adapted well to the single experienced force field (Vr or Va). In the two experimental groups, in contrast, the anti-correlated force fields and the monitor colors changed repeatedly between short blocks of trials. In the first experimental group (Reliable-cue subjects), there was a consistent relationship between the force and the stimulus (color of the monitor) - the red colour always signalled the resistive force while the green colour always signalled the assistive force. Adaptation to the two anti-correlated forces for the Reliable-cue group was significant during 10 days of training and almost as good as in the Irrelevant-cue groups who only experienced one of the two force fields. Furthermore, the Reliable-cue subjects quickly demonstrated predictive adaptive changes in their motor output whenever the monitor color changed, even during their first day of training, showing that they could use the reliable color context cues to recall the appropriate motor skills. In contrast, the monitor color also changed regularly between red and green in the second experimental group, but the force fields were not consistently associated with the color cue (Unreliable-cue group). These subjects took longer to adapt to the two force fields than the other three groups, and could not use the unreliable color cue change to make predictive changes to their motor output. Nevertheless, all Unreliable-cue subjects developed an ingenious strategy of making a specific “default” arm movement to probe the type of force field they would encounter in the first trial after the monitor color changed and used the proprioceptive feedback about the nature of the field to make appropriate predictive changes to their motor output for the next few trials, until the monitor color changed again, signifying the possibility of a change in force fields. This strategy was effective since the force remained constant in each short block of trials while the monitor color remained unchanged. This showed that the Unreliable-cue subjects were able to extract implicit and explicit information about the structure of the task from the color stimuli and use that knowledge to reduce interference when adapting to anti-correlated forces. The results of this first study encouraged us to advance our understanding of how subjects can recall multiple motor skills coupled to color context stimuli can be recalled, and how this phenomenon can be reflected by the neuronal activity in monkeys. Our aim was to elucidate how neurons of primary motor cortex (M1) can contribute to adaptive compensation for a wide range of different external forces during single-joint elbow flexion/extension movements. At the same time, we aimed to test the hypothesis evoked in the MOSAIC model, whereby multiple controller modules located in the cerebellum may predict each context and produce appropriate adaptive output signals for a small range of task conditions. Also, according to this hypothesis, M1 neurons may receive inputs from many specialized cerebellar controllers and show appropriate response modulations for a wide range of task conditions. We trained two monkeys to adapt their flexion/extension elbow movements against 5 different force-field conditions: null field without any external force disturbance, two anti-correlated viscous forces (assistive and resistive), which depended on movement speed and resembled that used in the human psychophysical study, a resistive elastic force which depended on elbow-joint position and finally, a visco-elastic field that was the linear sum of the elastic and viscous forces field. Each force field was reliably coupled to 5 different computer monitor background colors. The monkeys properly adapted to the 5 different force-field conditions and used the color context cues to recall the corresponding motor skill for the force field associated with each color, so that they could make predictive changes to their motor output before they physically encountered the force fields. EMG recordings eliminated the possibility that a co-contraction strategy was used by the monkeys to adapt to the force fields, since the EMG patterns were appropriate to compensate for each force-field condition. In parallel, M1 neurons showed systematic changes in their activity at the single-neuron and population level in each force-field condition that could signal the required changes in the direction, magnitude and time course of muscle force output required to compensate for the 5 force-field conditions. The patterns of response changes in each force field were consistent enough across M1 neurons to suggest that most M1 neurons contributed to the compensation for all force field conditions, in line with the predictions of the MOSAIC model. Also, these response changes do not support a strongly modular organization for M1.
Coallier, Émilie. "Étude du cortex prémoteur et préfrontal lors de la prise de décision pendant l'intégration temporelle des informations." Thèse, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/1866/11803.
Full textA variety of models of the decision-making process in many different contexts suggest that subjects sample, accumulate and integrate sensory evidence for and against different alternative choices, until one of those signals exceeds a decision criterion threshold. Early models assumed that this process is static and does not change during a trial or even between trials, but only between blocks of trials when task demands such as speed versus accuracy change. However, newer models suggest that the decision-making process is dynamic and factors that influence the evidence accumulation process might change both between trials in a block and even during a trial. This thesis project aims to demonstrate that decisions about reaching movements emerge from a mechanism of integration of sensory evidence to a decision criterion threshold. We developed a paradigm for decision-making about reach direction based on ambiguous sensory input to search for neural correlates of the decision-making process in primary motor cortex (M1), premotor cortex (PMd) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFc). We first tested several versions of the task with human subjects before developing a task (“Choose and Go”) that showed ideal behavior from the subjects to test our hypothesis. The task required subjects to choose between two color-coded targets in different spatial locations by deciding the predominant color of a central “decision cue” that contained different amounts of colored squares of the two target colors. The strength of the evidence was manipulated by varying the relative numbers of squares of the two colors. The response times and error rates both increased in parallel as the strength of the sensory evidence in the decision cue (its color bias) became increasingly weaker. Computational modelling showed that the choice behaviour of the subjects could be captured by different variants of the drift-diffusion model for accumulation of sensory evidence to a decision threshold. We then recorded cells from M1, PMd and DLPFc in 2 macaques while they performed the task. Behavioral data showed that response times and error rates increased with the amount of ambiguity of the decision cues. M1 cells discharged in correlation with movement onset and were not influenced by the ambiguity of the decision cues. In contrast, the discharge of PMd cells increased more slowly with increased ambiguity of the decision cues and took increasingly more time to signal the movement direction chosen by the monkeys. The changes in activity reflected the monkeys’ reach choices. These data support a role for PMd in the choice of reach direction. DLPFc data are preliminary but reveal a stronger effect of the color-location conjunction rule in the neuronal discharge than in PMd. Our conclusion is that PMd is involved in the evaluation of evidence for and against different alternatives and about target spatial location independent of the color of the targets. DLPFC neurons play a greater role in processing information about the color and location of the spatial targets and decision cue to resolve the color-location conjunction rule required to decide on the reach target direction.
Dea, Melvin. "Origine des projections sensorimotrices dans des sous-régions du cortex moteur primaire chez le singe capucin." Thèse, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/1866/13417.
Full textBooks on the topic "Monkeys motor cortex"
The cortical and subcortical efferent and afferent connections of a proposed cingulate motor cortex and its topographical relationship to the primary and supplementary motor cortices of the rhesus monkey. 1989.
Find full textBook chapters on the topic "Monkeys motor cortex"
Black, Perry, Ronald S. Markowitz, and Salvatore N. Cianci. "Recovery of Motor Function After Lesions in Motor Cortex of Monkey." In Novartis Foundation Symposia, 65–84. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470720165.ch5.
Full textRiehle, Alexa, Sonja Grün, Ad Aertsen, and Jean Requin. "Signatures of dynamic cell assemblies in monkey motor cortex." In Artificial Neural Networks — ICANN 96, 673–78. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 1996. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/3-540-61510-5_114.
Full textSasaki, K., and H. Gemba. "Compensatory Motor Function of the Somatosensory Cortex in the Monkey Following Cooling of the Motor Cortex and Cerebellectomy." In Hand Function and the Neocortex, 275–87. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 1985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-70105-4_17.
Full textAsanuma, Hiroshi. "Recovery of Motor Skill Following Deprivation of Direct Sensory Input to the Motor Cortex in the Monkey." In Neural Mechanisms of Conditioning, 187–96. Boston, MA: Springer US, 1986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-2115-6_10.
Full textControzzi, M., Y. Hao, Q. Zhang, C. Cipriani, S. Zhang, W. Chen, M. C. Carrozza, and X. Zheng. "Decoding Grasp Types from the Monkey Motor Cortex and On-Line Control of a Dexterous Artificial Hand." In Converging Clinical and Engineering Research on Neurorehabilitation, 67–71. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-34546-3_11.
Full textGoldman, Patricia S., and Walle J. H. Nauta. "Columnar Distribution of Cortico-Cortical Fibers in the Frontal Association, Limbic, and Motor Cortex of the Developing Rhesus Monkey." In Neuroanatomy, 561–81. Boston, MA: Birkhäuser Boston, 1993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-7920-1_28.
Full textTurton, A., C. Fraser, D. Flament, W. Werner, K. M. B. Bennett, and R. N. Lemon. "Organisation of Cortico-motoneuronal Projections from the Primary Motor Cortex: Evidence for Task-Related Function in Monkey and in Man." In Spasticity, 8–24. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 1993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-78367-8_2.
Full textRizzolatti, Giacomo, and Stefano Rozzi. "Motor Cortex and Mirror System in Monkeys and Humans." In Neurobiology of Language, 59–72. Elsevier, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-407794-2.00006-7.
Full textKaas, Jon H. "The Organization of Sensory and Motor Cortex in Owl Monkeys." In Aotus: the Owl Monkey, 321–51. Elsevier, 1994. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-072405-5.50017-2.
Full textMerchant, Hugo, and Apostolos P. Georgopoulos. "Inhibitory Mechanisms in the Motor Cortical Circuit." In Handbook of Brain Microcircuits, edited by Gordon M. Shepherd and Sten Grillner, 67–74. Oxford University Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med/9780190636111.003.0006.
Full textConference papers on the topic "Monkeys motor cortex"
Liu, Keyi, Wenjuan Hu, and Yao Chen. "Encoding of Stimulus-driven and Intention-driven Actions in Monkey's Primary Motor Cortex." In ICBBE '19: 2019 6th International Conference on Biomedical and Bioinformatics Engineering. New York, NY, USA: ACM, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3375923.3375945.
Full textMiyashita, Eizo, and Yutaka Sakaguchi. "Suggestive evidence for a forward model of the arm in the monkey motor cortex." In 2014 IEEE 13th International Workshop on Advanced Motion Control (AMC). IEEE, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/amc.2014.6823280.
Full textAlexander, G. E., and M. D. Crutcher. "Parallel processing within motor areas of cerebral cortex and basal ganglia in the monkey." In 1990 IJCNN International Joint Conference on Neural Networks. IEEE, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ijcnn.1990.137784.
Full textQian, Kai, Luiz Antonio dos Anjos, Karthikeyan Balasubramanian, Kelsey Stilson, Carrie Balcer, Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos, and Derek G. Kamper. "Using monkey hand exoskeleton to explore finger passive joint movement response in primary motor cortex." In 2017 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC). IEEE, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/embc.2017.8037642.
Full textWatanabe, Hidenori, Kazutaka Takahashi, and Tadashi Isa. "Phase locking of β oscillation in electrocorticography (ECoG) in the monkey motor cortex at the onset of EMGs and 3D reaching movements." In 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC). IEEE, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/embc.2015.7318299.
Full textWatanabe, Hidenori, Kazutaka Takahashi, Yukio Nishimura, and Tadashi Isa. "Phase and magnitude spatiotemporal dynamics of β oscillation in electrocorticography (ECoG) in the monkey motor cortex at the onset of 3D reaching movements." In 2014 36th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC). IEEE, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/embc.2014.6944796.
Full text