To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Maze Solving.

Journal articles on the topic 'Maze Solving'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Maze Solving.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Crowe, David A., Bruno B. Averbeck, Matthew V. Chafee, John H. Anderson, and Apostolos P. Georgopoulos. "Mental Maze Solving." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, no. 5 (September 2000): 813–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892900562426.

Full text
Abstract:
We sought to determine how a visual maze is mentally solved. Human subjects (N = 13) viewed mazes with orthogonal, unbranched paths; each subject solved 200-600 mazes in any specific experiment below. There were four to six openings at the perimeter of the maze, of which four were labeled: one was the entry point and the remainder were potential exits marked by Arabic numerals. Starting at the entry point, in some mazes the path exited, whereas in others it terminated within the maze. Subjects were required to type the number corresponding to the true exit (if the path exited) or type zero (if the path did not exit). In all cases, the only required hand movement was a key press, and thus the hand never physically traveled through the maze. Response times (RT) were recorded and analyzed using a multiple linear regression model. RT increased as a function of key parameters of the maze, namely the length of the main path, the number of turns in the path, the direct distance from entry to termination, and the presence of an exit. The dependence of RT on the number of turns was present even when the path length was fixed in a separate experiment (N = 10 subjects). In a different experiment, subjects solved large and small mazes (N = 3 subjects). The former was the same as the latter but was scaled up by 1.77 times. Thus both kinds of mazes contained the same number of squares but each square subtended 1.77° of visual angle (DVA) in the large maze, as compared to 1 DVA in the small one. We found that the average RT was practically the same in both cases. A multiple regression analysis revealed that the processing coefficients related to maze distance (i.e., path length and direct distance) were reduced by approximately one-half when solving large mazes, as compared to solving small mazes. This means that the efficiency in processing distance-related information almost doubled for scaled-up mazes. In contrast, the processing coefficients for number of turns and exit status were practically the same in the two cases. Finally, the eye movements of three subjects were recorded during maze solution. They consisted of sequences of saccades and fixations. The number of fixations in a trial increased as a linear function of the path length and number of turns. With respect to the fixations themselves, eyes tended to fixate on the main path and to follow it along its course, such that fixations occurring later in time were positioned at progressively longer distances from the entry point. Furthermore, the time the eyes spent at each fixation point increased as a linear function of the length and number of turns in the path segment between the current and the upcoming fixation points. These findings suggest that the maze segment from the current fixation spot to the next is being processed during the fixation time (FT), and that a significant aspect of this processing relates to the length and turns in that segment. We interpreted these relations to mean that the maze was mentally traversed. We then estimated the distance and endpoint of the path mentally traversed within a specific FT; we also hypothesized that the next portion of the main path would be traversed during the ensuing FT, and so on for the whole path. A prediction of this hypothesis is that the upcoming saccade would land the eyes at or near the locus on the path where the mental traversing ended, so that “the eyes would pick up where the mental traversal left off.” In this way, a portion of the path would be traversed during a fixation and successive such portions would be strung together closely along the main path to complete the processing of the whole path. We tested this prediction by analyzing the relations between the path distance of mental traverse and the distance along the path between the current and the next fixation spot. Indeed, we found that these distances were practically the same and that the endpoint of the hypothesized mental path traversing was very close to the point where the eye landed by the saccade to initiate a new mental traversing. This forward progression of fixation points along the maze path, coupled with the ongoing analysis of the path between successive fixation points, would constitute an algorithm for the routine solution of a maze.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Feuillet, C., and K. Eversole. "Solving the Maze." Science 326, no. 5956 (November 19, 2009): 1071–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1183463.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Suzuno, Kohta, Daishin Ueyama, and István Lagzi. "Chemical-based Maze Solving Techniques." Current Physical Chemistry 5, no. 1 (September 8, 2015): 29–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/1877946805666150622155712.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Lagzi, István, Siowling Soh, Paul J. Wesson, Kevin P. Browne, and Bartosz A. Grzybowski. "Maze Solving by Chemotactic Droplets." Journal of the American Chemical Society 132, no. 4 (February 3, 2010): 1198–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/ja9076793.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

., Akib Islam. "SHORTEST DISTANCE MAZE SOLVING ROBOT." International Journal of Research in Engineering and Technology 05, no. 07 (July 25, 2016): 253–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.15623/ijret.2016.0507038.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Yu, Dian, Qianqian Wan, Benjamin Balas, and Ruth Rosenholtz. "Perceptual factors in mental maze solving." Journal of Vision 19, no. 10 (September 6, 2019): 68b. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/19.10.68b.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Volk, Valerie. "Creative Problem Solving: Inside the Maze." Gifted and Talented International 20, no. 1 (August 2005): 53–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15332276.2005.11673059.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Suzuno, Kohta, Daishin Ueyama, Michal Branicki, Rita Tóth, Artur Braun, and István Lagzi. "Maze Solving Using Fatty Acid Chemistry." Langmuir 30, no. 31 (July 8, 2014): 9251–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/la5018467.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Nakagaki, Toshiyuki, Hiroyasu Yamada, and Ágota Tóth. "Maze-solving by an amoeboid organism." Nature 407, no. 6803 (September 2000): 470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/35035159.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Kirsch, Peter, Stefanie Lis, Christine Esslinger, Harald Gruppe, Peter Danos, Jochen Broll, Jörg Wiltink, and Bernd Gallhofer. "Brain Activation during Mental Maze Solving." Neuropsychobiology 54, no. 1 (2006): 51–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000095742.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Bansal, Arun, and Sunil Agrawal. "A Robust Maze Solving Algorithm for a Micromouse Robot." Advanced Materials Research 403-408 (November 2011): 4762–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amr.403-408.4762.

Full text
Abstract:
The problem of micromouse is 30 years old but its importance in the field of robotics is unparalleled, as it requires a complete analysis & proper planning to solve the problem. This paper proposes two computationally efficient algorithms for maze solving. Most of the reported algorithms explore complete maze before finding the shortest path to the center of the maze, thereby taking large exploration time. Our proposed algorithms take less time in maze exploration by exploring the maze partially and by traversing less number of cells to reach the center of the maze. An optimal method for maze solving with partial exploration of the maze can hardly be found applicable to all type of mazes, but our algorithms are found to be efficient in most of the cases. Simulation of our proposed algorithms on the some standard mazes show that one of the two algorithms described in this paper gives the better solution by only partially exploring the maze and the other algorithm also gives the comparative results when compared with the algorithms reported in the literature.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Nakagaki, T. "Solving a maze by true slime mold." Seibutsu Butsuri 41, supplement (2001): S3. http://dx.doi.org/10.2142/biophys.41.s3_1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Kumar, Rahul, Peni Jitoko, Sumeet Kumar, Krishneel Pillay, Pratish Prakash, Asneet Sagar, Ram Singh, and Utkal Mehta. "Maze Solving Robot with Automated Obstacle Avoidance." Procedia Computer Science 105 (2017): 57–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2017.01.192.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Yu, Yipeng, Gang Pan, Yongyue Gong, Kedi Xu, Nenggan Zheng, Weidong Hua, Xiaoxiang Zheng, and Zhaohui Wu. "Intelligence-Augmented Rat Cyborgs in Maze Solving." PLOS ONE 11, no. 2 (February 9, 2016): e0147754. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147754.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Lovass, Petra, Michal Branicki, Rita Tóth, Artur Braun, Kohta Suzuno, Daishin Ueyama, and István Lagzi. "Maze solving using temperature-induced Marangoni flow." RSC Advances 5, no. 60 (2015): 48563–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1039/c5ra08207b.

Full text
Abstract:
A temperature gradient can be utilized for maze solving using a temperature-induced Marangoni flow. Induced liquid flow drags passive tracers such as small dye particles, which dissolve in a water phase thus visualizing the shortest path.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Saar, Maya, Dar Hershkovitz, Orin Amano, Darar Bega, Aziz Subach, and Inon Scharf. "The effect of food preference, landmarks, and maze shift on maze-solving time in desert ants." Behaviour 157, no. 7 (July 13, 2020): 629–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10016.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract We studied how food type and available landmarks affect spatial learning in the ant Cataglyphis niger while searching for food in a maze. We expected the ants to solve the maze faster with consecutive runs, when the preferred food type is offered, and in the presence of landmarks. Ants should also solve the maze more slowly following a mirror-route switch in the maze. As expected, maze-solving improved when searching for a preferred food type than a less preferred one, as determined in a separate food preference experiment. In contrast, adding landmarks to the maze had only little effect on maze-solving and the number of searching workers. Switching the route to a mirror-imaged route in the maze delayed maze-solving and required more workers to search for food. Our findings extend the knowledge on the ants’ learning abilities and demonstrate how foragers detect food faster when offered a high-ranking food item.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

del Rosario, Jay Robert B., Jefferson G. Sanidad, Allimzon M. Lim, Pierre Stanley L. Uy, Allan Jeffrey C. Bacar, Mark Anthony D. Cai, and Alec Zandrae A. Dubouzet. "Modelling and Characterization of a Maze-Solving Mobile Robot Using Wall Follower Algorithm." Applied Mechanics and Materials 446-447 (November 2013): 1245–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amm.446-447.1245.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper is about a robot that would be able to solve mazes or labyrinths and look for the exit. The project will utilize the PIC microcontroller. This is like those in the micromouse competitions since it resembles a mouse put in a labyrinth searching for its cheese. This would also implement the Wall Follower algorithm to solve the maze and will use proximity sensors to detect the walls of the labyrinth. The robot would be as small as possible as to make its navigation of the maze more efficient in terms of turning left or right, or even doing a full U-turn since a bulky robot would have a hard time navigating the turns in the maze. This project would also require an 8x8-tiled square maze to effectively apply the algorithm and to have a test labyrinth to work on.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Bega, Darar, Yehonatan Samocha, Nitzan Yitzhak, Maya Saar, Aziz Subach, and Inon Scharf. "The effect of maze complexity on maze-solving time in a desert ant." Behavioural Processes 166 (September 2019): 103893. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103893.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Gallhofer, B., S. Krieger, S. Lis, L. Hargarter, C. Roder, C. Lammers, and A. Meyer-Lindenberg. "Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: Maze-Solving Behavior in Treated and Untreated Patients." CNS Spectrums 2, no. 8 (September 1997): 26–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1092852900005010.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractIn this article, we present data on the analysis of maze-solving behavior as a tool for the investigation of cognitive disturbance in schizophrenic patients. Solving maze tasks efficiently requires both an interaction between and an integration of perceptive and action-oriented processes. Starting from the hypothesis that these domains are preferentially disturbed in schizophrenia, we propose that the maze-solving behavior of schizophrenic patients permits insight into specific impairments in disease-related cognitive processing. We present the results of a maze task study comparing medication-free schizophrenic patients and matched healthy controls. This analysis forms the basis for an investigation of the influence of psychopharmacological treatment strategies on the observed behavior in the maze-solving paradigm. Finally, a third study concerned with improvement over time associated with medication is presented, and possible influences of extrapyramidal motor disturbances on schizophrenic patients' maze performance are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Lis, S., J. Wilhelm, B. Gallhofer, and S. Krieger. "Visual feedback in maze solving of schizophrenic patients." Schizophrenia Research 41, no. 1 (January 2000): 293–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(00)91046-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Semizer, Yelda, Dian Yu, and Ruth Rosenholtz. "The role of crowding in mental maze solving." Journal of Vision 21, no. 9 (September 27, 2021): 2896. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.9.2896.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Tak, Monika, and Soumyadeep Datta. "A Comprehensive and Comparative Study of Maze-Solving Techniques by implementing Graph Theory- implementation of Djikstra’s algorithm for solving a maze." International Journal of Engineering Trends and Technology 28, no. 2 (October 25, 2015): 61–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.14445/22315381/ijett-v28p212.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Zhang, Hao Ming, Lian Soon Peh, and Ying Hai Wang. "Study on Flood-Fill Algorithm Used in Micromouse Solving Maze." Applied Mechanics and Materials 599-601 (August 2014): 1981–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amm.599-601.1981.

Full text
Abstract:
Micromouse is a small autonomous electronic-mechanical robot, which is able to navigate through an unknown maze from the start to the destination. The main challenge for micromouse is to work out the maze after searching, find the optimum path for the shortest fast-run time and control the robot to win. Flood-fill algorithm is used in this project to develop a solver which is able to help the robot for finding the ‘best’ route from the starting position to the ‘goal’, maze solving result shows the correctness.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

BakarSayutiSaman, Abu, and Issa Abdramane. "Solving a Reconfigurable Maze using Hybrid Wall Follower Algorithm." International Journal of Computer Applications 82, no. 3 (November 15, 2013): 22–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.5120/14097-2114.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Klinteberg, Britt Af, Sten E. Levander, and Daisy Schalling. "Cognitive Sex Differences: Speed and Problem-Solving Strategies on Computerized Neuropsychological Tasks." Perceptual and Motor Skills 65, no. 3 (December 1987): 683–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1987.65.3.683.

Full text
Abstract:
Skill, strategy, and laterality measures obtained through computerized neuropsychological tasks, a reaction time (RT) test, and a visuospatial problem-solving test, the Perceptual Maze Test, were analyzed in relation to sex and handedness of 56 high-school students. Boys were significantly faster than girls on most RT subtasks (including a response-inhibition task) and made more two-choice RT response errors for right-sided stimuli, which may be interpreted as resulting from a less cautious strategy. In maze performance, boys were superior to girls. An analysis of separate phases of the maze-solution process suggested that boys preferentially used an impulsive-global strategy. Girls, using a more reflective-sequential task-solving strategy, were significantly slower, without hitting more targets. Compared to all other groups, left-handed girls (strongly left-handed) had lower performance on maze tasks with no target information, particularly in left-sided solution pathways. Results were interpreted as reflecting differences in hemispheric competence and activation patterns between the sexes. Signs of a less differentiated lateralization and slight dysfunction of visuospatial skills in the left-handed girls were discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Hong, Ki-Cheon. "A Study of Solving Maze Escape Problem through Robots' Cooperation." Journal of the Korea Academia-Industrial cooperation Society 11, no. 11 (November 30, 2010): 4167–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.5762/kais.2010.11.11.4167.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Baldi, Elisabetta, Carlo Ambrogi Lorenzini, and Bucherelli Corrado. "Task solving by procedural strategies in the Morris water maze." Physiology & Behavior 78, no. 4-5 (April 2003): 785–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00064-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Rizky Nur Hidayah and Wahyudi. "The Effectiveness of Media Geometry Maze Games to Improve Problem Solving Skills in Elementary Shool Students." Jurnal Sekolah Dasar 1, no. 1 (March 11, 2020): 11–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.36805/jurnalsekolahdasar.v1i1.893.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this research is to know about the effectiveness of geometry maze games media on problem skills in elemantary school students. This research is a pre-experimental using in group, pretest and posttest desaign. Data collection method is interview, questionnaire, and test. The effectiveness of media from maze games seen from the results of the pretest and posttest problem solving ability by means of a test Paired SamplesT-Test with helped by SPSS 25.0 for windows. The results is significant value (2-tailed) equals 0,000 pretest and posttest. From Paired Samples T-Test game geometry maze media towards probleam solving skilss
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Latha gade, Mary swarna, GAjitha GAjitha, and Deepthi S. "Design and Implementation of Swam Robotics using Flood Fill Algorithm." IAES International Journal of Robotics and Automation (IJRA) 6, no. 4 (December 1, 2017): 269. http://dx.doi.org/10.11591/ijra.v6i4.pp269-276.

Full text
Abstract:
<p>Swam Intelligence provides a basis with which it is possible to explore collective (or distributed) problem solving without centralized control or the provision of a global model. This paper presents design and implementation of swam robotics in a multi-agent environment. At the beginning, robot agents are ignorant of the maze. The robots are programmed with Flood fill algorithm to solve maze. The robot scans maze and stores the values in EEPROM. The robot agent shares the information to other robot agents through wireless communication. The proposed flood fill algorithm is found to be effective tool for solving maze of moderate size.</p>
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

King, Kathleen P., Lu Norstrand, and Julie A. Leos. "Navigating the Academic Developmental Maze." International Journal of Adult Vocational Education and Technology 6, no. 1 (January 2015): 1–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijavet.2015010101.

Full text
Abstract:
As an increased number of international students join College and University classrooms across the United States, their transition and acclimation to campuses has received attention over the past few years, particularly, in the areas of preparation and acculturation. This topic is important because faculty mentors can play a pivotal role in the professional development of international doctoral students. This article addresses these needs with a model which integrates adult learning and mentoring strategies to support international doctoral students. The model includes developing the skills which not only international graduates, but all graduates will benefit from in the 21st century. The significance of this model is its contribution to develop the valuable capabilities of proactive, problem solving, self-directed learners/academics able to self-reflect and navigate different cultural environments than their own. The discussion also provides future research recommendations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Barnouti, Nawaf Hazim, Sinan Sameer Mahmood Al-Dabbagh, and Mustafa Abdul Sahib Naser. "Pathfinding in Strategy Games and Maze Solving Using A* Search Algorithm." Journal of Computer and Communications 04, no. 11 (2016): 15–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/jcc.2016.411002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Washburn, David A. "Analyzing the path of responding in maze-solving and other tasks." Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 24, no. 2 (June 1992): 248–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03203502.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Barras, Colin. "What a maze-solving oil drop can tell us about intelligence." New Scientist 205, no. 2744 (January 2010): 8–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0262-4079(10)60147-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Redish, A. David, and David S. Touretzky. "The Role of the Hippocampus in Solving the Morris Water Maze." Neural Computation 10, no. 1 (January 1, 1998): 73–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976698300017908.

Full text
Abstract:
We suggest that the hippocampus plays two roles that allow rodents to solve the hidden-platform water maze: self-localization and route replay. When an animal explores an environment such as the water maze, the combination of place fields and correlational (Hebbian) long-term potentiation produces a weight matrix in the CA3 recurrent collaterals such that cells with overlapping place fields are more strongly interconnected than cells with nonoverlapping fields. When combined with global inhibition, this forms an attractor with coherent representations of position as stable states. When biased by local view information, this allows the animal to determine its position relative to the goal when it returns to the environment. We call this self-localization. When an animal traces specific routes within an environment, the weights in the CA3 recurrent collaterals become asymmetric. We show that this stores these routes in the recurrent collaterals. When primed with noise in the absence of sensory input, a coherent representation of position still forms in the CA3 population, but then that representation drifts, retracing a route. We show that these two mechanisms can coexist and form a basis for memory consolidation, explaining the anterograde and limited retrograde amnesia seen following hippocampal lesions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Ntinas, Vasileios, Ioannis Vourkas, Georgios Ch Sirakoulis, and Andrew I. Adamatzky. "Oscillation-Based Slime Mould Electronic Circuit Model for Maze-Solving Computations." IEEE Transactions on Circuits and Systems I: Regular Papers 64, no. 6 (June 2017): 1552–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/tcsi.2016.2566278.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Elshamarka, Ibrahim, and Abu Bakar Sayuti Saman. "Design and Implementation of a Robot for Maze-Solving using Flood-Fill Algorithm." International Journal of Computer Applications 56, no. 5 (October 20, 2012): 8–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.5120/8885-2882.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Gagnon, Léa, Fabien C. Schneider, Hartwig R. Siebner, Olaf B. Paulson, Ron Kupers, and Maurice Ptito. "Activation of the hippocampal complex during tactile maze solving in congenitally blind subjects." Neuropsychologia 50, no. 7 (June 2012): 1663–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.022.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Kwiecień, Joanna. "A Swarm-Based Approach to Generate Challenging Mazes." Entropy 20, no. 10 (October 5, 2018): 762. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/e20100762.

Full text
Abstract:
Swarm intelligence draws its inspiration from the collective behaviour of many individual agents interacting with both one another and their environment. This paper presents a possibility to apply a swarm-based algorithm, modelled after the behaviour of individuals operating within a group where individuals move around in the manner intended to avoid mutual collisions, to create the most challenging maze developed on a board with determined dimensions. When solving such a problem, two complexity measures are used. Firstly, the complexity of the path was assumed to be a quality criterion, depending on the number of bends and the length of the path between two set points that was subjected to maximisation. Secondly, we focus on the well-known concept of the maze complexity given as the total complexity of the path and all branches. Owing to the uniqueness of the problem, consisting in the maze modification, a methodology was developed to make it possible for the individuals belonging to their population to make various types of movements, e.g., approach the best individual, within the range of visibility, or relocate randomly. The test results presented here indicate a potential prospect of application of the swarm-based methods to generate more and more challenging two-dimensional mazes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Saar, Maya, Tomer Gilad, Tal Kilon-Kallner, Adar Rosenfeld, Aziz Subach, and Inon Scharf. "The interplay between maze complexity, colony size, learning and memory in ants while solving a maze: A test at the colony level." PLOS ONE 12, no. 8 (August 24, 2017): e0183753. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183753.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Macht, Victoria, Natalie Elchert, and Fulton Crews. "Adolescent Alcohol Exposure Produces Protracted Cognitive-Behavioral Impairments in Adult Male and Female Rats." Brain Sciences 10, no. 11 (October 28, 2020): 785. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10110785.

Full text
Abstract:
Binge drinking is common in adolescence. Rodent studies modeling adolescent binge drinking find persistent effects on the brain’s physiology, including increased expression of neuroimmune genes, impaired neurogenesis, and changes in behavioral flexibility. This study used females and males to investigate the effects of adolescent intermittent ethanol (AIE) on a battery of behaviors assessing spatial navigation using a radial arm water maze, working memory using the Hebb-Williams maze, non-spatial long-term memory using novel object recognition, and dominance using a tube dominance test. Results indicate that AIE impairs adult acquisition in spatial navigational learning with deficits predominantly driven by females. Surprisingly, AIE slowed the transition from random to serial search strategies in both sexes, suggesting AIE impairs flexibility in problem-solving processing. In the Hebb-Williams maze working memory task, adult AIE rats exhibited deficits in problem solving, resulting in more errors across the 12 maze configurations, independent of sex. Conversely, AIE decreased dominance behaviors in female rats, and at 7 months post-alcohol, female AIE rats continued to exhibit deficits in novel object recognition. These results suggest that cognitive-behavioral alterations after adolescent binge drinking persist well into middle age, despite abstinence. Future studies should focus on intervening treatment strategies in both females and males.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Ohta, T., I. Nakahara, R. Ishibashi, S. Matsumoto, M. Gomi, H. Miyata, H. Nishi, S. Watanabe, and I. Nagata. "The Maze-Making and Solving Technique for Coil Embolization of Large and Giant Aneurysms." American Journal of Neuroradiology 36, no. 4 (December 26, 2014): 744–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.a4198.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Kim, JunWoo, and Soo Kyun Kim. "Genetic Algorithms for Solving Shortest Path Problem in Maze-Type Network with Precedence Constraints." Wireless Personal Communications 105, no. 2 (April 12, 2018): 427–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11277-018-5740-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Chua, Sean Herbie P., Joseph Gabriel A. Dominguez, Jerald Steven G. Limqueco, Ervin Lester G. Lu, Sean Wyndell T. Que, Jay Robert B. del Rosario, and Donabel D. Abuan. "Development of a Maze Solving Mobile Robot Capable of Tracking the Distance It Traversed." Advanced Science Letters 24, no. 11 (November 1, 2018): 8640–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1166/asl.2018.12316.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

MATSUOKA, Junki, Yoshihisa TSURUMINE, and Takamitsu MATSUBARA. "Application of Soft Q-learning to Maze Solving Probrem and Verification of Policy Compositionality." Proceedings of JSME annual Conference on Robotics and Mechatronics (Robomec) 2019 (2019): 1P2—A09. http://dx.doi.org/10.1299/jsmermd.2019.1p2-a09.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Rossini, Joaquim Carlos, Leonardo Gomes Bernardino, Ederaldo José Lopes, and Renata Ferrarez Fernandes Lopes. "Computer Maze–Solving Task: Preliminary Psychometric Evidence in Young Adult and Older Adult Sample." Trends in Psychology 28, no. 1 (March 2020): 61–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s43076-020-00016-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Gourtzelidis, Pavlos, Charidimos Tzagarakis, Scott M. Lewis, David A. Crowe, Edward Auerbach, Trenton A. Jerde, Kâmil Uğurbil, and Apostolos P. Georgopoulos. "Mental maze solving: directional fMRI tuning and population coding in the superior parietal lobule." Experimental Brain Research 165, no. 3 (June 7, 2005): 273–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-2298-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Jerde, Trenton A., Scott M. Lewis, Ute Goerke, Pavlos Gourtzelidis, Charidimos Tzagarakis, Joshua Lynch, Steen Moeller, et al. "Ultra-high field parallel imaging of the superior parietal lobule during mental maze solving." Experimental Brain Research 187, no. 4 (February 28, 2008): 551–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1318-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Maarif, Alfian, Sofyan Iskandar, and Iswanto Iswanto. "New Design of Line Maze Solving Robot with Speed Controller and Short Path Finder Algorithm." International Review of Automatic Control (IREACO) 12, no. 3 (May 31, 2019): 154. http://dx.doi.org/10.15866/ireaco.v12i3.16501.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Miyata, Hiromitsu, Shigeru Watanabe, and Yasuyo Minagawa-Kawai. "Prefrontal activation while solving a computerized plus-shaped maze task: A near-infrared spectroscopy study." Neuroscience Research 68 (January 2010): e295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2010.07.1311.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Lis, Stefanie, Stephan Krieger, Johannes Wilhelm, and Bernd Gallhofer. "Feedback about previous action improves executive functioning in schizophrenia: An analysis of maze solving behaviour." Schizophrenia Research 78, no. 2-3 (October 2005): 243–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.02.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography