Academic literature on the topic 'Logophobie'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Logophobie.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Logophobie"

1

Kleinschmidt, Edward. "Logophobia." College English 53, no. 6 (October 1991): 664. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/377891.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Pigliucci, Massimo. "Logophobia." EMBO reports 10, no. 10 (September 11, 2009): 1068. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/embor.2009.206.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kaiser, Elsi. "Effects of perspective-taking on pronominal reference to humans and animals: Logophoricity in Finnish." Open Linguistics 4, no. 1 (December 1, 2018): 630–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/opli-2018-0031.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis paper investigates the logophoric pronoun system of Finnish, with a focus on reference to animals, to further our understanding of the linguistic representation of non-human animals, how perspective-taking is signaled linguistically, and how this relates to features such as [+/-HUMAN]. In contexts where animals are grammatically [-HUMAN] but conceptualized as the perspectival center (whose thoughts, speech or mental state is being reported), can they be referred to with logophoric pronouns? Colloquial Finnish is claimed to have a logophoric pronoun which has the same form as the human-referring pronoun of standard Finnish, han (she/he). This allows us to test whether a pronoun that may at first blush seem featurally specified to seek [+HUMAN] referents can be used for [-HUMAN] referents when they are logophoric. I used corpus data to compare the claim that han is logophoric in both standard and colloquial Finnish vs. the claim that the two registers have different logophoric systems. I argue for a unified system where han is logophoric in both registers, and moreover can be used for logophoric [-HUMAN] referents in both colloquial and standard Finnish. Thus, on its logophoric use, han does not require its referent to be [+HUMAN].
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Charnavel, Isabelle. "Logophoricity and Locality: A View from French Anaphors." Linguistic Inquiry 51, no. 4 (October 2020): 671–723. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/ling_a_00349.

Full text
Abstract:
In many unrelated languages, the same anaphor is either subject to Condition A of the binding theory, or exempt from it but with specific interpretive properties. On the basis of French data and crosslinguistic comparisons, I first show that such exempt anaphors must be anteceded by logophoric centers. Elaborating on but modifying Sells 1987 , I provide specific tests to argue that these logophoric antecedents can be classified into two kinds of perspective centers, attitude holders and empathy loci, thus reducing logophoricity to mental perspective. Next, I propose to derive the logophoricity of exempt anaphors from the following hypothesis: seemingly exempt anaphors are in fact bound by silent logophoric pronouns introduced by syntactically represented logophoric operators within their local domain. Crucially, this hypothesis, which is independently supported by exhaustive coreference constraints on locally cooccurring exempt anaphors, also accounts for their apparent exemption from Condition A, reanalyzed here as local binding by a silent logophoric binder.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Perekhvalskaya, Elena. "Logophoric strategy in San-Maka." Language in Africa 1, no. 4 (December 30, 2020): 115–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.37892/2686-8946-2020-1-4-115-130.

Full text
Abstract:
The article deals with strategies of coding the participants of communication in reporting dicours in San-Maka (Eastern Mande < Mande < Niger-Congo). San-Maka demonstrates an interesting case wherein 3rd person Sg and Pl pronouns, combined with an emphatic marker sɛ́, are used in a logophoric function. However, this construction is not fully grammaticalized. The combination of the pronouns with the emphatic marker acts in a logophoric function in specific reported speech contexts in the presence of the quotative particle mà ~ mə̀ ~ m’ which occupies the leftmost position of the clause. Different possible interpretations of this strategy are shown, followed by a conclusion about the ongoing formation of the logophoric strategy in San-Maka and the grammaticalization of pronouns with the marker sɛ́ into a specialized logophoric pronoun series.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Daniel, Michael. "Logophoric reference in Archi." Journal of Pragmatics 88 (October 2015): 202–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2015.07.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Śniszewska, Edyta. "Recenzja Zbigniew Tarkowski, Ewa Humeniuk: Shyness, logophobia, mutism. Diagnosis and therapy [Nieśmiałość, logofobia, mutyzm. Diagnoza i terapia]. New York, Nova Science Publishers, 2020, 221 stron." Logopedia Silesiana, no. 9 (December 29, 2020): 1–3. http://dx.doi.org/10.31261/logopediasilesiana.2020.09.11.

Full text
Abstract:
Zbigniew Tarkowski, Ewa Humeniuk: Shyness, logophobia, mutism. Diagnosis and therapy [Nieśmiałość, logofobia, mutyzm. Diagnoza i terapia]. New York, Nova Science Publishers, 2020, 221 stron - recenzja publikacji.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

McCready, Eric. "Discourse subordination and logophoric binding." Research on Language and Computation 5, no. 1 (November 14, 2006): 37–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11168-006-9017-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Oshima, David Y. "On empathic and logophoric binding." Research on Language and Computation 5, no. 1 (December 19, 2006): 19–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11168-006-9020-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Udayana, I. Nyoman. "On the Distribution of Reflexive Anaphors and Logophoric Anaphors in Balinese." Theory and Practice in Language Studies 12, no. 9 (September 1, 2022): 1848–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.17507/tpls.1209.18.

Full text
Abstract:
The central claim of this paper is that reflexive anaphors and logophoric anaphors in Balinese share the same forms. It is shown that Balinese possesses simple and complex reflexives. Only complex reflexives participate in the logophoric environment. Importantly it is claimed that the logophoric use of the reflexive anaphor occurs in a clausal complement of the verbs of communication and other verbs denoting a general state of consciousness. The logophor can appear in the subject or object position of the embedded clause while the reflexive use of the anaphor is only limited to occurring in a single clause and is restricted to occupying the object position, either the object of a verb or the object of a preposition. The characteristic differences in the distribution between the two are reflected in a syntactic domain having to do with passivization in that logophoric constructions allow it while reflexive constructions completely ban it. In addition, logophoricity may characterize an operation where there is a mismatch in the agreement between the logophor and its targeted antecedent whereas in reflexivity there must be an agreement in the phi-features between the binder and the bindee.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Logophobie"

1

Bonnot, Charles. "Le discours des documentaires musicaux : de Robert Johnson à LCD Soudsystem." Sorbonne Paris Cité, 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015USPCC316.

Full text
Abstract:
Cette thèse est une étude linguistique et discursive d'un corpus constitué de longs métrages documentaires consacrés à la musique populaire anglophone au 20ème siècle : rock, folk, blues punk, électro. Nous proposons en premier lieu une description des films du corpus tenant compte de critères formels, discursifs et narratifs. Nous voyons notamment de quelle façon le montage permet une articulation entre micro et macro-discours, ainsi que la création de pseudo-dialogues et d'une chaîne anaphorique plurisémiotique. Nous observons également un certain nombre de traits narratifs récurrents au sein des micro-récits comme des macro-récits et postulons qu'ils participent à l'expression de normes propres à la culture rock. La deuxième partie de cette étude est fondée sur la recherche de traces linguistiques de l'insoumission généralement prêtée au rock. Nous constatons grâce à une analyse textométrique une certaine plasticité du tabou langagier qui s'explique par les environnements dans lesquels apparaissent les mots grossiers. D'autre part, le corpus contient un grand nombre de transgressions interactionnelles révélatrices de la manière dont cette rébellion supposée innée est construite et gérée au sein du discours médiatique. Enfin la troisième partie de cette étude est consacrée aux contre-discours produits dans et par les documentaires musicaux, lesquels ont notamment pour but de corriger un discours doxique jugé fautif, de souligner de façon réflexive les limites du langage ou du discours artistique médiatique ou encore de valoriser l'indicible et des moyens d'expression alternatifs, ce que nous nommons la logophobie
This thesis is a linguistic and discursive study of documentaries on 20th century popular music in English-speaking countries: rock music, folk, blues, punk and electro. We first suggest a description of the corpus based on formal, discursive and narrative criteria. We see how editing allows an articulation of micro and macro-discourses as well as the creation of pseudo-dialogues and of a plurisemiotic anaphoric chain. We also notice certain patterns among micro-stories and macro-stories, which we believe are the expression of specific cultural norms. The second part of this study in based on the research of linguistic expressions of rock's supposedly innate rebellion. Corpus analysis shows the plasticity of linguistic taboo and of the use of swearwords, depending on the environment in which they appear. Besides, the corpus contains an important number of interactional transgressions that show how rock's rebellion is actually built and dealt with in media discourse. The third part of this study deals with the counter-discourses that are produced within and by music documentaries. These discourses aim at correcting a faulty doxa, they reflectively question the limits of speech and discourse, whether in songs or in the media, and tend to valu the unsa yable as well as alternative means of expression what we refer to as logophobia
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Lesage, Jakob. "A grammar and lexicon of Kam (àŋwɔ̀m), a Niger-Congo language of central eastern Nigeria." Thesis, Paris, INALCO, 2020. http://www.theses.fr/2020INAL0008.

Full text
Abstract:
Cette thèse fournit la première description détaillée du Kam (àŋwɔ̀m), une langue Niger-Congo parlée à Taraba State, dans le centre-est du Nigéria, par environ 8 000 à 10 000 personnes. La thèse propose une analyse de la grammaire et du lexique du Kam, informée par la typologie et la linguistique aréale, et contribue ainsi à notre compréhension de la structure grammaticale des langues Niger-Congo. Cette description est basée sur des nouvelles données linguistiques récoltées dans la communauté Kam entre 2016 et 2018. Elle se compose de six parties : (1) introduction, (2) analyse phonologique, (3) morphosyntaxe nominale, (4) morphosyntaxe verbale, (5) morphosyntaxe des propositions et (6) un échantillon de texte et un lexique composé de 1300 entrées. Les caractéristiques du Kam qui peuvent être d'un intérêt particulier pour la linguistique africaine et la linguistique générale comprennent les plosives labio-vélaires kp et gb ; une différence prosodique entre les positions fortes et les positions faibles dans les thèmes prosodiques ; la morphologie tonale utilisée pour la dérivation et pour la flexion ; l'absence de classes nominales et du genre grammatical ; des pronoms logophoriques ; les STAMP-morphs (des morphèmes qui expriment simultanément la personne et le nombre grammatical du sujet, le temps, l’aspect et la polarité d’une proposition); les constructions multi-verbes ; des stratégies de réduplication verbale; la négation de la proposition finale ; et les interrogatives de contenu bipartite. Les Kam et leur langue font partie du paysage linguistique et culturel diversifié du nord-est et du centre-est du Nigéria. A ce jour, on classifie le Kam comme une langue isolée dans la sous-famille Adamawa du Niger-Congo. Cependant, comme l'unité généalogique des langues Adamawa n'est plus largement acceptée aujourd’hui, la classification du Kam et des autres langues devrait être révisée. Bien que cette thèse ne traite pas la classification de Kam, elle fournit les données grammaticales et lexicales indispensables pour toute comparaison entre le Kam et d'autres langues et familles de langues
This thesis is the first extensive description of Kam (àŋwɔ̀m), a Niger-Congo language spoken in Taraba State, central eastern Nigeria, by an estimated 8,000 to 10,000 people. It offers a typologically and areally informed analysis of the grammar and lexicon of Kam, thereby advancing our understanding of the grammatical structure of Niger-Congo languages. This description is based on novel linguistic data collected in the Kam community between 2016 and 2018. There are six parts: (1) introduction, (2) phonological analysis, (3) nominal morphosyntax, (4) verbal morphosyntax, (5) clausal morphosyntax and (6) a sample of text and a lexicon with approx. 1,300 entries. Features of Kam that may be particularly interesting for African and general linguistics include labial-velar stops kp and gb; a difference between prosodically strong and prosodically weak positions in prosodic stems; tonal morphology used for both derivation and inflection; the absence of noun classes or gender; logophoric pronominals; STAMP-morphs; multi-verb constructions; verbal reduplication strategies; clause-final negation and bipartite content interrogatives. The Kam community and their language are part of the linguistically and culturally diverse landscape of north-east and central-east Nigeria. Previous research classified Kam as an isolated language within the Adamawa sub-family of Niger-Congo, whose genealogical unity is no longer widely accepted. Therefore, the classification of Kam and other languages should be reviewed. While classification is not addressed in this thesis, it provides grammatical and lexical data indispensable for any comparison between Kam and other languages and lineages
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Books on the topic "Logophobie"

1

Gouin, Jean-Luc. Hegel: De la Logophonie comme chant du signe. Paris: Presses de l'Université Laval, 2018.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Tarkowski, Zbigniew. Shyness Logophobia Mutism: Diagnosis and Therapy. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2020.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Tarkowski, Zbigniew. Shyness Logophobia Mutism: Diagnosis and Therapy. Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated, 2020.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Chen, Hsiang-Yun. De se marking, logophoricity, and ziji. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198786658.003.0006.

Full text
Abstract:
This chapter addresses the assumed connection between de se attitude ascription and logophoricity in the case of Chinese ziji. It is widely believed that logophors are among the paradigm cases of de se marking, and that long-distance ziji is logophoric. Drawing on a critical examination of a variety of analyses, this chapter argues that long-distance anaphora, de se interpretation, and logophoric marking are overlapping but distinct phenomena. Even if ziji is logophoric, it does not automatically trigger de se requirement. A de se-neutral analysis of ziji is consistent with pragmatic derivations of interpretations that emphasize the self. The findings point to a new approach to long-distance binding, and identify the blocking effect as the key issue for further research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Charnavel, Isabelle. Locality and Logophoricity. Oxford University Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190902100.001.0001.

Full text
Abstract:
Anaphors such as English herself, French elle-même, and Mandarin ziji are usually claimed to obey locality requirements stated by Condition A of Binding Theory. But we observe that in various languages, the same anaphors can be exempt from these locality requirements under certain conditions. The goal of this book is to describe and explain this widespread dual behavior of anaphors on the basis of French, English, Mandarin, Korean, and Icelandic. First, several strategies are proposed for distinguishing between the two possible behaviors of anaphors. Plain instances of anaphors require local and exhaustive binding, as well as sloppy readings in ellipsis. Exempt instances of anaphors, however, only require a logophorical interpretation, that is, to occur in phrases expressing the first-personal, mental perspective of their antecedent. Second, a new theory of exempt anaphora is proposed, which consists in deriving all properties distinguishing exempt from plain anaphors to one: the presence of a silent, syntactically represented logophoric operator introducing a local, perspectival binder for superficially exempt anaphors. This hypothesis parsimoniously reduces exempt to plain anaphors obeying Condition A, thus directly accounting for the cross-linguistically widespread morphological identity of plain and exempt anaphors. Under this proposal, the reason why exempt anaphors appear to escape locality requirements is that their binder is implicit, and their mandatory logophoric interpretation derives from the nature of this binder. Finally, several diagnostics are provided for testing the hypothesis that so-called long-distance anaphors can be analyzed just like exempt instances of anaphors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Logophobie"

1

Laitinen, Lea. "From logophoric pronoun to discourse particle." In Typological Studies in Language, 327–44. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/tsl.49.21lai.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Huang, Yan. "10. Logophoric marking in East Asian languages." In Typological Studies in Language, 211–24. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/tsl.52.15hua.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Culy, Chris. "9. The logophoric hierarchy and variation in Dogon." In Typological Studies in Language, 201–10. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/tsl.52.14cul.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

"logophobia, n." In Oxford English Dictionary. 3rd ed. Oxford University Press, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oed/1180167680.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Roncador, M. von. "Logophoric Pronouns." In Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics, 312–15. Elsevier, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b0-08-044854-2/04207-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Charnavel, Isabelle. "The Logophoric A-Binder Hypothesis." In Locality and Logophoricity, 213–63. Oxford University Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190902100.003.0004.

Full text
Abstract:
This chapter defends a new analysis of exempt anaphors based on logophoric A-binding. The reduction of exempt to plain anaphors results from the presence of a logophoric operator in their spellout domain, which provides an A-binder: the subject of this operator, a covert logophoric pronoun syntactically representing the local logophoric center, locally binds exempt anaphors, which are thereby interpreted as logophoric. This hypothesis is supported by exhaustive coreference constraints on co-occurring exempt anaphors, which shows that they must be exhaustively bound by a local binder; the illusion of split or partial antecedence is due to the fact that their implicit binder need not be exhaustively bound. Although it is inspired by previous hypotheses, the logophoric A-binder hypothesis improves upon them by locating the source of difference between plain and exempt anaphors in their binders: it thus derives all properties distinguishing exempt from plain anaphors while reducing exempt to plain anaphors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Colebrook, Claire. "Foucault: Anti-Representationalism and Logophobia." In Philosophy and Post-structuralist Theory, 162–201. Edinburgh University Press, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.3366/edinburgh/9780748622276.003.0006.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

"5. Foucault: Anti-Representationalism and Logophobia." In Philosophy and Post-structuralist Theory, 162–201. Edinburgh University Press, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9780748680740-007.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Charnavel, Isabelle. "How to Identify Exempt Anaphors." In Locality and Logophoricity, 28–107. Oxford University Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190902100.003.0002.

Full text
Abstract:
The goal of this chapter is to design tools for reliably identifying instances of exempt anaphors. In particular, the inanimacy-based strategy distinguishes between plain and exempt anaphors independently of the definition of Condition A: since inanimates cannot be logophoric, an anaphor occurring in a configuration disallowing an inanimate anaphor is necessarily exempt. This strategy allows us to re-examine the distributional properties of exempt anaphors: unlike plain anaphors, they need not be locally or exhaustively bound and can give rise to strict readings; complementarity with pronouns, however, is not a robust property of anaphors. Furthermore, potential confounds are identified, which may obscure the logophoric conditions on exempt anaphors. First, exempt anaphors may be unacceptable for reasons independent of logophoricity: strong anaphors compete with weak elements; anaphors are unacceptable in positions construed with agreement. Second, other elements like intensifiers, which are not subject to logophoric conditions, may be mistaken for exempt anaphors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Frajzyngier, Zygmunt. "The reference system of Mupun." In A Typology of Reference Systems, 25–63. Oxford University PressOxford, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780192896438.003.0002.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Mupun (West Chadic) has a fundamental distinction between reference to the environment of speech and to the anaphora, which is marked through distinct coding means that run throughout the grammatical system in the language. Mupun has one of the richest logophoric systems described so far in the literature in that logophoricity can have the subject, the object, and the addressee in its scope. The logophoric system is a part of the reference to speech environment-anaphoric distinction in the language. Anaphoric reference in Mupun distinguishes between pronouns whose antecedent occurs in the preceding discourse but not in the same clause; pronouns whose antecedents occur in the same clause; and logophoric pronouns whose antecedents occur in the same sentence but not in the same clause. The reference system of Mupun serves as a diagnostic tool for the discovery of syntactic categories in this language. From the point of view of reference, the topicalized phrase does not belong to the same clause as the comment on the phrase.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography