Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Illusioni'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Illusioni.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Pecis, Flavia <1997>. "Saudi Vision 2030: Tra ambizioni e illusioni." Master's Degree Thesis, Università Ca' Foscari Venezia, 2022. http://hdl.handle.net/10579/21027.
Full textLupi, Giulia. "Geometria Riemanniana per la descrizione di illusioni ottiche di scala." Bachelor's thesis, Alma Mater Studiorum - Università di Bologna, 2019. http://amslaurea.unibo.it/19465/.
Full textBERNARDIS, PAOLO. "TRASFORMAZIONI VISUOMOTORIE PER IL TRASPORTO DELLA MANO E PERCEZIONE DI ILLUSIONI OTTIO GEOMETRICHE." Doctoral thesis, Università degli studi di Trieste, 2004. http://thesis2.sba.units.it/store/handle/item/12508.
Full textLo scopo principale del sistema visivo dell'uomo è quello di costruire un modello interno del mondo per poter interagire con esso. Tale modello ,presumibilmente funge da impianto di riferimento per pensieri ed azioni. Milner e Goodale (1995) hanno proposto una teoria sul funzionamento del sistema visivo (TVSH) secondo la quale il sistema svolgerebbe le funzioni di (l) cognizione visiva e di (2) trasformazione sensomotoria in maniera indipendente. Alla base di questi due sottosistemi funzionali ci sarebbero due rappresentazioni differenti. Prova di questo modello viene dalla dimostrazione di Aglioti e Goodale (1995) che l'illusione di Ebbinghaus influenza i processi di cognizione visiva (percezione) ma non i processi visivi deputati al movimento (azione). In questi ultimi anni è stata prodotta una mole rilevante di dati sulle dissociazioni percezione-azione, in forte contrasto tra loro. È stata fatta una rassegna della letteratura più recente in cui sono state usate le illusioni visive con soggetti non affetti da deficit neurologici e un'analisi delle ipotesi alternative alla TVSH che cercano di spiegare questi dati. Questo lavoro si occupa di percezione visiva e controllo motorio nello spazio peripersonale. È stato studiato il processo di trasformazione visuo-motoria che avviene quando si giudica un'illusione di estensione con un compito motorio di puntamento. In tre serie di esperimenti sono state richiesti dei giudizi percettivi e motori di stimoli che riproducevano la dumbbell illusion, l 'illusione di compressione di Kanizsa, una variante dell'illusione di Brentano e l 'illusione di Miiller-Lyer. Oltre a dimensioni, orientazione e tipo di stimoli, sono state variate il tipo di azione (puntamento contro riproduzione) e il feedback visivo (open-loop contro closed-loop). Si è verificato il ruolo dei movimenti oculari saccadici in un questo tipo di compito. I risultati ottenuti mostrano che anche quando un giudizio visivo è influenzato da un illusione è possibile ottenere dei giudizi motori accurati. Le condizioni cruciali per l'osservazione di queste dissociazioni sono il tipo di codifica egocentrica contro allocentrica, l'utilizzo di feed-back visivo durante l'azione, la posizione nello spazio dove viene eseguita l'azione rispetto a quella dello stimolo. Anche se il modello TVSH fa una distinzione troppo rigida tra funzioni visive per la percezione e per l'azione, le dissociazioni percezione-azione non sono degli artefatti metodologici ma un problema teorico autentico. Sembra plausibile che in questi due sistemi ci siano diverse rappresentazioni spaziali ma anche delle connessioni non previste dal modello TVSH.
XVI Ciclo
1969
Versione digitalizzata della tesi di dottorato cartacea.
Selogna, C. "Il remo spezzato e le illusioni dei sensi. Il problema delle percezioni ingannevoli nel pensiero tardo-medievale." Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/2434/59572.
Full textPalmieri, Martina. "Realizzazione di un set-up sperimentale per lo studio dei potenziali EEG event-related dovuti a illusioni sensoriali." Bachelor's thesis, Alma Mater Studiorum - Università di Bologna, 2018.
Find full textUCCELLI, Stefano. "Il ruolo del contesto temporale in percezione e azione." Doctoral thesis, Università degli studi di Modena e Reggio Emilia, 2021. http://hdl.handle.net/11380/1239498.
Full textVisual information is fundamental to interact with objects. For instance, information is coded by the visual system to create internal representations used to guide actions such as grasping, an action we perform efficiently daily many times. However, how the motor system codes object features for grasping remains poorly understood. In particular, a long-standing debate in the cognitive neurosciences concerns the nature of internal representations of object size during motor preparation, a key aspect of grasping. According to the most influential functional interpretation of the primate visual system, size representations are coded in critically different ways in the dorsal and ventral streams. These key differences in turn have several behavioural consequences, including a relative immunity of the dorsal representations of size from stimulus-contextual effects such as those observed for consciously perceived size. Accordingly, the goal of this thesis is to contribute to the understanding of representations for visually-guided actions by collecting data from psychophysical, kinematics, and EEG paradigms. I conducted four experiments assessing motor and perceptual responses to the Uznadze illusion. In this illusion, the same ‘test’ object appears larger (or smaller) after having seen a smaller (or larger) ‘inducing’ object. Studies 1 and 2 investigated whether size representations in action and perception are affected by this form of size contrast. In Study 1, participants either grasped a test or provided a perceptual judgment of its size (by performing a cross-modal match called a “manual estimation”) after the presentation of an inducer that could be identical, smaller, or larger. Results revealed that finger apertures in both motor and perceptual responses showed a size contrast effect, that is, were affected by the inducer size. In Study 2, two further manipulations of the Uznadze illusion were investigated. Inducers were presented either haptically or visually, and either in the same or in a different position relative to the test. Surprisingly, results revealed that motor responses show size contrast (the Uznadze illusion), or size assimilation (the inverse Uznadze illusion), or even no illusion, depending on factors that seem related to multisensory integration and the body schema rather than a perception-action distinction. Overall, Studies 1 and 2 provide evidence that size representations for action are affected by contextual information. Studies 3 and 4 investigated the time course of motor representations in the dorsal stream. We focussed on the time needed to prepare a grasp, testing whether motor preparation is affected by viewing a previous distractor object equal or different in size. Study 3 showed that participants were slower in preparing the grasp when distractors were larger than the test, but not when they were smaller. Study 4 extended this finding to event-related potentials (ERPs) recordings. Here, cortical indices of motor preparation to grasp the test were investigated after presenting distractors equal or different in size. Results revealed that components of lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) were displaced in time consistent with the pattern of preparation times observed in Study 3 and 4. Taken together, Studies 3 and 4 show that grasping is not programmed solely from online information, but can be affected by information experienced recently. I conclude that the Uznadze temporal size contrast reveals that visuomotor preparation relies on relative spatiotemporal information comparable to that used to produce perceptual judgments. This conclusion challenges current theoretical models of the functional properties of the dorsal stream, suggesting a more nuanced view of factors affecting grasping and seeming behavioural perception-action dissociations.
TOSI, GIORGIA. "How embodiment shapes our perception: evidence of body and space." Doctoral thesis, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, 2020. http://hdl.handle.net/10281/277383.
Full textA large variety of sensory input from the world and the body, are continuously integrated in the brain in order to create supra-modal and coherent mental representations of our own body. Plasticity is a fundamental characteristic of the nervous systems, allowing constant adaptive changes in mental functions and behaviour. Thanks to this, even body representations can change according to experience and, crucially, they can be temporarily altered by means of experimental protocols. In the present work, we were interested in assessing the plasticity of the subjective metric of the body, and the effect of temporary changes in it on the processing of corporeal and spatial information. To this aim, two types of bodily illusion were used, i.e. the Mirror Box Illusion (MB) and the Full-Body Illusion (FBI), due to their known effects inducing strong modulations of body representation. The core mechanism accounting for the efficacy of these experimental procedures is likely to be the process of embodiment of an alien body part. In experiment 1 we used a visuotactile FBI-like paradigm to assess the feasibility and the replicability of the FBI for bodies of different sizes. Using this paradigm, we confirmed that it is possible to induce and replicate in the same participant, the embodiment towards mannequins of standard or bigger sizes. In experiment 2 and 3 we investigated body metric representation of the leg, and whether it can be plastically modulated by embodying mannequins of different sizes. To address this issue, we measured the effect of FBI induced by different body sizes, over a Body Distance Task (BDT), i.e. the assessment of the perceived distance between two touches applied to the participant’s leg. We found that the subjective experience of embodiment is also accompanied by a change in the perception of body metric that goes hand-in-hand with the current size of the embodied legs. Since we confirmed that, in healthy subjects, the metric representation of the body can be modulated, we addressed a similar question in patients with hemiplegia. In experiment 4, using a body bisection task we first observed that hemiparetic post-stroke patients show a proximal bias in the metric representation of their affected upper limb. Critically, we found that this bias shifts distally, towards the objective midpoint after a MB training session, compared to a control training without the mirror. In Experiment 5 we found a similar modulation of subjective body metric in a group of patients suffering from Ideomotor Apraxia, treated with a modified version of the MB setup, which was accompanied by an improvement in the programming of motor plans. In experiments 6 and 7 we focused more on the relationship between body metric and space representations. First, we tested the hypothesis that an altered body representation could modify the way in which individuals estimate their body affordances during a Motor Imagery Task. Our results showed that participants imagined walking faster after having been exposed to an illusion of longer legs. Furthermore, we found that the illusory embodiment of longer legs can affect the estimation of allocentric distances in extra-personal space. The embodiment of longer legs, on the one hand, reduced the perceived distance in meters, on the other hand, produced an enhancement of the number of steps that participants imagined they would have needed to walk between the same landmarks. In conclusion, we confirmed that it is possible to induce provisional modifications of the metric representation of the body, by means of body illusions. We showed that body representation is malleable to the point to shape our ability to estimate distances in the external world both in terms of reachability and allocentric distance estimation. Such plasticity of body representation and body-space interaction gives important clues for the understanding of body representation and its rehabilitation in neurological patients.
Ferraro, Roberto. "Teoria della scelta razionale." Master's thesis, Alma Mater Studiorum - Università di Bologna, 2019. http://amslaurea.unibo.it/18308/.
Full textThibierge, Stéphane. "Généalogie et analyse critique du syndrome d'illusion de Frégoli : contribution à la question des troubles de la reconnaissance et de la nomination en psychopathologie et spécialement dans les psychoses." Paris 13, 1997. http://www.theses.fr/1997PA131027.
Full textThe present work focuses on disorders of the body image and is based on the clinical study of psychosis with special reference to a particular syndrome, first identified by the french school of psychiatry in 1927 and known as the fregoli delusion syndrome. Part 1 examines the genealogy of the fregoli syndrome and the way it was discovered in connection with the capgras syndrome. An outline of the main epistemological aspects of the discovery. Part 2 recalls the discussion of the fregoli syndrome in the field of psychiatry, including an analysis of the series it seems to fit in with quite naturally and which also includes the capgras syndrome and the syndrome of intermetamorphosis. Part 3 examines the neurological contribution to major clinical issues that can be assigned to the fregoli syndrome, especially as far as the disjunction between name and body image is concerned. Part 4 deals with the contribution of psychoanalysis to the above mentioned clinical issues, with an emphasis on the work of jacques lacan which allows us to underline their unity from a theoretical point of view. Lacan's formula, which he writes i(a), shows the image of the body, i, as based on the repression of an object, a, which the image both conceals and clothes. In the case of the fregoli syndrome, the formula will be split asunder, with the image on the one side, and the object on the other, but always with the same name for the patient. The final section deals with a structural analysis of the fregoli syndrome which makes it possible to caracterize with a remarkably small number of items: 1) the clinical varieties shown by the delusional misidentification syndromes; and 2) a wide range of identification and recognition psychopathological disorders. Appended, the full princeps observation of the fregoli syndrome by courbon and fail, and two clinical observations
Pasquinelli, Elena. "An analysis of the notion of illusion and illusory phenomena : illusions in haptic, dynamic, kinesthetic touch." Paris, EHESS, 2006. http://www.theses.fr/2006EHES0118.
Full textThe aim of this thesis is to show that a theory of perception cannot easily renounce to the concept of illusion without loosing a part of its explanatory power. The proposed characterization of the notion of illusion is based on some characteristics of the perceptual experience with illusory phenomena: the existence of a violation of coherence which alerts the subject to the presence of an error, the robustness of the illusory experience and the presence of a reaction of surprise. The description of a special group of illusory perceptual phenomena that are characterized by those three elements provides. The studies on perception with an instrument for acquiring an insight on perceptual mechanisms. The notion of illusion hence presents a heuristic value also in relationship to views of perception (e. G. The ecological and sensorimotor approaches) that criticize the classical characterization of the notion of illusion
Beaujot, Frédéric. "Les illusions sensorielles vestibulaires." Bordeaux 2, 1992. http://www.theses.fr/1992BOR2M167.
Full textLaBerge, Nick. "Studying Geometric Optical Illusions through the Lens of a Convolutional Neural Network." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2019. https://scholarship.claremont.edu/cmc_theses/2265.
Full textBiber, Ulrich [Verfasser], and Uwe J. [Akademischer Betreuer] Ilg. "Visuelle Illusionen oder die Illusion des Sehens : Einflüsse von Augenbewegungen auf die visuelle Wahrnehmung / Ulrich Biber ; Betreuer: Uwe J. Ilg." Tübingen : Universitätsbibliothek Tübingen, 2011. http://d-nb.info/1162627018/34.
Full textRoux, Valerie. "Le rêve dans l’œuvre de J.-K. Huysman." Thesis, Lyon 2, 2012. http://www.theses.fr/2012LYO20091.
Full textDreaming is one of the well-known themes of Huysmans’ work. It has been well studied as far as Against nature or En Rade are concerned. However, since his earliest writings, Huysmans has presented dreamers who attempt to be somebody else or try to be somewhere else. When writing sketches and art criticism, he takes an interest in different possibilities or assumptions and abandons what he sees to explore virtuality. His conversion does not put an end to this interest, but emphasizes the rejection of the world and the temptation of the beyond. Thus the dream is not a parenthesis in narrow lives, it is also a way of writing. Huysmans’ attention is focused on what springs up: dreams in the heart of sleep but also the fantastic side of everyday life, memories coming back, and the mysterious aspects of what is already called the unconscious. The purpose of this study is to detect, in a synchronic perspective, the presence of dreams in his work and to consider what form they can take. It also wants to show how dreaming is included in the narrative and to evaluate what it brings to novels which reject the romanesque. However, we will be careful not to show Huysmans as an idealist: his work is strongly influenced by naturalism and constantly claims his rejection of a colourless or sentimental style of writing. The desire to “substitute the vision of a reality for the reality itself” (Against nature) is faced with a permanent will to destroy simulacra, to prevent the characters from escaping the real world. Huysmans’ doctrine of spiritual naturalism allows him to reconcile these two requirements and set him as a forerunner of the modern novel
Ledoux, Aurélie. "Le cinéma américain peut-il être sceptique ? : les effets de "trompe-l'oeil" dans le cinéma américain contemporain ( 1984-2001)." Paris 1, 2009. http://www.theses.fr/2009PA010580.
Full textBrodin, Lars Jonas. "Illusions of Freedom." College Park, Md. : University of Maryland, 2004. http://hdl.handle.net/1903/1550.
Full textThesis research directed by: Government and Politics. Title from t.p. of PDF. Includes bibliographical references. Published by UMI Dissertation Services, Ann Arbor, Mich. Also available in paper.
Auyer, Jonathan P. "Illusion in the commonplace| Reinterpreting Ernst Gombrich's concept of illusion." Thesis, State University of New York at Albany, 2013. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3565358.
Full textIn the dissertation I analyze and interpret Ernst Gombrich's book Art and Illusion, focusing on his view that illusion is involved in pictorial representation. Since Gombrich never gave a concise, systematic account of illusion, my goal will be to fill this void by using the text of Art and Illusion as well as Gombrich's subsequent writings in order to present a coherent account of how illusion might play a role in a picture's representing an object.
My goal is not to present an unassailable account of pictorial representation. Instead, I offer a version of Gombrich's theory that pushes readers towards a better comprehension of what a Gombrichian theory of illusion involves. In the process I introduce and defend a number of terms and concepts in the service of filling in those places where Gombrich is silent. Among other things, in response to Gombrich's notion of visual substitution I elaborate upon the claims that representational pictures function as relational models and afford recognition of the objects they represent; I reply to Richard Wollheim's "twofoldness" objection to Gombrich; and I contend that Gombrich's use of the notion of illusion is not open to the objections commonly made against it (e.g., that normal picture perception does not involve illusion because "illusion" is synonymous with "delusion").
Lincoln, Lucy, and lucy lincoln@bigpond com. "Light and Illusion." RMIT University. Art, 2008. http://adt.lib.rmit.edu.au/adt/public/adt-VIT20090306.145318.
Full textAasen, S. "Thought without illusion." Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2014. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1459420/.
Full textbrålander, emma. "illusion färg komposition." Thesis, Konstfack, Inredningsarkitektur & Möbeldesign, 2017. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:konstfack:diva-5873.
Full textMagerstädt, Sylvie. "Illusions for life : on the importance of illusions for our life and the role of cinema in creating these illusions." Thesis, University of Westminster, 2011. https://westminsterresearch.westminster.ac.uk/item/8zz24/illusions-for-life-on-the-importance-of-illusions-for-our-life-and-the-role-of-cinema-in-creating-these-illusions.
Full textPinar, Abuzer. "Essays on fiscal illusion." Thesis, University of Nottingham, 1998. http://eprints.nottingham.ac.uk/11496/.
Full textPoulet, Regis. "L'Orient, généalogie d'une illusion." Lyon 3, 2000. http://www.theses.fr/2000LYO31013.
Full textSvensson, Anton. "Tiden är en illusion." Thesis, Malmö högskola, Fakulteten för kultur och samhälle (KS), 2014. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:mau:diva-22934.
Full textHayden, Alexander. "No Love for Illusion." VCU Scholars Compass, 2013. http://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd/3097.
Full textAmato, Angela. "An illusion of reality /." Online version of thesis, 1987. http://hdl.handle.net/1850/10307.
Full textLovrič, Sara. "Illusion in Puppet Theatre." Master's thesis, Akademie múzických umění v Praze.Divadelní fakulta. Knihovna, 2014. http://www.nusl.cz/ntk/nusl-202410.
Full textLarsen, Ellinor. "Is Time an Illusion?" Thesis, Umeå universitet, Institutionen för idé- och samhällsstudier, 2019. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-162361.
Full textCarey, Shannon M. "Positive Illusions and Winter Depression: Do Illusions Go the Way of the Summer Sun?" PDXScholar, 1996. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/5251.
Full textParent, Mario. "L'auto-édition : illusions et réalités." Mémoire, Université de Sherbrooke, 1988. http://hdl.handle.net/11143/11456.
Full textKenny, Michael. "Narcissistic illusions : an empirical typology." Virtual Press, 2001. http://liblink.bsu.edu/uhtbin/catkey/1221270.
Full textDepartment of Educational Psychology
Yarrow, Kielan Nicholas. "Temporal illusions subsequent to movement." Thesis, University College London (University of London), 2003. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.404473.
Full textGrebot, Élisabeth. "Le rôle du style cognitif dans la suggestibilité hypnotique : l'influence des capacités d'imagerie et d'absorption sur les réponses à trois suggestions d'hallucination positive." Paris 10, 1997. http://www.theses.fr/1997PA100083.
Full textThis doctoral work studies relations between some sub-capacities of imagery and some dimensions of hypnotic suggestibility. In the domain of imagery, we have validated the inventory of cognitive style of Paivio (individual differences questionnaire), the questionnaire of controllability and vividness of Switras (survey of mental imagery. ) And an experimental questionnaire of sustained imagery capacity. The controllability, the vividness and the sustained imagery capacity were evaluated in four modalities : visual, auditory, somesthetic and kinesthesic. In an individual session, we have administered three hallucination suggestions with a randomized order to 81 subjects : a visual hallucination suggestion, an auditory hallucination suggestion and a somesthetic hallucination suggestion. Three dimensions of suggestibility are estimated : the ideational suggestibility, the motor suggestibility and the subjective involuntary suggestibility. For each suggestion, the loss of ideational control plays a motor role in the production of involuntary response to suggestions e. G. In the loss of motor control. This relation confirms the neo-dissociative theory of hypnotic suggestibility but infirms the ideomotrice theory. Between imagery and suggestibility, our results demonstrate two modal relations : first, between vividness and ideational suggestibility and second, between sustained imagery and subjective suggestibility. The analytic conception of mental imagery is confirmed because the vividness and the sustained imagery maintain distinct relations with the three dimensions of suggestibility. In addition, the differenciation of sub-capacities of imagery like vividness and sustained imagery may explain the contrasted results obtained between imagery and "hypnotic susceptibility" by most researchers
Inglis, Kelly. "Conscious will : illusion or reality? /." Thesis, View the Table of Contents & Abstract, 2006. http://sunzi.lib.hku.hk/hkuto/record/B35881549.
Full textSundberg, Martin, and Nikolai Nyqvist. "Illusion av val i spel." Thesis, Blekinge Tekniska Högskola, Institutionen för teknik och estetik, 2014. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:bth-2812.
Full textDetta är en reflektionsdel till en digital medieproduktion.
Smilansky, Saul. "Free will, justice and illusion." Thesis, University of Oxford, 1990. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.305685.
Full textHelgesson, Anna. "Constructing Reality : a textile illusion!" Thesis, Högskolan i Borås, Akademin för textil, teknik och ekonomi, 2016. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:hb:diva-10637.
Full textBencheikh, Farid Zine Eddine. "La criminologie : l'avenir d'une illusion." Paris 2, 1991. http://www.theses.fr/1991PA020118.
Full textThe criminology has as ambition the scientifical study of the criminal phenomenon. It is therefore the science of crime. Its purpose is to combat the juridical concepts of penal responsibility and free will which it qualifies as being scolastical and metaphysical. It pretends to be independent and autonomous, but can it dispose of the psychiatrical ascendancy ? can it define its object without referring to the notion of forbidden which is purely juridical ? what will remain of the criminological plan without the psychiatrical knowledge and the juridical definitions of the crime being an objectif of this pretended science ? the criminology is only an extralegal psychiatry. The penal law will continue to reign the field of criminality because it is the social representative of the psychic life
Andersson, Bastian, and Magnus Jansson. "Likvärdig bedömning – Illusion eller verklighet?" Thesis, Malmö högskola, Lärarutbildningen (LUT), 2006. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:mau:diva-35849.
Full textThe purpose with this paper is to study some strategies for assessing students, used by teachers working in Swedish secondary schools. The strategies are studied with the intention to find out if they can produce comparative results, when putting the emphasis on the result being equal between schools, teachers and years. We have looked at how the teachers assemble their view on a student, and how they later on link this to the grade that they will present to the student. We have interviewed six different teachers, three men and three women, working at both lower and upper secondary schools. We also interviewed two headmasters from the schools the teachers worked at. We have made use of the statements from the teachers and literature relevant to fair and equal assessing in this document. We have shown that the strategies the teachers were using are possible to place in categories, and that from these categories we are able to debate their strengths and weaknesses in regard of producing equal assessments and consequently equal grades.
Semtner, Alexis. "Magic, Mystery, Illusion and Magic." VCU Scholars Compass, 2008. http://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd/1072.
Full textSheahan, M. A. (Margaret Ann) 1933 Carleton University Dissertation Social Work. "Training programs - the great illusion." Ottawa.:, 1989.
Find full textFlannery, Denis. "Henry James : a certain illusion /." Aldershot : Ashgate, 2000. http://catalogue.bnf.fr/ark:/12148/cb38817265w.
Full textGrier, Michelle. "Kant's doctrine of transcendental illusion /." Cambridge : Cambridge university press, 2001. http://catalogue.bnf.fr/ark:/12148/cb40196532z.
Full textWong, Souk Yee. "Plato's illusion : republic of Singapore." Thesis, View thesis, 1999. http://handle.uws.edu.au:8081/1959.7/27293.
Full textWong, Souk Yee. "Plato's illusion : republic of Singapore /." View thesis, 1999. http://library.uws.edu.au/adt-NUWS/public/adt-NUWS20030911.110049/index.html.
Full textA novel and an essay submitted to the University of Western Sydney, Nepean for the degree of Master of Arts (Honours). References p. xx.
Poma, Sofia. "Modelli di analisi per l'integrazione multisensoriale." Bachelor's thesis, Alma Mater Studiorum - Università di Bologna, 2016. http://amslaurea.unibo.it/12254/.
Full textGagné, Faby. "Effects of mindset on relationship illusions." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1998. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape11/PQDD_0002/MQ43870.pdf.
Full textDaniels, Victoria. "Studies of occlusion and associated illusions." Thesis, University of Exeter, 1994. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.241130.
Full textCESARO, ANNA LISA. "UNA NUOVA ILLUSIONE VISIVA: L'EFFETTO "SLALOM"." Doctoral thesis, Università degli studi di Trieste, 1997. http://thesis2.sba.units.it/store/handle/item/12937.
Full textNovy, Andreas. "Illusionen grenzenloser Globalisierung." WU Vienna University of Economics and Business, 2018. http://epub.wu.ac.at/6055/1/sre%2Ddisc%2D2018_01.pdf.
Full textSeries: SRE - Discussion Papers