Journal articles on the topic 'Grammatical relations'

To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Grammatical relations.

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Grammatical relations.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Paul, Ileana. "Subjects: Grammatical Relations, Grammatical Functions and Functional Categories." Language and Linguistics Compass 4, no. 9 (September 2010): 890–902. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818x.2010.00234.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Abrefa, Kofi Busia, and Juliet Oppong-Asare Ansah. "Akan grammatical relations revisited." Drumspeak: International Journal of Research in the Humanities 5, no. 3 (December 1, 2021): 106–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.47963/drumspeak.v5i3.842.

Full text
Abstract:
Morphological case languages do not necessarily depend on word order to determine their grammatical relations. However, structural case languages depend largely on word order to determine the various grammatical relations. For most configurational languages, the agent/experiencer usually precedes the patient/theme in a simple clause (in the active voice). In the passive voice, the patient/theme occupies the subject position while the agent becomes an object of a preposition (oblique) or omitted as evident in English. Akan, a Kwa language of the Niger-Congo family, being a nominative-accusative language, allows the agent/experiencer to precede the patient/theme in the active construction. In the passive- like construction, however, unlike a language like English, an impersonal pronounoccupies the subject position while the patient or theme remains at the object position. This implies that agents/experiencers do not occur at the oblique position in Akan; neither dopatients/ themes occur at the subject position. Certain verbs (symmetrical verbs), however, may allow the experiencer and the theme arguments to switch positions in the active construction without affecting the meaning of the sentence in the language. This paper seeks to highlight these marked grammatical relations in Akan within the framework of Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Mathangwane, Joyce T., and E. Kweku Osam. "Grammatical relations in Ikalanga." Studies in African Linguistics 35, no. 2 (June 15, 2006): 189–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.32473/sal.v35i2.107308.

Full text
Abstract:
Although facts about grammatical relations in many Bantu languages have been established since the early 1970s, there are still languages in this family which have not benefited from such studies. One of these is Ikalanga, spoken in Botswana and Zimbabwe. This paper examines the core grammatical relations of Ikalanga, exploring its typological status in terms of double object constructions. In prototypical ditransitive constructions, the Recipient NP has all of the properties of Direct Object, whereas the Theme NP has only some of those features. The conclusion is that Ikalanga is an intermediate language if object symmetricity is scalar. However, it is shown that in marked applicative constructions where the Benefactive is non-human and the Theme is human, there is reason to argue for 'split direct-objecthood', reflecting the impact of animacy in the assignment of direct objecthood.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Dixon, R. M. W., and F. R. Palmer. "Grammatical Roles and Relations." Language 74, no. 1 (March 1998): 217. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/417620.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Durie, Mark. "Grammatical Relations in Acehnese." Studies in Language 11, no. 2 (January 1, 1987): 365–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/sl.11.2.05dur.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Hudson, Richard. "Coordination and grammatical relations." Journal of Linguistics 24, no. 2 (September 1988): 303–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226700011816.

Full text
Abstract:
The most serious recent work on the theory of coordination has probably been done in terms of three theories of grammatical structure: Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar (GPSG–see especially Gazdar, 1981; Gazdaret al., 1982; 1985; Saget al., 1985; Schachter & Mordechay, 1983), Categorial Grammar (CG–see especially Steedman, 1985; Dowty, 1985) and Transformational Grammar (TG–notably Williams, 1978, 1981; Neijt, 1979; van Oirsouw, 1985, 1987). Each of these approaches is different in important respects: for instance, according to whether or not they allow deletion rules, and according to the kinds of information which they allow to be encoded in syntactic features. However, behind these differences lies an important similarity: in each case the theory concerned makes two assumptions about grammatical structure in general (i.e. about all structures, including coordinate ones):I The basic syntagmatic relations in sentence-structure are part-whole relations (consituent structure) and temporal order; note that this is true whether or not syntactic structure is seen as a ‘projection’ of lexical properties, since these lexical properies are themselves defined in terms of constituent structure and temporal order.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Hudson, Richard A. "Gapping and grammatical relations." Journal of Linguistics 25, no. 1 (March 1989): 57–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s002222670001210x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. "Grammatical Relations in Tariana." Nordic Journal of Linguistics 17, no. 2 (December 1994): 201–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0332586500003012.

Full text
Abstract:
This article deals with the marking of grammatical relations in Tariana, North-Arawakan, and how this marking interrelates with topicality, definiteness and other discourse characteristics of nominal constituents. The following four case-marking systems are distinguished in Tariana: (i) a subject vs object case system, used with personal pronouns with animate reference; (ii) a case system characterized by an enclitic -nuku for marking topicalized and referential non-subjects, used with all types of nominal constituents; (iii) an ergative case-marking used with all types of nominal constituents under emphasis in A function, the ergative case marker being the same as instrumental; (iv) a system of peripheral cases – locative and instrumental, used with all types of nominal constituents, but obligatory only with pronouns. The overt case-marking in Tariana is related to such parameters as topicality, definiteness and emphasis, and consequently is dependent on the structure of discourse. I will argue that the unusual case-marking patterns in Tariana corroborate cross-linguistic generalizations on a dependency between case-marking and topical properties of NPs in languages with an opposition between marked and unmarked case forms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hudson, Richard. "Extraction and grammatical relations." Lingua 76, no. 2-3 (November 1988): 177–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(88)90039-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Owens, Melanie, and Talmy Givon. "Grammatical Relations: A Functionalist Perspective." Language 76, no. 2 (June 2000): 481. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/417698.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Bowern, Claire. "Grammatical Relations and Change (review)." Language 80, no. 1 (2004): 172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.2004.0013.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Velázquez-Castillo, Maura. "Grammatical relations in active systems." Interaction of Data, Description, and Theory in Linguistics 9, no. 2 (July 10, 2003): 133–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/fol.9.2.03vel.

Full text
Abstract:
An active system is frequently analyzed as the most semantically transparent case-marking system, where the agent-patient opposition underlies case marking and/or cross-referencing patterns. It has also been claimed that transitivity and its prototypical manifestation of subject-object opposition are irrelevant for this language type. This paper examines these claims in the light of the grammatical system of Guaraní, an active language spoken in Paraguay. Based on lexical and morpho-syntactic data such as reflexivization, passivization, relativization, incorporation and external possession, the results suggest that grammatical relations are indeed semantically driven and that they do not correlate with subjects and objects. The paper clarifies the semantic underpinnings of the active-inactive distinction in this language and shows that the relevant opposition is not that of agent-patient but rather that of source-locative. The study argues for an analysis based on language-specific event typing and construal.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Khatiwada, Karnakhar. "Coding Grammatical Relations in Dhimal." Gipan 3, no. 2 (November 1, 2017): 48–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.3126/gipan.v3i2.48899.

Full text
Abstract:
Grammatical relations play a vital role not only in the grammar of simple clauses but also in major syntactic processes in Dhimal. The overt coding properties of grammatical relations include nominal morphology and verb agreement in Dhimal. The nominal morphology as coding property presents a consistent nominative pattern of control in Dhimal. The pronominal verb agreement and number agreement also follow the nominative pattern. The Equi-NP deletion (or the co-referent deletion) in complement clauses displays the nominative control in the language.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Monogarova, A. G., and M. N. Latu. "LANGUAGE REPRESENTATION OF SUBJECT AND OBJECT RELATIONS BETWEEN THE ELEMENTS OF SCIENTIFIC KNOWLEDGE (BASED ON DEFINITIONS OF TERMS OF DEVELOPING PROFESSIONAL FIELDS)." Juvenis scientia, no. 11 (2018): 41–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.32415/jscientia.2018.11.12.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper presents the most common patterns of representation of subject and object relations in the definitions of terms, and identifies the ways of implementing subject relations between elements of scientific knowledge in active and passive structures that are part of the structure of applied models of organization of scientific knowledge. In addition, the article raises the question of the potential of various grammatical structures in the context of the transfer of subject and object relations. The results of the study show that system relation S can be represented by lexical and grammatical means. The lexical verbalizes of this relation are the key words of blocks of subject relations, and among the grammatical language means it is possible to distinguish the category of case.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Rispoli, Matthew. "The mosaic acquisition of grammatical relations." Journal of Child Language 18, no. 3 (October 1991): 517–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900011235.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTThe view that grammatical relations have substantial essence, designated as ‘subject’ or ‘object’ has difficulty in accounting for the variety of naturally acquirable grammatical relations. The acquisition of grammatical relations is examined from a theoretical framework, ROLE AND REFERENCE GRAMMAR, in which grammatical relations are decomposed into two separate types of structure: logical (semantic) structure and information (pragmatic) structure. The acquisition of grammatical relations from four languages is compared: (1) the definite accusative suffix and pragmatically motivated word order of Turkish; (2) Kaluli verb agreement, case and focus marking postpositions, and pragmatically motivated word order; (3) Hungarian definite and indefinite verb conjunction; and (4) Italian participial agreement and anaphoric, accusative case pronouns. Two conditions on structures are found to cause difficulty: the neutralization of a semantic or pragmatic distinction by interfering structures (e.g. Kaluli and Italian), and global case marking which forces the child to discover relevant semantic characteristics of both the actor and the undergoer (e.g. Hungarian and Kaluli). Structures that encode semantic or pragmatic distinctions independently are more easily acquired (e.g. Turkish). Piecing together discrete structures in a mosaic fashion, the child can acquire the great variety of grammatical relations that exist in human languages.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Arta, Putu Owen Purusa, I. Nyoman Sedeng, and Putu Lirishati Soethama. "Mapping Grammatical Relations of English Verbs." Humanis 26, no. 3 (August 31, 2022): 314. http://dx.doi.org/10.24843/jh.2022.v26.i03.p12.

Full text
Abstract:
This study entitled Mapping Grammatical Relations of English Verbs, concerns on numbers of arguments that a verb could assign and how an argument is syntactically motivated in clauses of which the mood is declarative. This study involves to library research, and the method that was applied in this study was descriptive method which describes linguistics phenomena like what it actually is. The data of this study were taken from the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA) by inputting verbs those have been determined to the website http://www.english-corpora.org/coca/. The main theory applied in this study was Kroeger’s (2005) regarding to transitivity and grammatical relation. Based on the analysis, verbs in English require argument(s), terms or obliques. An intransitive verb assigns a term that stands as subject, a transitive verb assigns two terms those stand as subject and primary object. On the other hand, a ditransitive verb assigns three terms those stand as subject, primary object and secondary object. In addition, there is an argument that stands as oblique.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Clark, Robin, and Alec P. Marantz. "On the Nature of Grammatical Relations." Language 62, no. 3 (September 1986): 674. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/415485.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Baltin, Mark, and Joan Bresnan. "The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations." Language 61, no. 4 (December 1985): 863. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/414493.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Clark, Robin. "On the nature of grammatical relations." Language 62, no. 3 (1986): 674–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.1986.0016.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Sun, Weiwei, Yufei Chen, Xiaojun Wan, and Meichun Liu. "Parsing Chinese Sentences with Grammatical Relations." Computational Linguistics 45, no. 1 (March 2019): 95–136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/coli_a_00343.

Full text
Abstract:
We report our work on building linguistic resources and data-driven parsers in the grammatical relation (GR) analysis for Mandarin Chinese. Chinese, as an analytic language, encodes grammatical information in a highly configurational rather than morphological way. Accordingly, it is possible and reasonable to represent almost all grammatical relations as bilexical dependencies. In this work, we propose to represent grammatical information using general directed dependency graphs. Both only-local and rich long-distance dependencies are explicitly represented. To create high-quality annotations, we take advantage of an existing TreeBank, namely, Chinese TreeBank (CTB), which is grounded on the Government and Binding theory. We define a set of linguistic rules to explore CTB’s implicit phrase structural information and build deep dependency graphs. The reliability of this linguistically motivated GR extraction procedure is highlighted by manual evaluation. Based on the converted corpus, data-driven, including graph- and transition-based, models are explored for Chinese GR parsing. For graph-based parsing, a new perspective, graph merging, is proposed for building flexible dependency graphs: constructing complex graphs via constructing simple subgraphs. Two key problems are discussed in this perspective: (1) how to decompose a complex graph into simple subgraphs, and (2) how to combine subgraphs into a coherent complex graph. For transition-based parsing, we introduce a neural parser based on a list-based transition system. We also discuss several other key problems, including dynamic oracle and beam search for neural transition-based parsing. Evaluation gauges how successful GR parsing for Chinese can be by applying data-driven models. The empirical analysis suggests several directions for future study.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Winford, Donald. "Grammatical relations in a radical creole." Lingua 76, no. 2-3 (November 1988): 258–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(88)90044-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Sharma, Lok Raj. "Form - Function Relations in English." Global Academic Journal of Linguistics and Literature 4, no. 1 (February 24, 2022): 11–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.36348/gajll.2022.v04i01.002.

Full text
Abstract:
English is an affluent language as it retains the flexibility of forms and the multiplicity of messages conveyed through such forms. Phrases and sentences which are the two forms of linguistic structures can serve a number of grammatical and communicative functions. A grammatical function of language refers to the role of phrasal forms in a sentence as a subject, a predicator, a complement and an adjunct. A communicative function denotes the purpose of using the forms as the sentences. The purposes may be to advise, request, order, invite, greet, warn, thank and so on. A key purpose of this article is to expose form- function relations in English. The article writer mustered some phrases and sentences as the nominal data from the books on communication in English. Phrases and sentences which are considered forms in the study are analyzed from the perspectives of their grammatical and communicative functions respectively. This article winds up with a striking remark that a phrasal form can serve a number of grammatical functions and a sentence form can serve numerous communicative functions and vice versa. Furthermore, there is not always one to one correspondence between a linguistic form and its grammatical or communicative function.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Tjia, Johnny. "Grammatical relations and grammatical categories in Malay The Indonesian prefix meN- revisited." Wacana, Journal of the Humanities of Indonesia 16, no. 1 (April 30, 2015): 105. http://dx.doi.org/10.17510/wjhi.v16i1.368.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Aribzhanova, Iryna. "The semantic-syntactical structure of the apposition phrase." Ukrainian Linguistics, no. 47 (2017): 52–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.17721/um/47(2017).52-62.

Full text
Abstract:
The article is devoted to the semantic-syntactical structure of the appositive phrase in the Ukrainian language. The term “double name”is used for the demonstration of semantical parameteres of the apposition phrase. Two levels of syntactic analysis are considered: abstract-grammatical level (internal structure of the appositive word-combinations) and concrete-grammatical level (the function in sentence structure). The concrete-grammatical analysis indicates that appositional phrases in the sentence are: syntactically indivisible components that serve as a simple part of the sentence (subject, object). The conflict arises between formal analyticity and functional syntheticity. This conflict is caused by nominative features of double name (compound nouns indicates the same subject of objective reality). Therefore they can be referred to as the type of lexical (stable) word-combinations. Abstract-grammatical analysis leads to the conclusion that internal structure of the appositional phrase can express different semantic-syntactical relations between nouns: appositional relations or mutual appositional relations. The mutual appositional relations are peculiar to the majority of stylistically neutral double names. O. Peshkovskiy wrote about mutual relations between nouns. The differentiation of two relation types was done with the help of the semantic principle and predicate transformation method.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Malchukov, Andrej L. "[review of:] Axel Holvoet & Nicole Nau, eds. 2014. Grammatical Relations and their Non-Canonical Encoding in Baltic." Baltic Linguistics 6 (December 31, 2015): 254–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.32798/bl.400.

Full text
Abstract:
Axel Holvoet & Nicole Nau (eds.). Grammatical Relations and their Non-Canonical Encoding in Baltic. (Valency, Argument Realization and Grammatical Relations in Baltic, vol. 1). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. ISBN: 9789027259097 (hardback), ISBN: 9789027270399 (e-book). The volume under review deals with variuos aspects of non-canonical argument marking, grammatical relations and argument alternations in Baltic languages. The volume is a gold mine both for typologists interested in grammatical relations as well as for the students of Baltic languages.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Lee, Song-Wook. "Cascaded Parsing Korean Sentences Using Grammatical Relations." KIPS Transactions:PartB 15B, no. 1 (February 29, 2008): 69–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.3745/kipstb.2008.15-b.1.69.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Bentley, Mayrene. "The marking of grammatical relations in Swahili." Studies in African Linguistics 27, no. 2 (June 15, 1998): 177–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.32473/sal.v27i2.107381.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper investigates the place of Swahili within a typological classification based on the morphological marking of grammatical relations as proposed by Nichols [1986]. Within Nichols' classification, Bantu languages are considered to be "split-marked" because the grammatical marking of a member of a clausal constituent is on the head while, in a phrase, the marking is on the dependent member. Although select clauses and phrases from Swahili support Nichols' claim, a closer examination of the data reveals an interesting variety of morphosyntactic marking in Swahili as well as in two other Bantu languages, Kikuyu and Chewa. Function words playa key role in marking genitive, instrumental, and locative relations in these languages. Function words also regularly occur as markers of object noun phrases with animate referents. Moreover, instrumental, locative, applicative, and some accusative relations in Swahili show considerable flexibility with respect to head- and non-head-marking.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Aceto, Michael, Francis Byrne, and Donald Winford. "Focus and Grammatical Relations in Creole Languages." Language 71, no. 2 (June 1995): 403. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/416183.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Hudson, Richard. "So-Called 'Double Objects' and Grammatical Relations." Language 68, no. 2 (June 1992): 251. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/416941.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Dixon, R. M. W., and Frans Plank. "Objects: Towards a Theory of Grammatical Relations." Language 62, no. 2 (June 1986): 437. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/414682.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Jakobi, Angelika, Ali Ibrahim, and Gumma Ibrahim Gulfan. "Verbal Number and Grammatical Relations in Tagle." Faits de Langues 51, no. 1 (November 25, 2020): 99–116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/19589514-05101007.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In Tagle, verbal number, as realized by singular and plural stems, has not only aspectual functions in expressing single and multiple events. Verbal number also serves as a morphosyntactic device encoding specific grammatical relations : The selection of a singular or plural stem is sensitive to the number of the intransitive subject (S) and the transitive object (P) participant. In derived ditransitive applicative constructions, the stem selection depends on the semantic roles of the two object participants. When they are assigned the roles of Beneficiary (B) and Theme (T), it is the number of the direct object T (rather than the indirect/applied object B) which selects a singular or plural stem. When assigned the roles of Experiencer (Exp) and T, however, the Exp (rather than T) interacts with verbal number. When comparing the two ditransitive objects of the Beneficiary construction, B and T, to the object P and when taking their interaction with verbal number as parameter of the comparison, one finds that T and P (rather than B) interact with verbal number. The alignment of T with P and the non-alignment of B can be identified as indirect object construction, T = P ≠ B. However, when the Exp role is assigned to the indirect/applied object and the T role to the direct object, Exp (rather than T) selects the singular or plural stem, i.e. Exp behaves like P. This alignment pattern, Exp = P ≠ T, is known as secondary-object construction.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Gerdts, Donna B. "Surface Case and Grammatical Relations in Korean." Studies in Language 11, no. 1 (January 1, 1987): 181–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/sl.11.1.08ger.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Nikolaeva, Irina. "Object Agreement, Grammatical Relations, and Information Structure." Studies in Language 23, no. 2 (November 12, 1999): 331–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/sl.23.2.05nik.

Full text
Abstract:
Northern Ostyak (Uralic) has optional object agreement. This paper analyzes the grammatical behavior of objects that trigger agreement and objects that do not, and demonstrates that while the former participate in certain syntactic processes, the latter are syntactically inert. The asymmetry cannot be explained with reference to semantics or argument status, as both objects bear an identical argument relationship to the predicate. Following the functional approach to language, under which the clause has three independent representational levels (syntax, semantics, and information structure), I suggest that the two objects differ in their information structure status. The object that does not trigger agreement bears the focus function, and systematically corresponds to the focus position. It is further argued that virtually all grammatical relations in Ostyak demonstrate reduced syntactic activity when they are in focus. This leads to a search for an information structure-driven motivation for certain behavioral properties.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Hudson, Richard. "English passives, grammatical relations and default inheritance." Lingua 79, no. 1 (September 1989): 17–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(89)90018-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Klienkova, I. B. "Peculiarities of Grammatical Properties in Nominal Word Combinations." MGIMO Review of International Relations, no. 4(37) (August 28, 2014): 300–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.24833/2071-8160-2014-4-37-300-305.

Full text
Abstract:
The article examines some peculiarities if grammatical properties of nominal word combinations based on the study of German-speaking Swiss press. The accumulated experience of linguists is analyzed and certain factors influencing the choice of syntactic relations in nominal word combinations with quantitative meaning are researched. It has been established that the choice of syntactic relations in such word combinations is determined by functional style, semantics and grammatical features of nouns - first or second components in a word combination. The aforementioned factors influencing the choice of syntactic relations in quantitative nominal word combinations are studied on the material of Germanspeaking Swiss press. The research revealed a number of peculiarities. First of all, German-speaking Swiss press uses such syntactic relation as genitive subordination quite often, not only on "special occasion" and unlike its use in Germany does not produce a magniloquent, pompous impression. Secondly, it has been established that the choice of syntactic relations in word combinations of the above-mentioned type of word combinations greatly depends on the semantic meaning of the noun - the first component of the word combination - and does not always coincide with the grammatical properties in nominal combinations in Germany. Thirdly, a clear dependence of syntactic relations choice is observed. Moreover, the article highlights the importance of grammatical features of the second component. These are such features as gender, number, case accompanied by noun declension. All the afore-mentioned features help to define the type of syntactic relations, which nominal quantitative word combinations may be connected by. The results of the comparisons of grammatical properties of quantitative nominal word combinations in German and Swiss press are demonstrated in a table below.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Hinzen, Wolfram. "On the Grammar of Referential Dependence." Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric 46, no. 1 (September 1, 2016): 11–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/slgr-2016-0031.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract All forms of nominal reference, whether quantificational, definite, rigid, deictic, or personal, require that the nominals in question appear in relevant grammatical configurations. Reference is in this sense a grammatical phenomenon. It is never determined lexically or a word-world relation in a purely semantic or causal sense. Here it is further argued that the principles of the grammar of object-reference naturally extend to cases where the reference of one nominal depends on that of another, i.e. the grammar of referential dependence, without any further special grammatical relations such as ‘binding’ required. This further includes a relation of (referential) identity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

LaPolla, Randy J. "On the dating and nature of verb agreement in Tibeto-Burman." Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies 55, no. 2 (June 1992): 298–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0041977x00004638.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper is part of an ongoing investigation into the nature of grammatical relations in the Sino-Tibetan language family. The ultimate goal of this investigation is to develop a hypothesis on the typological nature of word order and grammatical relations in the mother language which gave rise to all of the many languages within the Sino Tibetan language family. As the verb agreement (pronominalization) systems of Tibeto-Burman have been said to be a type of ergative marking, and to have been a part of Proto-Tibeto-Burman grammatical relations, the questions of the dating and nature of the agreement systems in Tibeto-Burman are relevant to the discussion of the nature of grammatical relations in Proto-Sino-Tibetan.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Foley, William A. "Grammatical Relations, Information Structure, and Constituency in Watam." Oceanic Linguistics 38, no. 1 (June 1999): 115. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3623395.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Roberts, Taylor. "Grammatical Relations and Ergativity in St'át'imcets (Lillooet Salish)." International Journal of American Linguistics 65, no. 3 (July 1999): 275–302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/466391.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Bickford, J. Albert. "Spanish clitic doubling and levels of grammatical relations." Lingua 65, no. 3 (January 1985): 189–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0024-3841(85)90183-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Matthiessen, Christian M. I. M., and Kazuhiro Teruya. "Grammatical realizations of rhetorical relations in different registers." WORD 61, no. 3 (July 3, 2015): 232–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00437956.2015.1071963.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Dixon, R. M. W. "Grammatical roles and relations By F. R. Palmer." Language 74, no. 1 (1998): 217–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.1998.0091.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Grant, Anthony P. "Grammatical Relations in Romani: The Noun Phrase (review)." Language 79, no. 2 (2003): 436–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.2003.0117.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Schwartz, Linda. "Levels of Grammatical Relations and Russian Reflexive Controllers." Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 12 (May 15, 1986): 235. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/bls.v12i0.1856.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Hudson, Richard A. "Review of Palmer (1994): Grammatical Roles and Relations." Functions of Language 2, no. 2 (January 1, 1995): 282–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/fol.2.2.13hud.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Lapointe, Steven G. "Erratum to: On the Nature of Grammatical Relations." Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 3, no. 4 (November 1985): 495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00133288.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Raupova, Laylo. "Logical and Grammatical Relations in Word Categories: The Factor of Difference and Incarnation." International Journal of Psychosocial Rehabilitation 24, no. 4 (April 30, 2020): 6828–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.37200/ijpr/v24i4/pr2020494.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Lunkova, Larisa N., and Julia I. Frolova. "Lexical-Grammatical Means of Expressing Oppositivity in an English Literary Text." RUDN Journal of Language Studies, Semiotics and Semantics 12, no. 3 (October 3, 2021): 758–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.22363/2313-2299-2021-12-3-758-773.

Full text
Abstract:
The article considers the notional nature and peculiarities of genus-species relations between the phenomena of opposition and oppositivity ; it also deals with the lexical-and-grammatica ways of expressing the latter in an English literary text. The study establishes that, besides the logical component, an opposition also expresses a subjective one, manifesting a communicants unique view on the communicative situation and thus displaying semantics of oppositivity. Unlike spatial, logical and grammatical oppositions, generally characterized by a symmetrical structure, the subjective (oppositive) opposition is asymmetrical. It is revealed that due to the structural asymmetry this kind of opposition is marked by greater semantic potential compared to logical and grammatical oppositions. It is proved that the complication of subjective oppositions with antonymous pairs, lexical and syntactical repetitions, antithetic units, oxymorons etc. extends the scope of meanings implied by them; and the use of subjective (oppositive) oppositions in a literary text themselves becomes an effective tool of contrast-building both on the lexical-grammatical and textual levels.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Boelens, Harrie, and Jeroen Mollers. "In Search of Competition between Lexical and Grammatical Growth." Psychological Reports 104, no. 2 (April 2009): 407–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.104.2.407-417.

Full text
Abstract:
Relations between lexical and grammatical growth were examined in a Dutch boy from age 1:0 to 2:6. The overall shape of lexical growth was a pronounced increase in rate until age 2:2 approximately and a slight decrease in rate thereafter. Two measures of early grammatical growth (the percentage of obligatory plural contexts in which plurals were used and mean length of utterances) reached high levels well before the age of 2:2. Further, there was no evidence for a relation between the change from one week to the next in the number of new words and the change from one week to the next on the two grammatical measures. Thus, no evidence for competition between lexical and grammatical growth was found on both a larger and a smaller time scale. Patterns of lexical and grammatical growth suggestive of competition may be especially likely when the productive lexicon grows very fast initially.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Fried, Mirjam. "Constructing grammatical meaning." Studies in Language 31, no. 4 (August 14, 2007): 721–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/sl.31.4.02fri.

Full text
Abstract:
In a usage-based analysis of four syntactic reflexives in Czech, this paper examines the question of representing speakers’ knowledge of polyfunctional grammatical categories. I argue that the reflexives form a prototype-based network of partially overlapping grammatical patterns, organized by the pragmatic concept of unexpected referential status in agent–patient relations. This concept is manifested in four distinct communicative functions: marking referential identity between agent and patient roles; distancing discourse participants from their involvement in the reported event; recasting a transitive event as a spontaneous change of state; expressing an attitude toward the reported event. Each function is shown to conventionally co-occur with a set of properties involving various combinations of the following: preferences in aspect and transitivity; semantic and/or pragmatic constraints on agents and patients; verb semantics; shifts in modality and pragmatic force; morphosyntactic constraints. Overall, the analysis supports the view that grammatical categories cannot be properly defined outside of broader grammatical context, thus arguing for a constructional approach to linguistic structure and for re-interpreting the principle of isomorphism in terms of ‘constructions’ in the sense of Construction Grammar.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography