Academic literature on the topic 'Free sorting task'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Free sorting task.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Free sorting task"

1

Evans, A. William, Raegan M. Hoeft, Florian G. Jentsch, and Clint A. Bowers. "Is a Picture Worth a Thousand Words? Investigating Structural Knowledge with Textual and Pictorial Stimuli." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 46, no. 3 (September 2002): 240–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193120204600305.

Full text
Abstract:
The differences between the processing of textual and pictorial information have been a topic of research for some time now. Previous research concerning the modality of information has often concentrated on the speed of processing rather than the organizational differences that may exist. This experiment utilized card sorting to evaluate the changes in knowledge organization that occur when information is presented in text and picture formats. In addition to this, the structure of the elicitation task was manipulated to evaluate its effects on sharedness. It was found that textual stimuli produced a greater sharedness among participants in a free sorting task. However, for a structured sorting task, results reversed, and pictorial stimuli created a greater level of sharedness. Overall, structured sorting tasks produced a greater level of sharedness than the free sorting condition, regardless of modality.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Lahne, Jacob. "Sorting backbone analysis: A network-based method of extracting key actionable information from free-sorting task results." Food Quality and Preference 82 (June 2020): 103870. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103870.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Berland, Aurore, Pascal Gaillard, Michèle Guidetti, and Pascal Barone. "Perception of Everyday Sounds: A Developmental Study of a Free Sorting Task." PLOS ONE 10, no. 2 (February 2, 2015): e0115557. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115557.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Qannari, El Mostafa, Philippe Courcoux, and Pauline Faye. "Significance test of the adjusted Rand index. Application to the free sorting task." Food Quality and Preference 32 (March 2014): 93–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.05.005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Pétel, Cécile, Philippe Courcoux, Noémie Génovesi, Jocelyn Rouillé, Bernard Onno, and Carole Prost. "Free Sorting and Association Task: A Variant of the Free Sorting Method Applied to Study the Impact of Dried Sourdough as an Ingredienton the Related Bread Odor." Journal of Food Science 82, no. 4 (March 7, 2017): 985–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1750-3841.13678.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Davies, Ian, Greville Corbett, Debi Roberson, and Marieta Vandervyver. "Free-Sorting of Colors Across Cultures: Are there Universal Grounds for Grouping?" Journal of Cognition and Culture 5, no. 3-4 (2005): 349–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853705774648536.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThese studies examined naming and free-sorting behavior by informants speaking a wide range of languages, from both industrialized and traditional cultures. Groups of informants, whose color vocabularies varied from 5 to 12 basic terms, were given an unconstrained color grouping task to investigate whether there are systematic differences between cultures in grouping behavior that mirror linguistic differences and, if there are not, what underlying principles might explain any universal tendencies. Despite large differences in color vocabulary, there were substantial similarities in grouping behavior across language groups, and substantial within-language variation across informants. It seems that all informants group stimuli based on some criterion of perceptual similarity, but those with large color vocabularies are more likely to group stimuli in line with their basic color terms. The data are best accounted for by a hybrid system that combines a universal principle of grouping by similarity with culture-specific category salience.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Courcoux, Ph, P. Faye, and E. M. Qannari. "Determination of the consensus partition and cluster analysis of subjects in a free sorting task experiment." Food Quality and Preference 32 (March 2014): 107–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.05.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Cliceri, Danny, Caterina Dinnella, Laurence Depezay, David Morizet, Agnés Giboreau, Katherine M. Appleton, Heather Hartwell, and Erminio Monteleone. "Exploring salient dimensions in a free sorting task: A cross-country study within the elderly population." Food Quality and Preference 60 (September 2017): 19–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.03.006.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Kloosterman, Fabian, Stuart P. Layton, Zhe Chen, and Matthew A. Wilson. "Bayesian decoding using unsorted spikes in the rat hippocampus." Journal of Neurophysiology 111, no. 1 (January 1, 2014): 217–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01046.2012.

Full text
Abstract:
A fundamental task in neuroscience is to understand how neural ensembles represent information. Population decoding is a useful tool to extract information from neuronal populations based on the ensemble spiking activity. We propose a novel Bayesian decoding paradigm to decode unsorted spikes in the rat hippocampus. Our approach uses a direct mapping between spike waveform features and covariates of interest and avoids accumulation of spike sorting errors. Our decoding paradigm is nonparametric, encoding model-free for representing stimuli, and extracts information from all available spikes and their waveform features. We apply the proposed Bayesian decoding algorithm to a position reconstruction task for freely behaving rats based on tetrode recordings of rat hippocampal neuronal activity. Our detailed decoding analyses demonstrate that our approach is efficient and better utilizes the available information in the nonsortable hash than the standard sorting-based decoding algorithm. Our approach can be adapted to an online encoding/decoding framework for applications that require real-time decoding, such as brain-machine interfaces.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Stepanova, Olga, and John Coley. "The Green Eyed Monster: Linguistic Influences on Concepts of Envy and Jealousy in Russian and English." Journal of Cognition and Culture 2, no. 4 (2002): 235–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685370260440991.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe present study examined linguistic relativity in the domain of emotion terms. Exp. 1 showed that American English speakers use the word "jealous" to describe both situations involving envy and those involving jealousy, whereas Russian speakers describe emotions involved in the situations using the Russian terms revnuet and zaviduet in a mutually exclusive manner. Bilinguals performed according to the language they were tested in. In Experiment 2 we sought evidence for conceptual consequences of the difference in how emotion terms mapped onto situations for English and Russian speakers. In a non-linguistic triad sorting task, all subjects clearly distinguished jealousy situations from envy situations, but monolingual English speakers and bilinguals were more likely to see envy situations and jealousy situations as similar than Russian speakers. In a free sorting task, high agreement across all groups was shown in sorting jealousy, envy and control situations. However, native Russian speakers, in contrast to native English speakers, labeled the groups in a mutually-exclusive way. While providing some evidence for the weak view of linguistic relativity, overall the study shows that despite the difference in labeling the emotions of jealousy and envy, Russian speakers, English speakers and bilinguals are very similar in how they conceptualize emotionally-laden situations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Free sorting task"

1

Cliceri, Danny. "New methodological approaches to investigate vegetables preference among consumers." Doctoral thesis, 2018. http://hdl.handle.net/2158/1123300.

Full text
Abstract:
Improved population nutrition is one of the key factors underlying increased health and longevity (Chernoff, 2001) and vegetables are often identified as the most important part of a diet recognized as beneficial for health (Appleton et al., 2016). The adoption of diets rich in vegetables resulted in positive effects not only for human health but also for planetary health. (Whitmee et al., 2015). Dietary change through an increase of plant-based foods to the detriment of animal-based foods has been suggested to be necessary to reduce the environmental impact of the food system (Notarnicola et al., 2017). Despite the recommendation, studies show that a large proportion of consumers consume fewer vegetables than recommended. Therefore, for improved health and diet sustainability, an increased intake of vegetables is required. These interventions should be based on in-depth understanding of the underlying determinants of the preference for vegetables and methodologies aimed to assess consumer preferences and perception in an effective and reliable way. An aspect that links many of the new methodological approaches in sensory and consumer science is the process of categorization. Categorization is a natural cognitive process where objects with common characteristics are grouped and inference is made about their properties, in order to obtain considerable information with minimum cognitive effort (Rosch & Lloyd, 1978). Among the methods based on categorization is the Free sorting task (Lawless et al., 1995), a procedure that can be used with consumers to study similarities among food products. There are a number of methodologies of implicit measure related to categorization, such as the Implicit association test (Greenwald et al., 1998), that find in the process of categorization the operative task carried out by subjects during the implicit test. Considering the relevance of categorization among the approaches to overcome the current limits in sensory and consumer research is therefore of interest in studying issues strongly related to the preference for vegetables with the contribution of the process of categorization. Many issues related to the study of preference for vegetables need attention. For instance, beside the suggestion to consume a specific number of portions of vegetable, of particular importance is the comprehension of which items consumers include in the category “vegetable”, highlighting possible mismatches between guidelines and consumers. Considering these aspects, a deeper comprehension of how consumers represent the vegetable category may be useful to improve the effectiveness of dietary guidelines and increase the consumption of recognized vegetables. This research question was explored in Study I recurring to the prototype theory of categorization, in order to investigate the relationships between the typicality of the dishes and of the dish features with expected liking among consumers. Taking into consideration the increasing interest of interventions targeted at older adults and children to increase vegetable consumption (Appleton et al., 2016), the use of investigative tools that allow evaluation of the perceptions and preferences in an effective and reliable way is needed. In healthy older adults most sensory and consumer methods can be applied (Methven et al., 2016). However the use of consumer tests with this segment of population should be evaluated carefully, due to the possible presence of difficulties related to the comprehension and use of rating scales (Dermiki et al., 2013) and cognitive and perceptive fatigue with long and complex methodologies. A methodology with big potential, yet to be fully explored with older adults is the Free sorting task. Considering these aspects, the study of usability of Free sorting task among healthy older adults would be of interest. This issue was explored in Study II, where older adults from Italy and France were involved in a Free sorting task with peas and sweetcorn samples. Among the different approaches to investigate eating behaviour, the use of interviews and questionnaires may be considered as the most common one, thanks to their relative low cost and ease of submission. However, explicitly measured concepts may suffer from limitations such as voluntary self-presentation strategies (e.g. social desirability), resulting in a discrepancy between declared and actual behavior (Maass et al., 2000). Implicit measurements (De Houwer & Moors, 2010) may overcome some of the problematic elements of traditional self-report measures of attitudes. The use of both explicit and implicit measurements, such as the Implicit Association Test, may therefore be an effective approach to classify with higher reliability consumers’ attitudes toward the vegetable category. This issue was explored in Study III, where vegetarians, flexitarians and omnivores were involved in an Implicit association test intended to assess their attitudes toward plant-based and animal-based dishes and the physiological and psychological variables that may influence these attitudes. Study I concluded that specific dishes, such as salads and boiled vegetables, were more typical of the plant-based dish category than others, such as soups and fried vegetables. Typicality affected expected liking for dishes depending on the consumers’ level of familiarity toward vegetables. Among consumers with a low level of familiarity toward vegetables, the less a dish is typical the higher the expected liking is. No similar relation was found among consumers with a high level of familiarity. Expected sensory attributes were found to influence the typicality of a dish. The attributes Bitter, Green and Bland positively influenced typicality, while Brown, White and Creamy negatively influenced it. The promotion of consumption of vegetables targeted at vegetables dislikers should therefore consider their representation of the category, in order to limit the exposure to features with a negative hedonic value. Study II concluded that the Free sorting task was a suitable method to use with healthy older adults, as it allowed the detection of differences in the categorization of stimuli even among the more aged representatives of the elderly population. Familiarity with the product was the main factor affecting the categorization maps of the tasted vegetables. Categorization maps from the familiar vegetables were found to be reliable to obtain information on sensory and hedonic dimensions, while maps obtained from the unfamiliar vegetables mainly depicted sensory variability. Study III concluded that the Implicit association test is an effective method to study attitudes toward the plant-based dish category. Vegetarians and Flexitarians were more inclined to implicitly associate positive emotions to meat-free dishes than omnivores, with vegetarians showing a stronger association than Flexitarians. Our findings showed that positive attitudes toward meat-free dishes were positively related to the empathic sensitivity toward humans and animals and positive attitudes toward healthy and natural products, whilst being negatively related to bitter responsiveness and sensitivity toward pathogen disgust. Conversely food pleasure emerged as equally important among the considered groups, highlighting a higher importance of food consciousness in determining the eating habits considered. In conclusion, all the methodological approaches considered in this research proved able to satisfy the different research questions related to the study of the preference for vegetables, therefore confirming the effectiveness and reliability of categorization in consumers’ studies.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Free sorting task"

1

Paté, Arthur, Danièle Dubois, and Catherine Guastavino. "Chapter 15. Free sorting task for exploring sensory categories." In Sensory Experiences, 537–72. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/celcr.24.15pat.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Chollet, Sylvie, Dominique Valentin, and Hervé Abdi. "Free Sorting Task." In Novel Techniques in Sensory Characterization and Consumer Profiling, 207–28. CRC Press, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/b16853-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Merlo, Thais Cardoso, Erick Saldaña, Iliani Patinho, Miriam M. Selani, and Carmen J. Contreras-Castillo. "Free sorting task method to optimize the development of smoked bacon: A case study." In Sensory Analysis for the Development of Meat Products, 173–79. Elsevier, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-822832-6.00010-2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Free sorting task"

1

Sokcic-Kostic, Marina, and Roland Schultheis. "Radiological Monitoring Systems for Waste Characterisation in the Environment of Our Decommissioning Solutions." In ASME 2009 12th International Conference on Environmental Remediation and Radioactive Waste Management. ASMEDC, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/icem2009-16013.

Full text
Abstract:
Decommissioning and dismantling of nuclear sites is an increasing business in Europe and worldwide. New methods and instruments are demanded to perform it effectively and economically. NUKEM Technologies has accepted this challenge and has shown that there is a specific solution for each task. Nukem monitoring systems include Bridge Monitors for incoming radwaste, Sorting Monitors to separate low, intermediate and high level waste, Drum/Container Monitors mainly for outgoing waste, Monitors for Room Clearance Measurements and High Throughput Free Release Measurement Systems. The last monitor was designed for a 100% measurement of soil or building material with up to 50.000 kg waste throughput per hour.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography