Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Feedback (Psychology)'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Feedback (Psychology).'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Brewer, Patricia Annette. "Feedback in training: Optimizing the effects of formative feedback timing." Scholarly Commons, 1989. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/3363.
Full textAllen, Andrea. "Informational effects of assessment feedback." FIU Digital Commons, 2001. http://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/etd/1094.
Full textChristie, Gregory J., and University of Lethbridge Faculty of Arts and Science. "Electrophysiological indices of feedback processing." Thesis, Lethbridge, Alta. : University of Lethbridge, Dept. of Neuroscience, 2010, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10133/2551.
Full textxii, 76 leaves : ill. (chiefly col.) ; 29 cm
Roberts, Pamela J. "Should Corrective Feedback Come Before or After Responding to Establish a "New" Behavior?" Thesis, University of North Texas, 1997. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc278439/.
Full textSong, Kye-Chung. "The effects of performance feedback, self-esteem, performance standard on feedback recipient's responses : an attributional analysis." Connect to resource, 1985. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view.cgi?acc%5Fnum=osu1262108814.
Full textChang, Chu-Hsiang. "Effects of transformational leadership on followers' feedback seeking, feedback preference, and reactions to feedback through cognitive and motivational processes." Akron, OH : University of Akron, 2005. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=akron1122910737.
Full text"August, 2005." Title from electronic dissertation title page (viewed 12/14/2005) Advisor, Rosalie J. Hall; Committee members, Paul E. Levy, Robert G. Lord, Ramona Otega-Liston, Aaron M. Schmidt; Department Chair, Paul E. Levy; Dean of the College, Charles B. Monroe; Dean of the Graduate School, George R. Newkome. Includes bibliographical references.
Roberts, Ariel. "ME VERSUS THEM: HOW INDIVIDUALS REACT TO SELF-RELATED AND OTHER-RELATED FEEDBACK." University of Akron / OhioLINK, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=akron1622216925622435.
Full textStallard, Anna. "Increasing the positive effects of negative feedback /." [St. Lucia, Qld.], 2005. http://www.library.uq.edu.au/pdfserve.php?image=thesisabs/absthe18539.pdf.
Full textWaples, Christopher J. "Receptivity to feedback: an investigation of the influence of feedback sign, feedback specificity, and goal orientation." Diss., Kansas State University, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/2097/20107.
Full textPsychological Sciences
Patrick A. Knight
This study was designed to examine the combined influence of feedback sign (i.e., positive or negative), feedback specificity, and goal orientation on individuals’ receptivity to performance feedback. Performance feedback is an often-prescribed solution to performance problems for both individuals and organizations, but evidence regarding its effectiveness as a mechanism for promoting positive outcomes has been mixed. It has been argued that one reason for the inconsistency in previous research findings may be a failure to adequately account for reactions to feedback (e.g., receptivity). Accordingly, this study focused on a series of variables with the potential to influence receptivity, in pursuit of a more comprehensive understanding of the feedback process. It was expected that individuals with certain achievement goal orientations would be more or less receptive to different characteristics of the feedback itself, and that the nature of the task being performed would further influence their willingness to accept feedback and implement task-relevant behavioral changes. Data were collected from 536 participants via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk marketplace. Participants completed the experiment in an online environment. Each participant was asked to complete a pair of error-detection tasks, focused on either mathematical computations or grammatical accuracy. Conditionally-assigned, fabricated feedback was provided after task performance on the initial trial. Surveys were used to assess goal orientation and feedback receptivity. Results indicated that greater feedback specificity was associated with greater receptivity to feedback. Analysis also revealed that feedback sign, feedback specificity, and goal orientation interact to influence receptivity, such that for performance-oriented individuals, specific positive feedback leads to the highest levels of receptivity and specific negative feedback prompts the lowest levels of receptivity. For mastery-oriented participants, however, specific feedback was associated with high levels of receptivity, regardless of whether that feedback was positive or negative. The results are discussed within the context of relative theoretical perspectives. Practical implications, promising avenues of future inquiry, and strengths and limitations of the research are discussed.
Rife, Gary Logan. "THE INFLUENCE OF FEEDBACK ORIENTATION AND FEEDBACK ENVIRONMENT ON CLINICIAN PROCESSING OF FEEDBACK FROM CLIENT OUTCOME MEASURES." University of Akron / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=akron1477989509706831.
Full textHu, Hongzhan. "Exploring the concept of feedback with perspectives from psychology and cognitive science." Thesis, Linköpings universitet, Interaktiva och kognitiva system, 2014. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-107090.
Full textFraser, Elizabeth J. "The influence of self-efficacy and feedback on performance." Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 1993. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/28659.
Full textWalker, Vennessa L. "Effects of rate of feedback and reinforcement on novel responding." Morgantown, W. Va. : [West Virginia University Libraries], 2008. https://eidr.wvu.edu/etd/documentdata.eTD?documentid=5954.
Full textTitle from document title page. Document formatted into pages; contains iv, 75 p. : ill. (some col.). Includes abstract. Includes bibliographical references (p. 37-42).
Nam, Sophia. "Do you agree with this critique? An analysis of the impacts of feedback, feedback acceptance, and fairness perceptions on performance." Thesis, California State University, Long Beach, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=1527736.
Full textPerformance feedback has been widely used to improve performance, motivate employees, and increase organizational effectiveness. However, feedback research has yielded mixed results, ranging from improving performance to decreasing performance. The present study examined the impact of feedback perceptions on a unique sample, university art students, on fairness perceptions and subsequent performance.
Seventy-one art students at a western public university were surveyed immediately following a critique on a draft of their artwork. Feedback was measured by duration and positivity and converted into ratio format for analysis through hierarchical regression. Feedback acceptance and perceptions on fairness of feedback were surveyed. Final versions of the art project scores were collected at the end of the academic semester. In contrast to expectations, perceptions of fairness and feedback acceptance were neither significant moderators nor mediators of performance.
Oden, Kevin Boyd. "Distributed team training effective team feedback /." Orlando, Fla. : University of Central Florida, 2008. http://purl.fcla.edu/fcla/etd/CFE0002483.
Full textDelgado, Kristin M. "INTERACTIVE EFFECTS OF FEEDBACK TYPE AND FEEDBACK PROPENSITIES ON TASK PERFORMANCE." Wright State University / OhioLINK, 2007. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=wright1176735457.
Full textDennis, Matthew Gordon. "Adapting feedback to learner personality to increase motivation." Thesis, University of Aberdeen, 2014. http://digitool.abdn.ac.uk:80/webclient/DeliveryManager?pid=215571.
Full textLee, Grace Leung. "Cultural Differences in the Relationship Between Feedback-Seeking Motives and Feedback-Seeking Behaviors." University of Akron / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=akron1374499755.
Full textWhitcomb, Kaitlyn Elizabeth. "Process Feedback in Group Psychotherapy: A Second Look at Leader Implementation of GQ Feedback." BYU ScholarsArchive, 2016. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/6098.
Full textHall, Margery J. "Task based performance feedback and subsequent worker performance." Diss., This resource online, 1990. http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/theses/available/etd-08252008-162924/.
Full textBailey, Kathleen. "The Effects of Cumulative Consumption Feedback On Demand For Money As A Commodity." Thesis, University of North Texas, 2001. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc2823/.
Full textWoodland, Sean Cameron. "Process Feedback in Group Psychotherapy: A Qualitative Inquiry into Leader Implementation of GQ/OQ Feedback." BYU ScholarsArchive, 2015. https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/etd/5606.
Full textDordevic, Larisa. "Fäkttränares upplevelse av feedback i tävlingssammanhang." Thesis, Högskolan i Halmstad, Akademin för hälsa och välfärd, 2018. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:hh:diva-37995.
Full textPham, Hieu Chi. "Effects of feedback, education, and work experience on self-efficacy." CSUSB ScholarWorks, 2006. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd-project/2985.
Full textYoon, Bang Seob. "Effects of feedback on intrinsic motivation : the role of communication style, feedback message, and individual differences." Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 1991. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/29843.
Full textEaston, Julia Elizabeth. "Teacher Acceptability of Treatment Plan Implementation Monitoring and Feedback Methods." NCSU, 2009. http://www.lib.ncsu.edu/theses/available/etd-05132009-111907/.
Full textWhitaker, Brian. "EXPLICATING THE LINKS BETWEEN THE FEEDBACK ENVIRONMENT, FEEDBACK SEEKING, AND JOB PERFORMANCE." University of Akron / OhioLINK, 2007. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=akron1187456858.
Full textSchnabelrauch, Arndt Chelsea A. "Tailoring feedback and messages to encourage meat consumption reduction." Diss., Kansas State University, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/2097/32159.
Full textDepartment of Psychological Sciences
Laura A. Brannon
Though the research on vegetarianism and meat consumption reduction has dramatically increased over the last few decades, almost all of this research focuses on current vegetarians/meat reducers and non-vegetarians’ perceptions of vegetarianism (Boyle, 2007; Ruby, 2012). Research targeting non-vegetarians and attempting to influence their meat consumption is virtually non-existent. Thus, the intent of the present dissertation was to effectively decrease individuals’ meat consumption habits and alter individuals’ attitudes toward meat. As research has repeatedly found that messages that are tailored to an individual are more persuasive and effective at influencing health behaviors (Bull et al., 2001; Ryan & Lauver, 2002)) and attitudes (e.g., Murray-Johnson & Witte, 2003; Rimal & Adkins, 2003), the present dissertation specifically sought to determine the effectiveness of tailored meat consumption reduction feedback and messages to influence individuals’ intentions to consume meat and attitudes toward meat consumption. Specifically, this dissertation investigated the effectiveness of messages specific to individuals’ behavior (a behavioral feedback approach), messages oriented to individuals’ self-schemas, egoistic and altruistic oriented messages, and feedback/messages tailored to individuals’ values. Contrary to the hypotheses, the results of the present studies suggested that individuals’ willingness to reduce their meat consumption is not differentially affected by different types of feedback/messages; however, this lack of significant impact for tailored feedback/messages may due to various limitations that are discussed at length within the present dissertation. Despite the possible limitations of the studies conducted, the present dissertation has made significant contributions to the meat consumption reduction literature. The first of its kind, this dissertation importantly illustrates the importance of and need for research encouraging meat consumption reduction.
Lucas, Joubert. "Perceptual Feedback, Interoception and Action-Specific Effects." University of Cincinnati / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ucin1447689462.
Full textDonlin, Joanne Mac. "Memory for performance feedback : a test of three self-motivation theories /." Diss., This resource online, 1990. http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/theses/available/etd-08232007-111919/.
Full textJones, Delinda D. "The use of feedback in web-based instruction : achievement, feedback study time, and efficiency /." Electronic version (PDF), 2004. http://dl.uncw.edu/etd/2004/jonesd/delindajones.pdf.
Full textAllen, Andrea, and Andrea Allen Keener. "The therapeutic effects of Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III assessment feedback." FIU Digital Commons, 2004. http://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/etd/1225.
Full textTseng, Pin-ya. "Effects of Compassionate and Self-image Goals on Giving Feedback." The Ohio State University, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1403801126.
Full textHenslee, Amber Marcelle Correia Christopher J. "Providing personalized feedback regarding alcohol use in a group format to college freshmen." Auburn, Ala, 2008. http://repo.lib.auburn.edu/EtdRoot/2008/SUMMER/Psychology/Dissertation/Henslee_Amber_18.pdf.
Full textMarando, Karen A. "An Examination of the Individual Level Antecedents of the Feedback Environment." University of Akron / OhioLINK, 2011. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=akron1321052210.
Full textRensvold, Roger Bury. "The effects of performance, individual differences, and arousal on feedback-seeking behavior in a novel computer-based task." Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 1994. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/30649.
Full textWestling, Magnus. "Effects of secure priming and performance feedback on creativity." Thesis, Stockholms universitet, Psykologiska institutionen, 2017. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-147322.
Full textBaudler, Chris. "The Role of Informal Performance Feedback in Job Satisfaction." Xavier University / OhioLINK, 2013. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=xavier1510247552397143.
Full textLevy, Paul E. "The effects of feedback sign, attributional discrepancy, and performance discrepancy on reactions to feedback." Diss., Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, 1989. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/54232.
Full textPh. D.
Goodman, Jodi Susanne. "The interactive effects of task and external feedback on performance and learning." Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 1996. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/29516.
Full textCochrane, Angela J. "When to correct errors when teaching a new task to children with autism." Thesis, University of North Texas, 2016. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc955059/.
Full textBaker, Rebecca A. "Maximizing Multisource Feedback: The Use of Goal Setting to Facilitate Performance Improvement." NCSU, 2007. http://www.lib.ncsu.edu/theses/available/etd-03262007-113052/.
Full textFlores, Catalina. "Linking Diversity Climate and Feedback Seeking Through Interpersonal Processes and Race Effects." University of Akron / OhioLINK, 2021. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=akron161778390895531.
Full textHawkins, Allyson K. "The effect of goal orientation on counselor trainees' acceptance of positive and negative feedback /." free to MU campus, to others for purchase, 1996. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/mo/fullcit?p9717156.
Full textGardner, Dianne University of New South Wales/Sydney University AGSM UNSW. "The role of feedback about errors in learning a complex novel task." Awarded by:University of New South Wales/Sydney University. AGSM, 2003. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/32230.
Full textKlein, Kevin C. Ostrosky Anthony L. Walbert Mark. "The effect of immediate feedback on learning supply and demand." Normal, Ill. Illinois State University, 1993. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/ilstu/fullcit?p9416868.
Full textTitle from title page screen, viewed March 6, 2006. Dissertation Committee: Tony Ostrosky, Mark Walbert (co-chairs), Mathew Moray, David D. Ramsey, Patricia Klass. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 99-111) and abstract. Also available in print.
Manderscheid, Etienne Gerald. "The influence of auditory feedback on vocal sequence production." Thesis, The University of Chicago, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3627856.
Full textThe fine control of learned movement sequences is a crowning achievement of higher vertebrates. Learned sequences critically depend on sensory feedback, and experimental manipulations of sensory feedback can elucidate its role in sequence acquisition and maintenance. Delayed auditory feedback (DAF) affects production of learned vocalizations in humans and songbirds but has been rarely been studied in songbirds. We used a skull-implanted accelerometer to maintain high amplitude (15-21 dB louder than singing) continuous DAF for 4-68 days in 12 young adult zebra finches. Nine birds sang songs showing new transitions (N = 19; one to five new syllable transitions per bird, e.g. A-B-A or A-B-E ...), including stutters (e.g. A-B-C-D-D) intermixed with songs with normal transitions (e.g. A-B-C-D-E). The mean latency of new transitions was short (7.3 days after DAF onset), presumably due to the high-gain and continuous delivery of DAF.
New transitions to the start of the motif (syllable A) disrupted 10 sites (e.g. the site of syllable B is disrupted by A-B-A), and a disproportionate number of the remaining new transitions occurred at the same site (e.g. A-B-E) instead of the undisrupted sites. Thus, there was statistical evidence that new transitions were concentrated at disrupted sites (χ2 = 7.8, p = 0.005). We observed lengthening of some intersyllable gap durations under DAF, and the minority of sites whose sequence was disrupted accounted for a disproportionate share (79%) of this effect. The average gap lengthening at disrupted sites (e.g the gap following B) was due both to an increase in the duration of the preexisting intersyllable gap (e.g. from B to C), and to the long gap duration of the new transition (e.g. B to E). In fact all 19 new transitions had longer gaps than did the preexisting transitions at the same site.
At a disrupted transition, the timecourse of gap lengthening typically coincided with that of sequence disruption, as gap duration and transition probability covaried over days (r = -0.39, p = 0.0009).
We also observed clear and frequent alterations of the morphology of 18 syllables following DAF. Morphological distortions were quantified, and we found that syllables at disrupted transitions had a trend towards greater average morphological change under DAF than other syllables. This effect became highly significant when we sampled syllable morphologies on the day of maximal sequence change (t(55) = 3.1, p = 0.003). Therefore sequence and morphological changes affected the same sites around the same time. This relation is highly intriguing because the neural pathways governing sequence generation are widely viewed as separate from those controlling the morphology of individual elements.
Thus, the distribution of new transitions, the lengthening of intersyllable gaps and distortions of syllable morphology all implicated a relatively small number of sites (or loci) in the song (14/57) to account for the overwhelming majority of effects. This distribution is reminiscient of human stuttering, where certain phonemes (e.g: k-words , or s-words) may be particularly troublesome.
Crystallization is a developmental stage of birdsong learning characterized by an increase in song tempo and reduced sequence variability. We review a body of evidence suggesting that sensorimotor internal models are learned during this stage to assist vocal sequencing. Our results suggest that DAF causes localized song decrystallization by disrupting these internal models. We propose that human stuttering may result from pathological vocomotor internal models, and that the ability of DAF and related methods to alleviate stuttering in human subjects by as much as 70-90% results from disrupting these internal models. The many parallels between birdsong learning and speech development are discussed and lend plausibility to this hypothesis.
Bush, Jerry J. "Individual differences in the recall of performance appraisal feedback /." This resource online, 1992. http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/theses/available/etd-10062009-020032/.
Full textTomes, Candace N. "Mandated Students Perceptions of Alcohol Related Feedback." Xavier University / OhioLINK, 2017. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=xavier1495569348099438.
Full textGonzalez, Denise Marie. "The effect of feedback on predictions of fear." Scholarly Commons, 2000. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/2698.
Full text