Academic literature on the topic 'Face-voice associations'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Face-voice associations.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Face-voice associations"

1

Uttley, Lesley, Anne Hillairet de Boisferon, Eve Dupierrix, Kang Lee, Paul C. Quinn, Alan M. Slater, and Olivier Pascalis. "Six-month-old infants match other-race faces with a non-native language." International Journal of Behavioral Development 37, no. 2 (January 23, 2013): 84–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025412467583.

Full text
Abstract:
Early in life, infants possess an effective face-processing system which becomes specialized according to the faces present in the environment. Infants are also exposed to the voices and sounds of caregivers. Previous studies have found that face–voice associations become progressively more tuned to the types of association most prevalent in the environment. The present study investigated whether 6-month-old infants associate own-race faces with their native language and faces from a different race with a non-native language. Infants were presented with pictures of own- and other-race faces simultaneously, with a native or non-native language in a habituation paradigm. Results indicate that 6-month-olds are able to match other-race faces to a non-native language.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kachel, Sven, Melanie C. Steffens, Sabine Preuß, and Adrian P. Simpson. "Gender (Conformity) Matters: Cross-Dimensional and Cross-Modal Associations in Sexual Orientation Perception." Journal of Language and Social Psychology 39, no. 1 (November 9, 2019): 40–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0261927x19883902.

Full text
Abstract:
Although sexual orientation (SO) is perceptually ambiguous, people are able to detect it with above-chance accuracy from faces and, sometimes, from voices. Despite a multitude of “gaydar” studies, it is unclear (1) whether vocal or facial signals carry more SO information, (2) whether raters refer to target’s SO instead of gender-role conformity when forming SO impressions, and (3) whether there are any differences for female and male targets. We collected face photographs, voice recordings, and self-reported gender-role conformity of 18 lesbian/gay and straight female and male target persons each. Study 1 (rating of SO) showed that faces led to higher accuracies than voices, which was especially true for female targets. Study 2 (rating of gender-role conformity) showed that the link between self-reported and attributed SO was mediated by self-reported and attributed gender-role conformity. Results support the centrality of gender-role conformity, more than that of SO, in impression formation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Campanella, Salvatore, Raymond Bruyer, Sophie Froidbise, Mandy Rossignol, Frédéric Joassin, Charles Kornreich, Xavier Noël, and Paul Verbanck. "Is two better than one? A cross-modal oddball paradigm reveals greater sensitivity of the P300 to emotional face-voice associations." Clinical Neurophysiology 121, no. 11 (November 2010): 1855–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Li, Shaofeng, Rod Ellis, and Yan Zhu. "The associations between cognitive ability and L2 development under five different instructional conditions." Applied Psycholinguistics 40, no. 03 (April 12, 2019): 693–722. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716418000796.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis study examines the effects of two cognitive abilities—language analytic ability (LAA) and working memory (WM)—on language learning under five different instructional conditions. One hundred fifty eighth-grade English as a foreign language learners underwent a 2-hr treatment session. They were divided into five groups based on whether and when they received form-focused instruction. One group received pretask instruction on the linguistic target (English passive voice) before performing two narrative tasks; a second group received within-task feedback but no pretask instruction; a third group received both pretask instruction and within-task feedback; a fourth group received feedback after completing the tasks; and the fifth group only performed the tasks. The results showed that (a) LAA was predictive of the posttest scores of the group that only performed the communicative tasks and the group who received posttask feedback, (b) WM was associated with the learning outcomes of the two groups receiving within-task feedback, and (c) neither cognitive variable was implicated in the group that received pretask instruction before performing the tasks. The results suggest that the impact of LAA is evident when there is less external assistance and that WM is involved when learners face the heavy processing burden imposed by within-task feedback.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Sievers, Beau, Caitlyn Lee, William Haslett, and Thalia Wheatley. "A multi-sensory code for emotional arousal." Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286, no. 1906 (July 10, 2019): 20190513. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0513.

Full text
Abstract:
People express emotion using their voice, face and movement, as well as through abstract forms as in art, architecture and music. The structure of these expressions often seems intuitively linked to its meaning: romantic poetry is written in flowery curlicues, while the logos of death metal bands use spiky script. Here, we show that these associations are universally understood because they are signalled using a multi-sensory code for emotional arousal. Specifically, variation in the central tendency of the frequency spectrum of a stimulus—its spectral centroid—is used by signal senders to express emotional arousal, and by signal receivers to make emotional arousal judgements. We show that this code is used across sounds, shapes, speech and human body movements, providing a strong multi-sensory signal that can be used to efficiently estimate an agent's level of emotional arousal.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Dánél, Mónika. "Inf(l)ection of the medium." Alphaville: Journal of Film and Screen Media, no. 9 (October 27, 2015): 38–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.33178/alpha.9.03.

Full text
Abstract:
Slitfilm (Résfilm, 2005) and The Gravedigger (A sírásó, 2010) are two Hungarian experimental films made using a slit camera. The director/photographer Sándor Kardos’s adaptations of Ryūnosuke Akutagawa’s short story “The Handkerchief” and of Rainer Maria Rilke’s “The Gravedigger” expose a particular “physiognomy” of the filmic medium through the use of this technique. Likewise, the face as the privileged medial surface for emotion becomes an uncanny, stretched painting with grotesque associations, similar to Francis Bacon’s paintings. The sharp, clear narrator’s voice, layering the literary texts “onto” the moving image further emphasises the colour-stained plasticity of the visible. Both films attempt to articulate a liminal experience: the cultural differences between the East and the West that are inherent in expressing and concealing emotions (Slitfilm) or the questions relating to life and death, the speakable/conceivable and the unspeakable/inconceivable (The Gravedigger) that are embedded in the communicative modalities of social interaction. Through the elastic flow of images, the face and the hand become two uncovered, visible, corporeal surfaces engaged in a rhythmic, chromatic relationship (due to the similar skin tones of face and hand), and thus gradually uncover the medium of the film as a palpable skin surface or violated, wounded flesh. The article approaches the fluid, sensuous imagery that displaces the human towards the inhuman uncanny of the unrecognisable flesh through Deleuzian concepts of fold and inflection.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Hall, Jeffrey. "The Experience of Mobile Entrapment in Daily Life." Journal of Media Psychology 29, no. 3 (July 2017): 148–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000228.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. This multistudy investigation examines how entrapment, which is the guilt, anxiety, or stress to respond and be available to others via mobile devices, shapes and is shaped by patterns of mobile use. Using structural equation modeling on cross-sectional survey responses, Study 1 (N = 300) tested relationships among offline social network size, voice and text frequency, entrapment, and well-being. Offline social network size was associated with text message frequency, and both were indirectly associated with lower subjective well-being via entrapment. Study 2 used experience sampling to confirm associations among entrapment, texting, and well-being. Participants (N = 112) reported on face-to-face, phone, and text interactions five times a day for 5 consecutive days (n = 1,879). Multilevel modeling results indicated that beginning-of-week entrapment was associated with more interactions with acquaintances and strangers, and with reporting lower affective well-being and relatedness when interacting via text. Well-being reported during text interactions and number of interactions with acquaintances and strangers during the week both predicted changes in entrapment by the week’s end. Change in entrapment was associated with lower subjective well-being at the week’s end. Results suggest that entrapment is associated with using texting to maintain larger networks of social relationships, potentially stressing individuals’ capacity to maintain less close relationships via mobile communication.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Amamio, Regie Panadero. "SHE THINKS, HE SAYS: THE VOICE OF THE OTHER IN NOBEL LAUREATES’ GENDERED LITERATURE." International Journal of Humanity Studies (IJHS) 6, no. 2 (March 1, 2023): 288–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.24071/ijhs.v6i2.5426.

Full text
Abstract:
The gendered language system is often used in literary works to present distinct character perspectives however, the issue of representation is accentuated when a writer presents a perspective of a different gender. Kawabata Yasunari and Gabriel Garcia Marquez who depicted female perspectives in their stories, have to face the issue of reliability of representation: theirs is argued asa patriarchal perception of a female’s perspective. Employing Spivak’s argument in Can the Subaltern Speak?, this paper positioned her statement as “through the perspective of the West (men), subaltern (women) become/s dependent on them (men) to speak for their condition rather than allowing them to speak for themselves.” This paper discussed the gendered language by examining the characters’ uncertain finitude utilizing Asher-Greve’s established gender markers to identify gender associations. Withthe stories of the two Nobel Laureates, this paper has established that through exploring the narrators’ usage of gendered language, both writers have inadvertently revealed their own male biases. The narrators of both writers turned out to be the voice of the other not because they have truthfully and successfully spoken for the marginalized; instead, they have become estranged voices of the subjects they are supposed to represent.Hence, the voices that cry for connection and understanding.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Pérès, Karine, Alfonso Zamudio-Rodriguez, Jean-Francois Dartigues, Hélène Amieva, and Stephane Lafitte. "Prospective pragmatic quasi-experimental study to assess the impact and effectiveness of an innovative large-scale public health intervention to foster healthy ageing in place: the SoBeezy program protocol." BMJ Open 11, no. 4 (April 2021): e043082. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-043082.

Full text
Abstract:
IntroductionWith the accelerating pace of ageing, healthy ageing has become a major challenge for all societies worldwide. Based on that Healthy Ageing concept proposed by the WHO, the SoBeezy intervention has been designed through an older person-centred and integrated approach. The programme creates the environments that maximise functional ability to enable people to be and do what they value and to stay at home in best possible conditions.Methods and analysisFive levers are targeted: tackling loneliness, restoring feeling of usefulness, finding solutions to face material daily life difficulties, promoting social participation and combating digital divide. Concretely, the SoBeezy programme relies on: (1) a digital intelligent platform available on smartphone, tablet and computer, but also on a voice assistant specifically developed for people with digital divide; (2) a large solidarity network which potentially relies on everyone’s engagement through a participatory intergenerational approach, where the older persons themselves are not only service receivers but also potential contributors; (3) an engagement of local partners and stakeholders (citizens, associations, artisans and professionals). Organised as a hub, the system connects all the resources of a territory and provides to the older person the best solution to meet his demand. Through a mixed, qualitative and quantitative (before/after analyses and compared to controls) approach, the research programme will assess the impact and effectiveness on healthy ageing, the technical usage, the mechanisms of the intervention and conditions of transferability and scalability.Ethics and disseminationInserm Ethics Committee and the Comité Éthique et Scientifique pour les Recherches, les Études et les Évaluations dans le domaine de la Santé approved this research and collected data will be deposited with a suitable data archive.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Perrodin, Catherine, Christoph Kayser, Nikos K. Logothetis, and Christopher I. Petkov. "Natural asynchronies in audiovisual communication signals regulate neuronal multisensory interactions in voice-sensitive cortex." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112, no. 1 (December 22, 2014): 273–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1412817112.

Full text
Abstract:
When social animals communicate, the onset of informative content in one modality varies considerably relative to the other, such as when visual orofacial movements precede a vocalization. These naturally occurring asynchronies do not disrupt intelligibility or perceptual coherence. However, they occur on time scales where they likely affect integrative neuronal activity in ways that have remained unclear, especially for hierarchically downstream regions in which neurons exhibit temporally imprecise but highly selective responses to communication signals. To address this, we exploited naturally occurring face- and voice-onset asynchronies in primate vocalizations. Using these as stimuli we recorded cortical oscillations and neuronal spiking responses from functional MRI (fMRI)-localized voice-sensitive cortex in the anterior temporal lobe of macaques. We show that the onset of the visual face stimulus resets the phase of low-frequency oscillations, and that the face–voice asynchrony affects the prominence of two key types of neuronal multisensory responses: enhancement or suppression. Our findings show a three-way association between temporal delays in audiovisual communication signals, phase-resetting of ongoing oscillations, and the sign of multisensory responses. The results reveal how natural onset asynchronies in cross-sensory inputs regulate network oscillations and neuronal excitability in the voice-sensitive cortex of macaques, a suggested animal model for human voice areas. These findings also advance predictions on the impact of multisensory input on neuronal processes in face areas and other brain regions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Books on the topic "Face-voice associations"

1

Cardoso, Flávia Pieretti, Maria Leda Pinto, and Léia Teixeira Lacerda. Memória discursiva sobre a violência de gênero na voz de mulheres com deficiência. Brazil Publishing, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.31012/978-65-5861-323-7.

Full text
Abstract:
The work Discursive memory on gender violence in the voice of women with disabilities originated from the Master of Arts in Literature research developed by Flávia Pieretti Cardoso, under the guidance of professors Maria Leda Pinto and Léia Teixeira Lacerda, at the State University of Mato Grosso do Sul. In her role as an interpreter of Brazilian Sign Language at the Casa da Mulher Brasileira and her experience with women from the Association of Women with Disabilities of Mato Grosso do Sul, Flávia was concerned about the invisibility and lack of accessible data and information in the area of gender and disability. The concern resulted in this book, which analyses the speeches of women with disabilities living in Campo Grande / MS, from the theme of gender violence in order to seek possibilities to implement actions to face this type of violence. The theoretical path is based on qualitative research and the corpus analysis grounded on French Discourse Analysis (FDA) studies, as well as on scholars from the Bakhtin Circle, on the analysis of texts of oral communication and the gender and violence area. The analyses presented will enable the reader to conclude that girls and women with disabilities are subject to double exclusion and vulnerability – for having a disability and for being women – by the sexist and capacitist speeches of “power” and “truth”. Therefore, it is a matter of urgency that the Brazilian authorities of power implement effective public programs and policies aimed at the specificities of those subjects.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Face-voice associations"

1

Carneiro, Hugo, Cornelius Weber, and Stefan Wermter. "FaVoA: Face-Voice Association Favours Ambiguous Speaker Detection." In Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 439–50. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-86362-3_36.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Rolls, Edmund T. "Orbitofrontal cortex damage effects in humans and other primates." In The Orbitofrontal Cortex, 130–44. Oxford University Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198845997.003.0004.

Full text
Abstract:
Impairments in the rapid reversal learning of stimulus-reward associations, when expected rewards are not obtained or punishers are obtained, are produced by damage to the orbitofrontal cortex; and contribute to the major changes in emotion, personality, and impulsiveness that can be produced by damage to the orbitofrontal cortex. Impairments in the processing of rewards are found, with alterations in food choice and eating, and in the identification of face and voice expressions, which are important for social behavior.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Olberding, Amy. "Managing the Face." In The Wrong of Rudeness, 112–32. Oxford University Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190880965.003.0007.

Full text
Abstract:
This chapter considers the ways that the style of an action can matter to its meaning. In performing civil gestures and speech, matters such as tone of voice, facial expression, and demeanor influence what we communicate to others. Performing a “polite” action with the wrong bodily expression or tone can undercut or even wholly cancel its politeness. This chapter examines the many ways the style of an action can work on our understanding of the action, suggesting that being civil requires a bodily management we often overlook. The chapter also considers how emphasizing the body’s role in communication can invoke historically problematic associations with disgust and with class- and status-bound assumptions about what causes disgust. The chapter seeks to articulate a way of considering bodily management that evades falling prey to reasoning that encourages pernicious forms of disgust.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Bedford, Charlotte. "Restorative Justice In Action: The Face to Face Documentary." In Making Waves Behind Bars, 141–60. Policy Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1332/policypress/9781529203363.003.0009.

Full text
Abstract:
This chapter details the context, content, and effects of the Face to Face programme in furthering the development of the Prison Radio Association (PRA), and argues that the case demonstrates the potential of prison radio to promote, facilitate, and inform restorative justice practice. First, the chapter examines the restorative justice theme within the contemporary political context before turning to the programme itself, the wider reception, and its impact and significance for the PRA. Through discussion of the increasingly victim-centred reporting of crime within mainstream media, the chapter shows that prison radio not only provides a voice for prisoners, but is able to empower victims of crime.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Face-voice associations"

1

Saeed, Muhammad Saad, Muhammad Haris Khan, Shah Nawaz, Muhammad Haroon Yousaf, and Alessio Del Bue. "Fusion and Orthogonal Projection for Improved Face-Voice Association." In ICASSP 2022 - 2022 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP). IEEE, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icassp43922.2022.9747704.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Zhu, Boqing, Kele Xu, Changjian Wang, Zheng Qin, Tao Sun, Huaimin Wang, and Yuxing Peng. "Unsupervised Voice-Face Representation Learning by Cross-Modal Prototype Contrast." In Thirty-First International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence {IJCAI-22}. California: International Joint Conferences on Artificial Intelligence Organization, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.24963/ijcai.2022/526.

Full text
Abstract:
We present an approach to learn voice-face representations from the talking face videos, without any identity labels. Previous works employ cross-modal instance discrimination tasks to establish the correlation of voice and face. These methods neglect the semantic content of different videos, introducing false-negative pairs as training noise. Furthermore, the positive pairs are constructed based on the natural correlation between audio clips and visual frames. However, this correlation might be weak or inaccurate in a large amount of real-world data, which leads to deviating positives into the contrastive paradigm. To address these issues, we propose the cross-modal prototype contrastive learning (CMPC), which takes advantage of contrastive methods and resists adverse effects of false negatives and deviate positives. On one hand, CMPC could learn the intra-class invariance by constructing semantic-wise positives via unsupervised clustering in different modalities. On the other hand, by comparing the similarities of cross-modal instances from that of cross-modal prototypes, we dynamically recalibrate the unlearnable instances' contribution to overall loss. Experiments show that the proposed approach outperforms state-of-the-art unsupervised methods on various voice-face association evaluation protocols. Additionally, in the low-shot supervision setting, our method also has a significant improvement compared to previous instance-wise contrastive learning.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Wang, Rui, Xin Liu, Yiu-ming Cheung, Kai Cheng, Nannan Wang, and Wentao Fan. "Learning Discriminative Joint Embeddings for Efficient Face and Voice Association." In SIGIR '20: The 43rd International ACM SIGIR conference on research and development in Information Retrieval. New York, NY, USA: ACM, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3397271.3401302.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Chen, Guangyu, Deyuan Zhang, Tao Liu, and Xiaoyong Du. "Self-Lifting: A Novel Framework for Unsupervised Voice-Face Association Learning." In ICMR '22: International Conference on Multimedia Retrieval. New York, NY, USA: ACM, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3512527.3531364.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Wen, Peisong, Qianqian Xu, Yangbangyan Jiang, Zhiyong Yang, Yuan He, and Qingming Huang. "Seeking the Shape of Sound: An Adaptive Framework for Learning Voice-Face Association." In 2021 IEEE/CVF Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR). IEEE, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cvpr46437.2021.01608.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Yu, Zhenning, Xin Liu, Yiu-Ming Cheung, Minghang Zhu, Xing Xu, Nannan Wang, and Taihao Li. "Detach and Enhance: Learning Disentangled Cross-modal Latent Representation for Efficient Face-Voice Association and Matching." In 2022 IEEE International Conference on Data Mining (ICDM). IEEE, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icdm54844.2022.00075.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography