Academic literature on the topic 'Evidence accumulation modelling'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Evidence accumulation modelling.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Evidence accumulation modelling"

1

Karamched, Bhargav, Simon Stolarczyk, Zachary P. Kilpatrick, and Krešimir Josić. "Bayesian Evidence Accumulation on Social Networks." SIAM Journal on Applied Dynamical Systems 19, no. 3 (January 2020): 1884–919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1137/19m1283793.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Tipples, Jason, Michael Lupton, and David George. "Speeding up Time: Hierarchical Bayesian Drift Diffusion Modelling Evidence for Accelerating Temporal Accumulation." Timing & Time Perception 9, no. 4 (April 13, 2021): 393–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10030.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Time perception is malleable ‒ it can be made to speed up and slow down by various experimental manipulations including the presentation of a sequence of auditory clicks and also angry facial expressions. Recent evidence supports the idea that auditory click trains increase accumulation of evidence across time. Here, we test this idea for both angry expressions and auditory clicks by modelling response times (and choice responses) using Bayesian Hierarchical Drift Diffusion Modelling. Two separate groups of participants (Experiment 1; n = 29; Experiment 2; n = 38) judged the duration of angry and neutral facial expressions preceded by either a 3-s sequence of auditory clicks or silence. In both experiments, standard psychophysical analyses showed that both clicks and angry expressions lengthened the perception of time. The original finding came from the analyses of the Drift Diffusion Modelling parameter that represents the speed of information accumulation ‒ the drift rate parameter. Drift rates grew in magnitude with the duration of the face and moreover this effect was larger when the faces were either preceded by clicks or appeared angry ‒ evidence for accelerating temporal accumulation. This novel insight would not have been possible from traditional psychophysical analyses and therefore, the results highlight the potential value of Bayesian Hierarchical Drift Diffusion Modelling as a tool for understanding how we perceive time.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Tipples, Jason. "Rapid temporal accumulation in spider fear: Evidence from hierarchical drift diffusion modelling." Emotion 15, no. 6 (2015): 742–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000079.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Abbott, M. B., J. Larsen, and Jianhua Tao. "Modelling circulations in depth-integrated flows Part 1: The accumulation of the evidence." Journal of Hydraulic Research 23, no. 4 (August 1985): 309–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221688509499342.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Xiao, Xuewei, Honghan Chen, Chunquan Li, Xiuyan Liu, Zecheng Wang, and Hua Jiang. "Fluid Inclusion Evidence for Oil Charge and Cracking in the Cambrian Longwangmiao Dolomite Reservoirs of the Central Sichuan Basin, China." Geofluids 2022 (April 15, 2022): 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2022/3019100.

Full text
Abstract:
The Cambrian Longwangmiao gas reservoirs in the Moxi area of the Sichuan Basin have a complex associated gas accumulation history. Based on core and thin section examination, fluid inclusion analyses and 1-D burial-thermal modelling, the diagenetic evolution and hydrocarbon accumulation processes in the Longwangmiao Formation have been reconstructed. Various diagenetic events were identified, making up a complete pore fill sequence as follows: solid bitumen/dolomite→ nonfluorescent solid bitumen → dolomite → quartz/yellow fluorescent oily bitumen → residual hole. Analyses of oil inclusions and bitumen-bearing inclusions are key to the understanding of the hydrocarbon accumulation processes. The Th values of the aqueous inclusions that are contemporaneous with hydrocarbon inclusions range from 74.3 to 214.3°C. In conjunction with burial-thermal history modelling results, the results indicate that there were two stages of oil charge and three stages of natural gas accumulation in the Longwangmiao carbonate reservoirs. The two stages of oil charge occurred in the Late Silurian and Middle Triassic, respectively. Three gas accumulation events occurred in the Middle to Late Triassic, Middle Jurassic to Early Cretaceous, and Late Cretaceous, respectively.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Davidson, Jacob D., and Ahmed El Hady. "Foraging as an evidence accumulation process." PLOS Computational Biology 15, no. 7 (July 24, 2019): e1007060. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007060.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Markkula, Gustav, Zeynep Uludağ, Richard McGilchrist Wilkie, and Jac Billington. "Accumulation of continuously time-varying sensory evidence constrains neural and behavioral responses in human collision threat detection." PLOS Computational Biology 17, no. 7 (July 15, 2021): e1009096. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009096.

Full text
Abstract:
Evidence accumulation models provide a dominant account of human decision-making, and have been particularly successful at explaining behavioral and neural data in laboratory paradigms using abstract, stationary stimuli. It has been proposed, but with limited in-depth investigation so far, that similar decision-making mechanisms are involved in tasks of a more embodied nature, such as movement and locomotion, by directly accumulating externally measurable sensory quantities of which the precise, typically continuously time-varying, magnitudes are important for successful behavior. Here, we leverage collision threat detection as a task which is ecologically relevant in this sense, but which can also be rigorously observed and modelled in a laboratory setting. Conventionally, it is assumed that humans are limited in this task by a perceptual threshold on the optical expansion rate–the visual looming–of the obstacle. Using concurrent recordings of EEG and behavioral responses, we disprove this conventional assumption, and instead provide strong evidence that humans detect collision threats by accumulating the continuously time-varying visual looming signal. Generalizing existing accumulator model assumptions from stationary to time-varying sensory evidence, we show that our model accounts for previously unexplained empirical observations and full distributions of detection response. We replicate a pre-response centroparietal positivity (CPP) in scalp potentials, which has previously been found to correlate with accumulated decision evidence. In contrast with these existing findings, we show that our model is capable of predicting the onset of the CPP signature rather than its buildup, suggesting that neural evidence accumulation is implemented differently, possibly in distinct brain regions, in collision detection compared to previously studied paradigms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Gesiarz, Filip, Donal Cahill, and Tali Sharot. "Evidence accumulation is biased by motivation: A computational account." PLOS Computational Biology 15, no. 6 (June 27, 2019): e1007089. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007089.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Perea, Manuel, Ana Marcet, Marta Vergara-Martínez, and Pablo Gomez. "On the limits of familiarity accounts in lexical decision: The case of repetition effects." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 73, no. 3 (September 25, 2019): 375–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1747021819878385.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent modelling accounts of the lexical decision task have suggested that the reading system performs evidence accumulation to carry out some functions. Evidence accumulation models have been very successful in accounting for effects in the lexical decision task, including the dissociation of repetition effects for words and nonwords (facilitative for words but inhibitory for nonwords). The familiarity of a repeated item triggers its recognition, which facilitates ‘word’ responses but hampers nonword rejection. However, reports of facilitative repetition effects for nonwords with several repetitions in short blocks challenge this hypothesis and favour models based on episodic retrieval. To shed light on the nature of the repetition effects for nonwords in lexical decision, we conducted four experiments to examine the impact of extra-lexical source of information—we induced the use of episodic retrieval traces via instructions and list composition. When the initial block was long, the repetition effect for nonwords was inhibitory, regardless of the instructions and list composition. However, the inhibitory effect was dramatically reduced when the initial block included two presentations of the stimuli and it was even facilitatory when the initial block was short. This composite pattern suggests that evidence accumulation models of lexical decision should take into account all sources of evidence—including episodic retrieval—during the process of lexical decision.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

John, Amber, Sarah-Naomi James, Urvisha Patel, Jennifer Rusted, Marcus Richards, and Darya Gaysina. "Longitudinal associations of affective symptoms with mid-life cognitive function: evidence from a British birth cohort." British Journal of Psychiatry 215, no. 5 (March 21, 2019): 675–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.24.

Full text
Abstract:
BackgroundAffective disorders are associated with poorer cognition in older adults; however, whether this association can already be observed in mid-life remains unclear.AimsTo investigate the effects of affective symptoms over a period of 30 years on mid-life cognitive function. First, we explored whether timing (sensitive period) or persistence (accumulation) of affective symptoms predicted cognitive function. Second, we tested how different longitudinal trajectories of affective symptoms were associated with cognitive function.MethodThe study used data from the National Child Development Study. Memory, verbal fluency, information processing speed and accuracy were measured at age 50. Affective symptoms were measured at ages 23, 33, 42 and 50 and used to derive longitudinal trajectories. A structured modelling approach compared a set of nested models in order to test accumulation versus sensitive period hypotheses. Linear regressions and structural equation modelling were used to test for longitudinal associations of affective symptoms with cognitive function.ResultsAccumulation of affective symptoms was found to be the best fit for the data, with persistent affective symptoms being associated with poorer immediate memory (b = −0.07, s.e. = 0.03, P = 0.01), delayed memory (b = −0.13, s.e. = 0.04, P < 0.001) and information processing accuracy (b = 0.18, s.e. = 0.08, P = 0.03), but not with information processing speed (b = 3.15, s.e. = 1.89, P = 0.10). Longitudinal trajectories of repeated affective symptoms were associated with poorer memory, verbal fluency and information processing accuracy.ConclusionsPersistent affective symptoms can affect cognitive function in mid-life. Effective management of affective disorders to prevent recurrence may reduce risk of poor cognitive outcomes and promote healthy cognitive ageing.Declaration of interestNone.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Evidence accumulation modelling"

1

Minkley, Michael. "Ironing out the pathophysiology of neurodegeneration with brain iron accumulation (NBIA) : clinical investigations and disease modelling yield novel evidence of systemic dysfunction and provide a robust and accurate disease model of NBIA." Thesis, 2018. https://dspace.library.uvic.ca//handle/1828/9321.

Full text
Abstract:
Neurodegeneration with Brain Iron Accumulation (NBIA) disorders, such as Phospholipase A2G6-Associated Neurodegeneration (PLAN) and Pantothenate Kinase-Associated Neurodegeneration (PKAN), are a group of rare early-onset, genetic disorders characterized by neurodegeneration and iron accumulation inside of the basal ganglia (BG), which is accompanied by progressive motor symptoms. In order to address the limitations in available models of NBIA, a B6.C3-Pla2g6m1J/CxRwb mouse model of PLAN was characterized. This model demonstrated key hallmarks of the disease presentation in NBIA, including a severe and early-onset motor deficit, neurodegeneration inside of the substantia nigra (SN) including a loss of dopaminergic function and the formation of abnormal spheroid inclusions as well as iron accumulation. The capture of these hallmarks of NBIA makes this an ideal animal research model for NBIA. Additionally, exploration of candidate systemic biomarkers of NBIA was performed in a case study of a patient with PLAN and in a cohort of 30 patients with PKAN. These investigations demonstrated reductions in transfer and slight, but not significant elevations in soluble transferrin receptor. No significant difference was seen in serum iron parameters. A systemic disease burden including chronic oxidative stress; elevated malondialdehyde, and inflammation; elevated C-reactive protein (CRP), IL-6 and TNFα was noted in both investigations. A number of candidate protein biomarkers including: fibrinogen, transthyretin, zinc alpha-2 glycoprotein and retinol binding protein were also identified. These markers correlated with measures of the severity of iron loading in the globus pallidus (GP); based on R2* magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and the severity of motor symptoms (Barry-Albright Dystonia Rating Scale) making them potential candidates markers of dysfunction in NBIA. In the patient with PLAN, 37 weeks of therapy with the iron chelator deferiprone (DFP) as well as 20 months of therapy with the antioxidants alpha lipoic acid (ALA) and n-acetylcysteine (NAC) were efficacious in reducing the systemic oxidative and inflammatory disease burden, but it did not significantly alter the progression of the disease. In the antioxidant therapy, this efficacy was primarily due to ALA. When the cohort of patients with PKAN were treated with DFP for 18 months it was highly efficacious in lowering brain iron accumulation in the GP. No significant reduction in the speed of disease progression was seen in DFP treated patients compared to placebo based on initial analysis. Similar to the PLAN patient, DFP also mitigated the systemic disease burden in PKAN patients. In both cases DFP was well tolerated and had minimal impact on serum iron levels, TIBC and transferrin saturation. Collectively these investigations provide valuable insights into disease progression in NBIA. They also provide tools to aid further investigations in NBIA. These are provided in the form of a well-characterized B6.C3-Pla2g6m1J/CxRwb model of PLAN, which robustly captures the disease presentation seen in patients, as well as a panel of systemic blood-based markers of disease burden in NBIA and candidate markers of dysfunction in NBIA. These markers were used to assess two novel therapies in NBIA chelation with DFP and antioxidant therapy with ALA and NAC.
Graduate
2019-04-19
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Evidence accumulation modelling"

1

Zainal Abidin, Faieqah, Rozaidi Toha, M. Nizam M. Zin, M. Baihaky M. Abdullah, Remy Azrai M. Amin, and M. Solehuddin A. Razak. "Minimizing Salt Creeping Through Geomechanics Application and Systematic Drilling Fluids Design in Ultra Deepwater Wells - Operator's First Experience in West Africa." In ADIPEC. SPE, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.2118/211743-ms.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In 2017, 2019 and 2021, an Operator drilled 3 ultra-deepwater exploration wells in offshore West Africa. The water depth for well A is around 2900m, well B is around 2800m while well C is circa 2100m. The objective of these wells is to evaluate the potential hydrocarbon accumulation in the subsurface area. Besides the usual challenges being wildcat wells and located far offshore, the wells drilled through pre-salt formations. Hole enlargement due to salt dissolution is also a concern apart from salt movement to close the hole owing to its plastic nature. Furthermore, the lack of offset wells for determining wellbore behavior also caused another challenge. Experiences from the nearby wells showed many signs of wellbore instability such as tight spots and losses. The common causes of wellbore instability are unbalanced stress dependent on stress change and rock strength, and shale-fluid interaction due to hydration of clays. A geomechanical model was constructed and detailed salt creep modelling was developed. Well A was drilled until the final target depth with minimal wellbore instability and encountered no losses at all while drilling. Well B which was drilled later, also drilled to the final target depth with very minimal wellbore instability. Well C was drilled with longer salt exposure around 490m. The mud weight selected for all wells also successfully minimized salt creeping. These are strongly evident while drilling and running in casing, no tight spots were encountered. Both wells successfully drilled through salt formation with very minimal salt dissolution. Through the application of geomechanics and systematic drilling fluids design reduce potential non-productive time (NPT) related to salt plastic movement that could be faced by Operator eventually saving Operator's drilling hours.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography