Journal articles on the topic 'Dog temperament'

To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Dog temperament.

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 49 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Dog temperament.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Ruben, J. "German shepherd dog temperament." Veterinary Record 120, no. 20 (May 15, 1987): 488. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/vr.120.20.488.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Goodloe, Linda P., and Peter L. Borchelt. "Companion Dog Temperament Traits." Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1, no. 4 (October 1998): 303–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0104_1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Łapiński, Stanisław, Joanna Bzymek, Piotr Niedbała, Łukasz Migdał, Andrzej Zoń, and Marcin Lis. "Effect of Age and Temperament Type on Reproductive Parameters of Female Raccoon Dogs (Nyctereutes Procyonoides Gray) / Wpływ Wieku I Temperamentu Na Parametry Rozrodcze Samic Jenota (Nyctereutes Procyonoides Gray)." Annals of Animal Science 13, no. 4 (September 1, 2013): 807–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/aoas-2013-0021.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The aim of the study was to determine the relationship between age and temperament as well as reproductive results in female raccoon dogs. The study was carried out at two raccoon dog breeding farms located in south-eastern Poland. A total of 189 foundation stock females were evaluated for temperament using a modified behavioural empathy test. Animals were classified into five temperament groups: very fearful (VF), fearful (F), confident (C), aggressive (A) and very aggressive (VA). The animals with calm temperament (C) formed the largest group (49.9%) whereas the smallest number of animals was classified as VA (5.6%). The influence of age on the temperament of raccoon dog females and the number of young born and weaned (P≤0.01) was revealed. At the same time, temperament did not affect reproductive parameters (P>0.05). Summing up, the results of this study indicate lack of correlation between temperament and reproductive parameters. It can be assumed that the elimination of aggressive animals from the foundation stock will not compromise production results and can help to facilitate handling and improve animal welfare.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Jakuba, Tomáš, Zuzana Polcová, Denisa Fedáková, Jana Kottferová, Jana Mareková, Magdaléna Fejsáková, Olga Ondrašovičová, and Miloslav Ondrašovič. "Differences in Evaluation of a Dog’s Temperament by Individual Members of the Same Household." Society & Animals 21, no. 6 (2013): 582–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341314.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract A questionnaire is an integral component of methods determining the temperaments of dogs. From the range of questionnaires used for evaluation of a dog’s temperament, we selected C-BARQ. This particular type of questionnaire allowed us to determine the degree of agreement of evaluations of the same dog by individual members of one household. The evaluation included dogs in 29 households with a total of 71 members. The degree of agreement between ratings of individual members of the same household was determined by Intraclass correlations coefficient (ICC). The correlations observed ranged between 0.53 and 0.9, depending on the category. This result is further supported by the results of other studies that showed similar perceptions of the same dog between family members.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Kusakina, E. A., and R. V. Malchikov. "Psychophysiological compatibility of a specialist cynologist and service dog." Penitentiary science 13, no. 3 (December 9, 2019): 416–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.46741/2686-9764-2019-13-3-416-422.

Full text
Abstract:
The article discusses the results of the study devoted to one of the most important aspects of service dog breeding – the psychophysiological compatibility of a specialist cynologist and a dog. The main methods for collecting empirical material were the system of checking the dynamic characteristics of ARAKS and the methodology for determining the characteristics of the temperament of cynologists, developed by H. Eysenck. The results of the study emphasize the direct influence of the compatibility of the temperament of the cynologist and the service dog on the results of training and joint performance of service tasks. In institutions of the penal system in order to increase the effectiveness of the cynological service it is advisable to take into account the psychophysiological features in the formed cynologist-dog pairs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Davis, Aaron L., David C. Schwebel, Barbara A. Morrongiello, Julia Stewart, and Melissa Bell. "Dog Bite Risk: An Assessment of Child Temperament and Child-Dog Interactions." International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 9, no. 8 (August 20, 2012): 3002–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph9083002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Robson, Howard. "Dog Guide and Blind Person: The Matching Process." Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness 79, no. 8 (October 1985): 356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0145482x8507900806.

Full text
Abstract:
A discussion of the variations of temperament and physique exhibited by dog guides and potential masters that must be considered when they are paired. The author recommends keeping these considerations on a simple, factual level, noting the animal's sensitivity, its future surroundings, and the blind person's capacity for controlling the dog.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Tomkins, L. M., P. C. Thomson, and P. D. McGreevy. "Lateralization and temperament tests can predict guide dog success." Journal of Veterinary Behavior 6, no. 1 (January 2011): 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2010.08.028.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Schneider, Luke A., Paul H. Delfabbro, and Nicholas R. Burns. "Temperament and lateralization in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)." Journal of Veterinary Behavior 8, no. 3 (May 2013): 124–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2012.06.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Karpiński, Mirosław, Justyna Wojtaś, and Aleksandra Garbiec. "Temperament Assessment Algorithm in Dogs." Animals 12, no. 5 (March 2, 2022): 634. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani12050634.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this study was to evaluate the temperament of dogs on the basis of behavioral observations, with emphasis on 24 selected traits and behaviors. From the observations, the temperament of the dogs was determined and the animals were assigned to one of two personality groups: introvert or extrovert. The study involved 46 dogs. The agglomeration method, Pearson’s 1-r distance, and Ward’s binding method were used. As shown by the statistical analysis, 18 dogs (39%) were assessed as introverts and 28 dogs (61%) exhibited extrovert traits. To construct a model for the assessment of canine temperament using the identified traits, logistic regression was performed with the independent variables, number of extrovert traits (ETs) and introvert traits (ITs), and a dichotomous dependent variable (1 = extrovert, 0 = introvert), reflecting the assessment of the temperament of the dog based on the observations and results of the original questionnaire.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Bray, Emily E., Mary D. Sammel, Dorothy L. Cheney, James A. Serpell, and Robert M. Seyfarth. "Effects of maternal investment, temperament, and cognition on guide dog success." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, no. 34 (August 7, 2017): 9128–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704303114.

Full text
Abstract:
A continuing debate in studies of social development in both humans and other animals is the extent to which early life experiences affect adult behavior. Also unclear are the relative contributions of cognitive skills (“intelligence”) and temperament for successful outcomes. Guide dogs are particularly suited to research on these questions. To succeed as a guide dog, individuals must accomplish complex navigation and decision making without succumbing to distractions and unforeseen obstacles. Faced with these rigorous demands, only ∼70% of dogs that enter training ultimately achieve success. What predicts success as a guide dog? To address these questions, we followed 98 puppies from birth to adulthood. We found that high levels of overall maternal behavior were linked with a higher likelihood of program failure. Furthermore, mothers whose nursing style required greater effort by puppies were more likely to produce successful offspring, whereas mothers whose nursing style required less effort were more likely to produce offspring that failed. In young adults, an inability to solve a multistep task quickly, compounded with high levels of perseveration during the task, was associated with failure. Young adults that were released from the program also appeared more anxious, as indicated by a short latency to vocalize when faced with a novel object task. Our results suggest that both maternal nursing behavior and individual traits of cognition and temperament are associated with guide dog success.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Riemer, Stefanie, Corsin Müller, Ludwig Huber, Friederike Range, Erik Kersting, and Zsófia Virányi. "Can early temperament tests predict behavioral tendencies in dog puppies?" Journal of Veterinary Behavior 6, no. 1 (January 2011): 79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2010.09.012.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Lopes, Luiz Patricio, and Sandra Márcia Tietz Marques. "Cães operacionais no sistema prisional do Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil." Revista Agraria Academica 5, no. 2 (March 1, 2022): 11–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.32406/v5n2/2022/11-25/agrariacad.

Full text
Abstract:
Intervention dogs in prison units are fundamental in helping operations that demand action in situations of stress and controlled force with the prison population. The prison system is an area where the dog can be used the most, whether for intervention, scent, searching and operations to inhibit drug trafficking. This mission is performed by the canine technician and his dogs. Dogs are distinguished by racial types, temperament and learning characteristics, which make them excellent working partners. This article contributes with the description of specifications for the use of the dog in the prison system of the state of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil, regarding the regulation and the legal foundation, the profile of the operational dog, the training, the security of the dog-police binomial in routine tasks, with the dog playing a supporting role in tactical employment and the talent of the canine technician who leads him on a leash.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Concas, Maria Pina, Alessandra Minelli, Susanna Aere, Anna Morgan, Paola Tesolin, Paolo Gasparini, Massimo Gennarelli, and Giorgia Girotto. "Genetic Dissection of Temperament Personality Traits in Italian Isolates." Genes 13, no. 1 (December 21, 2021): 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/genes13010004.

Full text
Abstract:
Human personality (i.e., temperament and character) is a complex trait related to mental health, influenced by genetic and environmental factors. Despite the efforts performed during the past decades, its genetic background is only just beginning to be identified. With the aim of dissecting the genetic basis of temperament, we performed a Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) on Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory in 587 individuals belonging to different Italian genetic isolates. Data analysis led to the identification of four new genes associated with different temperament scales, such as Novelty Seeking (NS), Harm Avoidance (HA), and Reward Dependence (RD). In detail, we identified suggestive and significant associations between: MAGI2 (highest p-value = 9.14 × 10−8), a gene already associated with schizophrenia and depressive disorder, and the NS–Extravagance scale; CALCB (highest p-value = 4.34 × 10−6), a gene likely involved in the behavioral evolution from wild wolf to domestic dog, and the NS–Disorderliness scale; BTBD3 (highest p-value = 2.152 × 10−8), a gene already linked to obsessive–compulsive disorder, and the HA–Fatigability scale; PRKN (highest p-value = 8.27 × 10−9), a gene described for early onset Parkinson’s disease, and the RD scale. Our work provides new relevant insights into the genetics of temperament, helping to elucidate the molecular basis of psychiatric disorders.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Blackman, Simone A., Bethany J. Wilson, Alistair R. Reed, and Paul D. McGreevy. "Reported Acquisition Practices of Australian Dog Owners." Animals 9, no. 12 (December 17, 2019): 1157. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani9121157.

Full text
Abstract:
In Australia, the UK and the US dog ownership is prevalent with an estimated 40% of Australian households, 25% of UK households, and 50% of US households owning a dog. Once acquired, a dog usually becomes a family companion so, unlike a faulty product, it can rarely be returned or resold without some emotional impact on both the acquirer and the dog. Regarding the reality of dog relinquishment, there is a growing need for cross-disciplinary research that considers how dog owners are making their acquisition choices and, if prioritising different attributes, leads to more optimal acquisition choices. This research collected data from 2840 dog owners via an online survey and examines how owners prioritised various attributes when acquiring their latest dog. The Pearson-Blotchky analysis of survey results show owners are split into two groups, with each group prioritising different attributes or characteristics in their search for a new dog. The first group are those dog owners who prioritised: the ability to rescue a dog, how compatible the dog was on the first meeting, and how compatible they believed the dog would be with their household. The second group are those owners who prioritised: a dog’s morphology, temperament predictability, and breeding practices. While each group prioritised different attributes, neither group made substantially more optimal acquisition choices in terms of overall satisfaction with the dog that they ultimately selected.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Glickman, LT, NW Glickman, DB Schellenberg, K. Simpson, and GC Lantz. "Multiple risk factors for the gastric dilatation-volvulus syndrome in dogs: a practitioner/owner case-control study." Journal of the American Animal Hospital Association 33, no. 3 (May 1, 1997): 197–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.5326/15473317-33-3-197.

Full text
Abstract:
A study was conducted of 101 dogs (i.e., case dogs) that had acute episodes of gastric dilatation-volvulus (GDV) and 101 dogs (i.e., control dogs) with nonGDV-related problems. The control dogs were matched individually to case dogs by breed or size, and age. Predisposing factors that significantly (p less than 0.10) increased a dog's risk of GDV were male gender, being underweight, eating one meal daily, eating rapidly, and a fearful temperament. Predisposing factors that decreased the risk of GDV significantly were a "happy" temperament and inclusion of table foods in a usual diet consisting primarily of dry dog food. The only factor that appeared to precipitate an acute episode of GDV was stress.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Muñiz de Miguel, Susana, Francisco Javier Diéguez, Joao Pedro da Silva-Monteiro, Beatriz Parra Ferreiro-Mazón, and Ángela González-Martínez. "Study of Behavioural Traits in Can de Palleiro (Galician Shepherd Dog)." Animals 11, no. 11 (November 9, 2021): 3198. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani11113198.

Full text
Abstract:
The Can de Palleiro (CP) is an autochthonous canine breed from Galicia (NW Spain). Interestingly, no previous research has been published about the behaviour of this breed. Thus, the aim of the present study was to obtain a deeper understanding of CP behavioural and temperamental traits and detect any potentially problematic behaviour by using the Canine Behavioural Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) and the Socially Acceptable Behaviour (SAB) test. Behavioural information was obtained from 377 dogs—177 CPs and 200 general population (GP) dogs—using the C-BARQ. Additionally, 32 dogs were enrolled to perform the SAB test (19 CPs and 13 GP dogs) in order to directly evaluate their temperament. Our results indicated that CP dogs had a lower tendency to show aggressiveness towards their owners (0.18 times lower, p = 0.033) and less fear of other dogs (by 0.43 times, p = 0.001), as well as higher trainability levels (2.56 times higher, p < 0.001) when compared to GP dogs. CP dogs also had increased odds of showing chasing behaviour (3.81 times higher, p < 0.001). Conversely, CPs had reduced odds of non-social fear, separation-related problems and excitability (by 0.42, 0.35 and 0.48 times, respectively; p < 0.001, p < 0.001 and p = 0.002). The current research represents a starting point for the study of the behaviour of CPs, which appear to be a working breed, with guarding and, especially, herding characteristics.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Jensen, Clare L., Kerri E. Rodriguez, Evan L. MacLean, Ahmad Hakeem Abdul Wahab, Arman Sabbaghi, and Marguerite E. O’Haire. "Characterizing veteran and PTSD service dog teams: Exploring potential mechanisms of symptom change and canine predictors of efficacy." PLOS ONE 17, no. 7 (July 27, 2022): e0269186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269186.

Full text
Abstract:
Psychiatric service dogs are an emerging complementary intervention for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Initial evidence suggests that partnership with a service dog may be related to less PTSD symptom severity. However, it remains unclear how or why this might occur. To address this gap, we conducted a longitudinal investigation of 82 post-9/11 military members or veterans and their PTSD service dogs to (1) evaluate service dog characteristics as potential predictors of efficacy, (2) assess dog and human characteristics as potential predictors of veteran-dog bond, and (3) explore potential mechanisms for mental health outcomes. Aim 1 results demonstrated that most service dog characteristics did not predict veterans’ mental health outcomes, but lower service dog excitability was associated with less PTSD symptom severity at follow-up. Aim 2 results showed that closer dog-veteran relationships were associated with less excitable dog temperament. Aim 3 results indicated that worse mental health at follow-up was associated with greater use of the specifically trained PTSD service dog task to initiate a social greeting (“make a friend”), whereas better mental health was related to less use of dominance-based training methods, lower perceived emotional/logistical costs of service dog partnership, and closer veteran-dog relationships. More frequent use of the trained service dog task to signal when someone approaches from behind (cover/watch back) was associated with greater anxiety, but less PTSD symptom severity. Overall, veterans spent an average of 82% of their time with service dogs (assessed via Bluetooth proximity between dog collar and veteran smartphone), and most frequently asked their service dogs to perform the trained task for calming their anxiety (calm/comfort anxiety). The present study provides subjective and objective metrics of the heterogeneity among veteran-service dog dyads while also suggesting which of the assessed metrics might be potential mechanisms involved in the intervention.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Jakeman, Molly, James A. Oxley, Sara C. Owczarczak-Garstecka, and Carri Westgarth. "Pet dog bites in children: management and prevention." BMJ Paediatrics Open 4, no. 1 (August 2020): e000726. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmjpo-2020-000726.

Full text
Abstract:
Dog bite injuries are a significant public health problem and many are sustained by children. These injuries can be complex, both physically and psychologically, and in rare cases fatal. This paper will review current evidence-based approaches to treatment, explore identified patterns in biting incidents and discuss the effectiveness of prevention strategies. Safe management of these patients requires a comprehensive approach. Physical injuries need to be accurately assessed with a high index of suspicion for underlying injuries, particularly in younger children less able to communicate. Treatment depends on severity and location, but all bites must be irrigated to reduce the risk of infection but may not always require prophylactic antibiotic use. Careful exploration of the circumstances in which the bite occurred is essential to make safeguarding decisions and prevent future bites. Reducing the incidence of paediatric dog bites requires education of both children and parents that any dog can bite, regardless of breed, and all child–dog interactions must be highly supervised. However, education alone is unlikely to prevent dog bites. Policies that support environmental changes need to be developed such as provision of pet dogs less likely to bite (or bite as severely), through breeding for temperament and appropriate socialisation. Additionally, investment in psychological support for bite victims and their families is required to reduce the long-term impacts of being bitten.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Barnard, Shanis, Danielle Kennedy, Reuben Watson, Paola Valsecchi, and Gareth Arnott. "Revisiting a Previously Validated Temperament Test in Shelter Dogs, Including an Examination of the Use of Fake Model Dogs to Assess Conspecific Sociability." Animals 9, no. 10 (October 20, 2019): 835. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani9100835.

Full text
Abstract:
This study assessed the feasibility and reproducibility of a previously validated temperament test (TT) for shelter dogs. The test was developed to measure dog behaviour in the kennel, and traits of sociability towards people and other dogs, docility to leash, playfulness, cognitive skills, and reactivity. We introduced the use of differently sized fake dogs to check their appropriateness in correctly assessing sociability to dogs to broaden its applicability (as the original study used real stimulus dogs). We hypothesised that dogs’ responses may be modulated by the body size of the stimulus dog presented. The reduction analysis of the TT scores extracted five main dimensions (explaining 70.8% of variance), with high internal consistency (alpha > 0.65) and being broadly consistent with existing research. Behavioural components that were extracted from the fake dog experiment showed that dogs are likely to show signs of anxiety and fear toward both the real and fake dog. Dogs’ responses towards a real vs. fake stimulus were significantly correlated (p < 0.05) and they were not affected by the size of the stimulus (p > 0.05). We discuss the importance of interpreting these data with caution and use behavioural tests as a partial screening tool to be used in conjunction with more extensive behavioural and welfare monitoring.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Hecht, E. E., I. Zapata, C. E. Alvarez, D. A. Gutman, T. M. Preuss, M. Kent, and J. A. Serpell. "Neurodevelopmental scaling is a major driver of brain–behavior differences in temperament across dog breeds." Brain Structure and Function 226, no. 8 (August 29, 2021): 2725–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-021-02368-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Mota-Rojas, Daniel, Chiara Mariti, Míriam Marcet-Rius, Karina Lezama-García, Angelo Gazzano, Ismael Hernández-Ávalos, Patricia Mora-Medina, Adriana Domínguez-Oliva, and Alexandra L. Whittaker. "The Welfare of Fighting Dogs: Wounds, Neurobiology of Pain, Legal Aspects and the Potential Role of the Veterinary Profession." Animals 12, no. 17 (August 31, 2022): 2257. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani12172257.

Full text
Abstract:
Throughout history it has been common to practice activities which significantly impact on animal welfare. Animal fighting, including dogfighting, is a prime example where animals often require veterinary care, either to treat wounds and fractures or to manage pain associated with tissue and where death may even result. Amongst the detrimental health effects arising are the sensory alterations that these injuries cause, which not only include acute or chronic pain but can also trigger a greater sensitivity to other harmful (hyperalgesia) or even innocuous stimuli (allodynia). These neurobiological aspects are often ignored and the erroneous assumption made that the breeds engaged in organized fighting have a high pain threshold or, at least, they present reduced or delayed responses to painful stimuli. However, it is now widely recognized that the damage these dogs suffer is not only physical but psychological, emotional, and sensory. Due to the impact fighting has on canine welfare, it is necessary to propose solution strategies, especially educational ones, i.e., educating people and training veterinarians, the latter potentially playing a key role in alerting people to all dog welfare issues. Therefore, the aim of this review is to describe the risk factors associated with dogfighting generally (dog temperament, age, sex, nutrition, testosterone levels, environment, isolation conditions, socialization, education, or training). A neurobiological approach to this topic is taken to discuss the impact on dog pain and emotion. Finally, a general discussion of the format of guidelines and laws that seek to sanction them is presented. The role that veterinarians can play in advancing dog welfare, rehabilitating dogs, and educating the public is also considered.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Pustova, N. V., T. Y. Potapenko, and K. V. Seleman. "METHODS AND TECHNIQUES OF TRAINING OF OFFICIAL DOGS." Scientific and Technical Bulletin оf State Scientific Research Control Institute of Veterinary Medical Products and Fodder Additives аnd Institute of Animal Biology 22, no. 2 (October 7, 2021): 300–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.36359/scivp.2021-22-2.35.

Full text
Abstract:
Training of official dogs takes place in a result from the action of certain methods, methodologies, techniques, receptions and complex of methods on the organism of dog from early age and in particular on the nervous system, that together with the inherited signs forms for the puppy-dog of basis of activity of the nervous system on necessary to the trainer internalss and skills. The aim of our researches was to define the basic methods of training of official dogs in the cynologistic center of the Khmelnytsk area. Due to a selection and selection a man formed desirable internalss at the different breeds of dogs, sometimes nature strengthened properties of certain orientation, for today to the basic breeds of official dogs belong: the German, Caucasian and Belgian shepherds, doberman, rottweiler and other. During researches studied the purchased experience of the use of the marked breeds of official dogs at implementation by them such tasks: to the search and watching, being of explosive, drugs, weapon and others like that. Analysing the got results of implementation of tasks the different breeds of official dogs (in accompaniment with a cynologist) it was educed that taking into account genetic potential and individual features of every animal determine an official dog will work in what direction. The methods of training of dogs in a cynologistic center apply such: mechanical, encouraging, contrasting, inheritance. Taking into account the different methods and techniques of training of official dogs in their basis, making of pavlovian reflexes lies on certain irritants, however in basis of training there is encouragement (delicious, by a toy, stroking, and others like that) : did right - got a reward, wrong - got nothing. Training of official dogs conduct, in an order to produce the special skills for a dog. Not depending on a breed official dogs are able to watch and detain criminals, find a weapon, explosive and drugs, search disappearing people and many other different tasks. Direction an official dog will work after that is determined by his nature and temperament.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Santaniello, Antonio, Susanne Garzillo, Serena Cristiano, Alessandro Fioretti, and Lucia Francesca Menna. "The Research of Standardized Protocols for Dog Involvement in Animal-Assisted Therapy: A Systematic Review." Animals 11, no. 9 (September 2, 2021): 2576. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani11092576.

Full text
Abstract:
Dogs are considered the most important species involved in animal-assisted therapy (AAT), and the scientific literature focuses on the benefits linked to the involvement of dogs in various therapeutic areas. In this study, we carried out a systematic review according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, exploring the scientific literature from the last 5 years (2016–2021) on three databases (PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science) to highlight the characteristics of the dogs involved in AATs. Based on the scientific literature relevant to such dogs, we considered different parameters (i.e., number, age, sex, breed, temperament, methods of choice and training, health status, research goals, and activities with dogs) to include studies in our paper. After screening 4331 papers identified on the searched databases, we selected 38 articles that met the inclusion criteria. Analysis of the included articles showed that the characteristics of the dogs were neglected. Our findings indicated a lack of information about the dogs, as well as the absence of standardized and univocal criteria for dog selection, training programs, and health protocols.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

WITOSŁAWSKA, ANNA, and KRZYSZTOF KLIMASZEWSKI. "Evaluation of the usefulness of the temperament tests in utility dog breeding on the example of a Boxer breed." Annals of Warsaw University of Life Sciences - SGGW - Animal Science 58, no. 3 (October 7, 2019): 235–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.22630/aas.2019.58.3.24.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Spada, Eva, Daniela Proverbio, Luis Miguel Viñals Flórez, Maria del Rosario Perlado Chamizo, Roberta Perego, Giada Bagnagatti De Giorgi, and Luciana Baggiani. "Prevalence of dog erythrocyte antigens 1, 4, and 7 in galgos (Spanish Greyhounds)." Journal of Veterinary Diagnostic Investigation 27, no. 4 (July 2015): 558–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1040638715592025.

Full text
Abstract:
Galgos (Spanish Greyhounds), in common with other sighthounds, have higher hematocrits, hemoglobin concentrations, and red blood cell counts than other breeds. In addition to these hematological characteristics, the physical characteristics of these dogs (medium to large dogs with an easily accessible jugular vein and a good temperament) make galgos ideal blood donors. However, to date, there are only published reports concerning dog erythrocyte antigen (DEA) 1 in this breed. Information on DEAs 4 and 7 would be useful when recruiting blood donors to donation programs, as DEA 1 and 7–negative and DEA 4–positive dogs can be considered universal donors. Ethylenediamine tetra-acetic acid–anticoagulated jugular blood samples were collected from 205 galgos. Dogs were aged between 1 and 10 years, 102 were female (49.8%) and 103 male (50.2%), and all were living in South Madrid, Spain. All 205 blood samples were tested for DEA 1 by card agglutination, and 150 of these samples were tested for DEA 4 and DEA 7 by gel column agglutination using polyclonal anti-DEA antibodies. Of the 205 galgos blood samples typed, 112 out of 205 (54.6%) were positive for DEA 1. Of the 150 blood samples tested, all (150/150, 100%) were positive for DEA 4, and 12 out of 150 (8%) samples tested positive for DEA 7. Of these samples, 70 out of 150 (46.7%) were positive only for DEA 4. There was no relationship between blood types and sex. In addition to the hematological characteristics previously reported and the physical characteristics of these dogs, the relative prevalence of blood types DEA 1, 4, and 7 make galgos good candidates for blood donation in blood donor programs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Hennessy, Michael B., Regina M. Willen, and Patricia A. Schiml. "Psychological Stress, Its Reduction, and Long-Term Consequences: What Studies with Laboratory Animals Might Teach Us about Life in the Dog Shelter." Animals 10, no. 11 (November 7, 2020): 2061. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani10112061.

Full text
Abstract:
There is a long history of laboratory studies of the physiological and behavioral effects of stress, its reduction, and the later psychological and behavioral consequences of unmitigated stress responses. Many of the stressors employed in these studies approximate the experience of dogs confined in an animal shelter. We review how the laboratory literature has guided our own work in describing the reactions of dogs to shelter housing and in helping formulate means of reducing their stress responses. Consistent with the social buffering literature in other species, human interaction has emerged as a key ingredient in moderating glucocorticoid stress responses of shelter dogs. We discuss variables that appear critical for effective use of human interaction procedures in the shelter as well as potential neural mechanisms underlying the glucocorticoid-reducing effect. We also describe recent studies in which enrichment centered on human interaction has been found to reduce aggressive responses in a temperament test used to determine suitability for adoption. Finally, we suggest that a critical aspect of the laboratory stress literature that has been underappreciated in studying shelter dogs is evidence for long-term behavioral consequences—often mediated by glucocorticoids—that may not become apparent until well after initial stress exposure.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Santos, Natalia R. dos, Alexandra Beck, and Alain Fontbonne. "The View of the French Dog Breeders in Relation to Female Reproduction, Maternal Care and Stress during the Peripartum Period." Animals 10, no. 1 (January 17, 2020): 159. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani10010159.

Full text
Abstract:
In France, as in many other western countries, dogs are an important part of the society as pets or working animals. The exact demand for puppies in France is unknown, as is the proportion of dogs coming from different breeding sources. Nevertheless, the origin of puppies is important since young dogs from puppy mills and pet stores appear to be more likely to develop behavioral disorders. Puppies coming from responsible breeders, on the other hand, tend to be better adapted. In well-managed kennels, an explanation for these behavioral differences may be associated with greater contact of litters with the dam and humans. Another factor that might influence a dog’s temperament and character is maternal behavior. Although recent studies have shown that the quality of maternal care in dogs is important, direct effects on the development of behavioral problems such as fearfulness or noise sensitivity are still controversial. To better understand the view of breeders, due to an increased interest in maternal care of dogs, an online questionnaire was developed to assess the dog breeders’ profiles and to explore their perception of normal maternal and stress-related behaviors during the peripartum period. In addition, the management of the female during this critical time was queried. Three-hundred and forty-five French dog breeders, representing 91 breeds, completed the online survey. Considering the demographics of the responders, breeding activity in France is mostly family-based with 76% raising two breeds of dogs that produce, on average, five litters/year. Around 60% of the breeders use progesterone levels to determine breeding date. The whelping date is estimated using multiple criteria and most labors and deliveries happen under human supervision. The majority of behaviors associated to good maternal care are defined by the vast majority as being related to more attention of the bitch towards the puppies with the frequency of nursing and licking being important clues. Globally, the peripartum is perceived as a stressful period and to minimize stress and reassure the bitch the favored method used is increasing human presence. Problems related to maternal behavior were described, notably with primiparous bitches.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Marshall-Pescini, Sarah, Jonas F. L. Schwarz, Inga Kostelnik, Zsófia Virányi, and Friederike Range. "Importance of a species’ socioecology: Wolves outperform dogs in a conspecific cooperation task." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, no. 44 (October 16, 2017): 11793–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1709027114.

Full text
Abstract:
A number of domestication hypotheses suggest that dogs have acquired a more tolerant temperament than wolves, promoting cooperative interactions with humans and conspecifics. This selection process has been proposed to resemble the one responsible for our own greater cooperative inclinations in comparison with our closest living relatives. However, the socioecology of wolves and dogs, with the former relying more heavily on cooperative activities, predicts that at least with conspecifics, wolves should cooperate better than dogs. Here we tested similarly raised wolves and dogs in a cooperative string-pulling task with conspecifics and found that wolves outperformed dogs, despite comparable levels of interest in the task. Whereas wolves coordinated their actions so as to simultaneously pull the rope ends, leading to success, dogs pulled the ropes in alternate moments, thereby never succeeding. Indeed in dog dyads it was also less likely that both members simultaneously engaged in other manipulative behaviors on the apparatus. Different conflict-management strategies are likely responsible for these results, with dogs’ avoidance of potential competition over the apparatus constraining their capacity to coordinate actions. Wolves, in contrast, did not hesitate to manipulate the ropes simultaneously, and once cooperation was initiated, rapidly learned to coordinate in more complex conditions as well. Social dynamics (rank and affiliation) played a key role in success rates. Results call those domestication hypotheses that suggest dogs evolved greater cooperative inclinations into question, and rather support the idea that dogs’ and wolves’ different social ecologies played a role in affecting their capacity for conspecific cooperation and communication.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Zhaldybin, V. V., P. V. Savinov, and A. Y. Demchenko. "PECULIARITIES OF TRAINING OF SERVICE DOGS BY DOG SPECIALISTS." Ecology and Animal World, no. 1 (May 30, 2022): 51–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.47612/2224-1647-2022-1-51-56.

Full text
Abstract:
The article considered a comparative characteristic of the compatibility of temperaments of cynologists and service dogs for training in a cynological center. The results of studies on the study of types of higher nervous activity in cynologists and service dogs are presented in order to determine the effectiveness of their training.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Farnam, Alireza, Bahareh Deljou, and Masumeh Zamanlu. "The Establishment of a Clinic for Personality Modulation in Iran: A Novel Approach in Personality Psychiatry with a Dimensional Approach." Depiction of Health 12, no. 3 (September 6, 2021): 209–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.34172/doh.2021.21.

Full text
Abstract:
Mental health and treatment approaches have indeed garnered attention in health care. Treatment approaches have shifted from severe and chronic disorders prevalent in the past to milder disorders. Further, these approaches aid in improving performance, health, happiness, and superiority (1). To this end, new diagnostic systems have been introduced to move away from traditional approaches (i.e., stratified diagnosis and treatment) to a dimensional approach. This shift in attitude may affect major current mental health practices requiring innovative approaches, guidelines, and care to revolutionize mental health. Thus, setting up the clinic of personality modulation was inevitable as it provides educational-therapeutic services in accordance with the most up-to-date diagnostic classification system. It can also promote the growth of mental health. Even healthy people or those who pay attention to their mental growth and development can also benefit from the clinic's services. In a dimensional approach, services are provided for those who fall in the category of difficult code of personality in the new psychiatric divisions of the ICD-11. These services will lead to their personal and professional development.To compensate for the shortcomings of previous classification systems, the ICD-11 has adopted a dimensional approach, a new approach to the classification of personality disorders, in which the focus is primarily on diagnosing and determining the severity of the disease. Moreover, the range of symptoms includes five ranges of traits related to personality disorders. These five domains encompass Negative Affectivity, Detachment, Dissociality, Disinhibition, and Anankastia (2-3). Thierry, Crawford, Mulder, Bleschfeld, Farnam, et al. (4), establishing this dimensional classification, proposed a new diagnosis called personality difficulty. Diagnosis of the personality difficulty is not a psychological disorder in itself. However, it can still be used for the clinical benefit of virtually normal individuals and has been included in the Z-scores of the ICD-11 for Non-Patients. This diagnosis consists of people who are neither typically ill nor have a personality disorder. Rather, these people have problems with life management that causes medical problems and incurs costs for the community and treatment systems (5). The researchers concluded that the proposed changes could aid clinical efficacy in diagnosing the personality disorder and selecting appropriate treatment. In this way, the misconceptions concerning the false label attached to the disorder term can be dispelled (4).Therefore, current psychiatry is moving in a direction with a program not only for patients but also for almost normal people who have problems in their lives or feel that there are some difficulties in developing their personality. One style of psychiatric treatment called cosmetic psychiatry (6) or positive psychiatry (7) has attracted attention among the whole range of psychiatric disciplines. Cosmetic Psychiatry refers to the empowerment and strengthening of people's cognitive, behavioral, and emotional processes who do not suffer from a specific disorder. In other words, the treatment is to improve a person's mental state in the absence of any clinical disorder (8). The development of the ICD-11 has led to innovative approaches which were not available before. The reason for the clinic's foundation lies in the philosophical theories presented by Georg Wilhelm Friedrich Hegel and Sadr al-Muta'allehin Shirazi. They saw soul and psyche as a constantly evolving category. According to their theories, the soul is inherently growing and ascending unless a factor disrupts this process (9). The theoretical foundation of the approaches used in the personality modulation clinic rests on Cloninger's personality theory. This theory is known as the model of temperament and character. Cloninger divides personality into three components: temperament, character, and self-aware psyche. Temperament concerns the genetic and inherited aspect of individuals and has certain biological elements. Character deals with the semantic component of human personality and considers the individual's relationship with himself, others, and the universe. The third component is the self-aware psyche which its definition hinges on the individual's sense of intuition towards his inner abilities (10). Each element has its own characteristics and brain structures. The elements of this model and the treatment strategies presented in Cloninger's theory are employed to regulate the clinical approach in the personality modulation clinic. In this regard, pharmacotherapy and new neuroscience therapies (e.g., Biofeedback, Neurofeedback, and rTMS) are implemented to moderate temperament status and its extreme features, which can worsen the condition by affecting the patient's somatic state and ultimately inhibit Rhythmic growth of self-awareness. Understanding the neurotransmitters involved in the four temperamental traits (i.e., harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward dependence, and persistence) and the role of neuroscience trends in our context are vital. After identifying and determining the fundamental conflicts related to character traits, pertinent psychodynamic analysis methods and eclectic psychotherapy are used for their treatment. Character aims to identify and remove barriers to the growth of all three components of self-direction, cooperation, and self- Transcendence involved in the relevant conflicts. In this particular treatment, different interventions are regarded for each person commensurate with his/her inner problems and conflicts. To develop the personality of these people, participatory intervention methods are opted. This model combines the training and supervision of a specialized psychiatric team to identify and treat clients' problems. The ultimate aim of this treatment method is to expand self-awareness on the physiological and psychological processes of the mind so that the person becomes more self-aware. Hitherto, no educational-therapeutic program with the objectives of the personality modulation clinic has been introduced and implemented neither in our country nor in the world. Hence, there is a call for a codified and coherent program with regard to the educational-therapeutic course of personality modulation due to lack of evidence. Considering the importance of this field of psychiatry, as described earlier, setting up such a clinic becomes indispensable. Also, finding such a place to get medical care is difficult for people with personality difficulty who feel that their existing personality practices do not match their capacity for growth and development. These people need to receive services with a biopsychosocial approach, although they might receive some degrees of non-specific services. This sort of service is offered to them in psychological clinics. On the other hand, counselors' opinions were based on scattered styles derived from schools of psychoanalysis- they often considered the psychological development of character and lacked a biological component (i.e., biological therapies such as pharmacotherapy and advanced neuroscience techniques used as accelerators). In sum, the clinic was established to adapt mental health services to up-to-date approaches in the country and the region to prevent the progression of personality difficulty to severe psychiatric disorders, increasing self-satisfaction in society, facilitating personal growth, and lowering high medical costs. This clinic was established by the suggestion of the first author in 2016 to combine the existing knowledge in psychotherapy, personology, and psychiatric pharmacotherapy leading to a new solution for people in need of receiving the diagnosis of personality difficulty.In addition, there is a golden opportunity for training skilled psychiatrists and residents in this field to provide services for clients in the future.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Littlejohn, Brittni P., David G. Riley, Don A. Neuendorff, Charles R. Long, Thomas H. Welsh, and Ronald D. Randel. "304 Transgenerational influence of prenatal stress on temperament, birth weight, and weaning weight of Brahman calves." Journal of Animal Science 97, Supplement_3 (December 2019): 3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jas/skz258.003.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The objective was to evaluate the transgenerational influence of prenatal transportation stress (PNS) on temperament, birth weight, and weaning weight in Brahman calves. Dams were derived from multiparous cows that were transported for 2 h at 60, 80, 100, 120, and 140 d of gestation (n = 48) or maintained as Controls (n = 48). Heifers born to Transported or Control cows were followed through maturity and their calves (n = 79; 35 calves born to Control dams and 44 to PNS dams) studied across multiple years. Calf birth and weaning weights were recorded. Calf temperament at weaning was evaluated utilizing pen score (PS; 1=calm and 5=excitable), exit velocity (EV; m/sec), and temperament score (TS; (PS+EV)/2). Data were analyzed using mixed linear models. Fixed effects included dam treatment, calf sex, and birth season nested within birth year (season nested within year was identified as an efficient parameterization). Calf sire was a random effect. Birth weight was not affected by dam treatment, calf sex, or season nested within year (P &gt; 0.1). Weaning weight was greater in males than females (P &lt; 0.01). Weaning weight was affected by season nested within year (P &lt; 0.01). DOA at weaning was a covariate for weaning weight (0.74±0.09 kg; P &lt; 0.01), but not PS, EV, or TS (P &lt; 0.1). Weaning weight was not affected by dam treatment (P &gt; 0.1). TS was affected by season nested within year (P = 0.04) but not dam treatment or calf sex (P &gt; 0.1). EV was not affected by dam treatment, calf sex, or season nested within year (P &gt; 0.1). PS was affected by season nested within year (P &lt; 0.01) but not calf sex (P &gt; 0.1). Calves born to PNS dams had greater (P = 0.02) PS than calves born to Controls (3.17±0.20 and 2.58±0.22, respectively). There was a transgenerational influence of PNS on temperament but not birth or weaning weight.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Skandakumar, S., G. Stodulski, and J. Hau. "Salivary IgA: A Possible Stress Marker in Dogs." Animal Welfare 4, no. 4 (November 1995): 339–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0962728600018030.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractStress in humans has been reported to be associated with a decrease in the salivary immunoglobulin A (s-IgA) levels enabling the possible use of s-IgA to assess stress. Prolonged stress, if reliably assessed in a non-invasive manner, may be used to assess animal welfare. This study analysed groups of dogs undergoing physical and temperamental training and sIgA levels were measured by rocket immunoelectrophoresis in prospective samples. Behavioural assessment was carried out and Cortisol levels in saliva were measured by ELISA. A significant negative correlation (P <0.007) between the logarithmic Cortisol concentrations and s-IgA levels in saliva was recorded. The behavioural assessment of the dogs agreed well with the biochemical markers. It is concluded that IgA levels in saliva may be a useful marker of dog well-being and that stress results in decreased s-IgA levels.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

da Silva, Lúcia Daniel Machado. "Evaluation of ACP Derma Barrier Cream in the Healing Process of Induced Wounds in Dogs." Open Access Journal of Veterinary Science & Research 7, no. 2 (2022): 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.23880/oajvsr-16000229.

Full text
Abstract:
The treatment of skin lesions comes up against the difficulty of the treatment due to the animal's temperament, causing stress and reluctance to administer medication, which may lead the tutor to interrupt the treatment. Knowing that coconut water has therapeutic properties that benefit tissue regeneration, we aimed to evaluate the healing activity of ACP Derma barrier cream, a coconut water-based product, to measure the degree of repair of the wound and to compare the evolution of the healing process, through macroscopic evaluations of the wounds. Five dogs were used in which 3 circular incisions were made on their backs. Wounds were treated with the 3 ointments (ACP, CMR® and Vetaglós®) and were macroscopically evaluated in the periods of 4, 7, 10 and 14 days of treatment. There were significant reductions in the wound area up to the 14th day, resulting in a satisfactory healing of the ointments used. ACP Derma was shown to be superior in terms of adherence to the lesion and showed effective potential for wound healing in dogs, equivalent to treatment with commercial ointments, making it a more cost-effective therapeutic option.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Pauli-Pott, U., B. Mertesacker, and D. Beckmann. "Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung des «frühkindlichen Temperaments» im Elternurteil *Teile der präsentierten Daten wurden im Rahmen einer durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) geförderten Studie gewonnen (Kennziffern: Pa 543/2-1, 2-2, 2-3, 2-4)." Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie 31, no. 2 (May 2003): 99–110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1422-4917.31.2.99.

Full text
Abstract:
Zusammenfassung: Fragestellung: Eine deutschsprachige Adaptation des Infant Behavior Questionnaire von Rothbart wird hinsichtlich der internen Konsistenzen der Skalen, der Skaleninterkorrelationen und Stabilität untersucht. Mögliche Zusammenhänge der Skalen mit der Depressivität und Ängstlichkeit der beurteilenden Mutter werden geprüft. Methode: Untersucht werden unabhängige Stichproben 6-8- und 10-12-monatiger Säuglinge (n = 149 resp. n = 109) und deren Bezugspersonen. Eine Stichprobe von 101 Mutter-Säugling-Paaren wird im Längsschnitt, im Alter der Säuglinge von vier, acht und 12 Monaten untersucht. Ergebnis: Die fünf Skalen des Fragebogens sind in den beiden Altersgruppen zufriedenstellend intern konsistent und unabhängig voneinander. Die Stabilitätskoeffizienten entsprechen denen der amerikanischen Version des Verfahrens. Schlussfolgerung: Damit liegt ein in der Forschung und Praxis verwendbares deutschsprachiges Verfahren zur Messung frühkindlicher Temperamentsmerkmale im Elternurteil vor.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Burch, Mary R. "Assessment of Canine Temperament: Predictive or Prescriptive?" International Journal of Comparative Psychology 33 (2020). http://dx.doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2020.33.01.01.

Full text
Abstract:
Canine temperament testing has historically been linked to the predictability of future behavior. A predictive model of canine temperament testing assumes that a dog’s behavior in one situation will likely be similar to its behavior in a variety of other situations. An alternative model is proposed for a canine temperament test that could identify areas in which a dog might fail to perform certain test items, but by using modern behavior analysis techniques, behaviors could be modified through a prescriptive approach. This article describes the AKC Temperament Test (ATT), which is the first prescriptive canine temperament test. The ATT is designed to provide pet dog owners with information about potential problem areas that can be modified through training.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Shan, Shuwen, Fangzheng Xu, and Bertram Brenig. "Genome-Wide Association Studies Reveal Neurological Genes for Dog Herding, Predation, Temperament, and Trainability Traits." Frontiers in Veterinary Science 8 (July 21, 2021). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2021.693290.

Full text
Abstract:
Genome-wide association study (GWAS) using dog breed standard values as phenotypic measurements is an efficient way to identify genes associated with morphological and behavioral traits. As a result of strong human purposeful selections, several specialized behavioral traits such as herding and hunting have been formed in different modern dog breeds. However, genetic analyses on this topic are rather limited due to the accurate phenotyping difficulty for these complex behavioral traits. Here, 268 dog whole-genome sequences from 130 modern breeds were used to investigate candidate genes underlying dog herding, predation, temperament, and trainability by GWAS. Behavioral phenotypes were obtained from the American Kennel Club based on dog breed standard descriptions or groups (conventional categorization of dog historical roles). The GWAS results of herding behavior (without body size as a covariate) revealed 44 significantly associated sites within five chromosomes. Significantly associated sites on CFA7, 9, 10, and 20 were located either in or near neuropathological or neuronal genes including THOC1, ASIC2, MSRB3, LLPH, RFX8, and CHL1. MSRB3 and CHL1 genes were reported to be associated with dog fear. Since herding is a restricted hunting behavior by removing killing instinct, 36 hounds and 55 herding dogs were used to analyze predation behavior. Three neuronal-related genes (JAK2, MEIS1, and LRRTM4) were revealed as candidates for predation behavior. The significantly associated variant of temperament GWAS was located within ACSS3 gene. The highest associated variant in trainability GWAS is located on CFA22, with no variants detected above the Bonferroni threshold. Since dog behaviors are correlated with body size, we next incorporate body mass as covariates into GWAS; and significant signals around THOC1, MSRB3, LLPH, RFX8, CHL1, LRRTM4, and ACSS3 genes were still detected for dog herding, predation, and temperament behaviors. In humans, these candidate genes are either involved in nervous system development or associated with mental disorders. In conclusion, our results imply that these neuronal or psychiatric genes might be involved in biological processes underlying dog herding, predation, and temperament behavioral traits.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Byrne, Ceara, Thad Starner, and Melody Jackson. "Quantifying canine interactions with smart toys assesses suitability for service dog work." Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9 (September 2, 2022). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.886941.

Full text
Abstract:
There are approximately a half million active service dogs in the United States, providing life-changing assistance and independence to people with a wide range of disabilities. The tremendous value of service dogs creates significant demand, which service dog providers struggle to meet. Breeding, raising, and training service dogs is an expensive, time-consuming endeavor which is exacerbated by expending resources on dogs who ultimately will prove to be unsuitable for service dog work because of temperament issues. Quantifying behavior and temperament through sensor-instrumented dog toys can provide a way to predict which dogs will be suitable for service dog work, allowing resources to be focused on the dogs likely to succeed. In a 2-year study, we tested dogs in advanced training at Canine Companions for Independence with instrumented toys, and we discovered that a measure of average bite duration is significantly correlated with a dog's placement success as a service dog [Adjusted OR = 0.12, Pr(&gt;|z|) = 0.00666]. Applying instrumented toy interactions to current behavioral assessments could yield more accurate measures for predicting successful placement of service dogs while reducing the workload of the trainers.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Savalli, Carine, Natalia Albuquerque, Angélica S. Vasconcellos, Daniela Ramos, Fernanda T. de Mello, and Daniel S. Mills. "Assessment of emotional predisposition in dogs using PANAS (Positive and Negative Activation Scale) and associated relationships in a sample of dogs from Brazil." Scientific Reports 9, no. 1 (December 2019). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-54645-6.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe English version of the Positive and Negative Activation Scale (PANAS) is a useful tool for the assessment of dog temperament, helping to identify highly sensitive individuals that could be at risk of developing fears, phobias and anxiety problems, and potentially depressive states. This study evaluated the association between dogs’ and owners’ characteristics and dog temperament in Brazil. To accomplish this, we adapted and validated a Portuguese language version of PANAS for dogs. Data from 1744 owner-dog dyads were analysed and a two-factor structure similar to the original PANAS was revealed that met the requirements for validity and internal consistency. We found that dogs owned by women, neutered dogs and those who live in single-dog households show higher negative activation. Moreover, the older the owner, the less the negative activation for dogs that are bought. We also found that the older the dog, the less the positive activation, but this score is higher in dogs that sleep inside the house. Interestingly, mixed-breed dogs scored higher for both negative and positive emotional activation compared to purebreds. These findings alongside the particular profile of dogs in Brazil, including its large population of mixed-breed, emphasise the value of cross-cultural investigations in order to develop a full understanding of dog behaviour.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Weseley-Jones, Kate. "For Pits’ Sake: Effect of Breed Labels on Perceptions of Shelter Dog Attractiveness." Society & Animals, September 1, 2022, 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685306-bja10033.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Pit bulls are the most common type of dog found in shelters, and negative perceptions of their temperament are believed to reduce their likelihood of adoption. Two experiments investigated the effects of breed label on perceptions of shelter dog attractiveness. The first experiment recruited 176 participants to rate the attractiveness of a dog labeled either a Pit Bull or Mixed Breed. The second study added an unlabeled condition and a Staffordshire Bull Terrier condition, and recruited 257 participants. While the Pit Bull was rated significantly less attractive than the Mixed Breed in Experiment 1, in Experiment 2, the Mixed Breed was rated the least attractive and significantly less so than the dog in the unlabeled condition. In addition, both experiments demonstrated that younger people viewed the dog as more attractive than older people. Given the conflicting results regarding breed labels, further research is necessary.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Tonoike, Akiko, Ken-ichi Otaki, Go Terauchi, Misato Ogawa, Maki Katayama, Hikari Sakata, Fumina Miyasako, Kazutaka Mogi, Takefumi Kikusui, and Miho Nagasawa. "Identification of genes associated with human-canine communication in canine evolution." Scientific Reports 12, no. 1 (June 9, 2022). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11130-x.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe dog (Canis familiaris) was the first domesticated animal and hundreds of breeds exist today. During domestication, dogs experienced strong selection for temperament, behaviour, and cognitive ability. However, the genetic basis of these abilities is not well-understood. We focused on ancient dog breeds to investigate breed-related differences in social cognitive abilities. In a problem-solving task, ancient breeds showed a lower tendency to look back at humans than other European breeds. In a two-way object choice task, they showed no differences in correct response rate or ability to read human communicative gestures. We examined gene polymorphisms in oxytocin, oxytocin receptor, melanocortin 2 receptor, and a Williams–Beuren syndrome-related gene (WBSCR17), as candidate genes of dog domestication. The single-nucleotide polymorphisms on melanocortin 2 receptor were related to both tasks, while other polymorphisms were associated with the unsolvable task. This indicates that glucocorticoid functions are involved in the cognitive skills acquired during dog domestication.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Zapata, Isain, Alexander W. Eyre, Carlos E. Alvarez, and James A. Serpell. "Latent class analysis of behavior across dog breeds reveal underlying temperament profiles." Scientific Reports 12, no. 1 (September 17, 2022). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20053-6.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractLatent class analysis (LCA) is a type of modeling analysis approach that has been used to identify unobserved groups or subgroups within multivariate categorical data. LCA has been used for a wide array of psychological evaluations in humans, including the identification of depression subtypes or PTSD comorbidity patterns. However, it has never been used for the assessment of animal behavior. Our objective here is to identify behavioral profile-types of dogs using LCA. The LCA was performed on a C-BARQ behavioral questionnaire dataset from 57,454 participants representing over 350 pure breeds and mixed breed dogs. Two, three, and four class LCA models were developed using C-BARQ trait scores and environmental covariates. In our study, LCA is shown as an effective and flexible tool to classify behavioral assessments. By evaluating the traits that carry the strongest relevance, it was possible to define the basis of these grouping differences. Groupings can be ranked and used as levels for simplified comparisons of complex constructs, such as temperament, that could be further exploited in downstream applications such as genomic association analyses. We propose this approach will facilitate dissection of physiological and environmental factors associated with psychopathology in dogs, humans, and mammals in general.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Miller, Sharmaine L., James A. Serpell, Kathryn R. Dalton, Kaitlin B. Waite, Daniel O. Morris, Laurel E. Redding, Nancy A. Dreschel, and Meghan F. Davis. "The Importance of Evaluating Positive Welfare Characteristics and Temperament in Working Therapy Dogs." Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9 (April 4, 2022). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.844252.

Full text
Abstract:
To date, investigations of the welfare of therapy dogs have focused largely on examining physiological and behavioral measures that could indicate if the animal is experiencing stress or distress. However, this approach does not fully address the definition of welfare which is often described as existing on a continuum from negative (or stressful) to positive. With therapy dogs, it would be worth addressing if they experience positive emotional affect while working since the quality and efficacy of animal-assisted interventions for the human recipient is likely to be influenced by the animal's emotional state during the interaction. The purpose of this review is to articulate how objective measurements of the HPA axis and measurements of behavioral observations and standardized questions can be used to evaluate positive welfare in therapy dogs. A potentially relevant indicator of positive welfare is the peripheral concentration of the neurohormone oxytocin, which has been found to increase in systemic circulation within a variety of species during positive social and affiliative contexts, including during human-dog interaction. Oxytocin is also a negative-feedback regulator of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis, which culminates with the production of the stress hormone cortisol. Cortisol is widely used as a physiological indicator to assess negative welfare states in animals, including therapy dogs. Observable behavior during interactions with humans that may convey enjoyment could provide indicators of positive welfare in dogs such as engagement in play, or human-directed affiliative behaviors including leaning against, nudging, or licking the patient. However, in assessing positive welfare, it is also critical to consider that all animal behavioral displays and physiological responses are dependent on the dog's individual (and breed) temperament. Temperament directly drives how the animal copes and responds to its current physical and social environment, including during stressful situations such as when therapy dogs interact with unfamiliar humans in novel healthcare settings. Coupled with both positive and negative physiological and behavioral welfare indicators, questionnaire data can provide further context to, and enhance interpretations of, therapy dog welfare assessment results. Overall, to date, no studies have measured all of these factors to assess therapy dog welfare.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Plueckhahn, Tania C., Luke A. Schneider, and Paul H. Delfabbro. "Comparing Owner-Rated Dog Temperament Measures and a Measure of Owner Personality: An Exploratory Study." Anthrozoös, April 28, 2022, 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2022.2062870.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

van Rooy, Diane, and Claire M. Wade. "Association between coat colour and the behaviour of Australian Labrador retrievers." Canine Genetics and Epidemiology 6, no. 1 (November 30, 2019). http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40575-019-0078-z.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Background Making assumptions regarding temperament and intelligence based on the physical appearance of dogs can be a conscious or unconscious human act. Labrador retrievers with chocolate-coloured coats are anecdotally considered to be less trainable and more hyperactive and aggressive than their black or yellow peers. To test these assertions, we analysed the owner-reported behavioural traits of Labradors in relation to both their observable coat colour, and their TYRP1 and MC1R genotypes. Results We used the results of an owner-based questionnaire to determine scores for 21 behavioural traits and test whether these scores varied with coat colour (n = 225). Familiar dog aggression was the only trait that was found to vary significantly with coat colour (P = 0.013). Yellow Labradors had a higher score than chocolate Labradors, even when corrected for multiple testing (P = 0.021). We repeated the analyses for a subset of 63 Labradors with available genotyping data for the genes (MC1R and TYRP1) that are known to determine the primary coat colours in Labradors. Familiar dog aggression scores varied with both the observed coat colour and MC1R genotype. Dogs homozygous for MC1R recessive allele (with yellow coat colour) scored higher for familiar dog aggression than either black or chocolate Labradors. However, no association maintained significance when incorporating Bonferroni correction. Dog trainability scores decreased additively as the number of recessive brown alleles for TYRP1 increased. This allelic association was independent of the observable coat colour. Dogs homozygous for the brown allele were considered less trainable than dogs with no brown alleles (P = 0.030). Conclusions Our results do not support that chocolate-coloured Labradors are more hyperactive or aggressive than either black or yellow Labradors. Trainability scores varied with TYRP1 genotype but not the observable coat colour. Further validation is required.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Bray, Emily E., Kerinne M. Levy, Brenda S. Kennedy, Deborah L. Duffy, James A. Serpell, and Evan L. MacLean. "Predictive Models of Assistance Dog Training Outcomes Using the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire and a Standardized Temperament Evaluation." Frontiers in Veterinary Science 6 (February 27, 2019). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2019.00049.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Jia, Lei, Xiaojing Yang, and Yuwei Jiang. "The Pet Exposure Effect: Exploring the Differential Impact of Dogs Versus Cats on Consumer Mindsets." Journal of Marketing, March 18, 2022, 002224292210780. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222429221078036.

Full text
Abstract:
Despite the ubiquity of pets in consumers’ lives, scant research has examined how exposure to them (e.g., recalling past interactions with dogs and cats, viewing ads featuring a dog or a cat) influences consumer behavior. The authors demonstrate that exposure to dogs (cats) reminds consumers of the stereotypical temperaments and behaviors of the pet species, which activates a promotion- (prevention-) focused motivational mindset among consumers. Using secondary data, Study 1 shows that people in states with a higher percentage of dog (cat) owners Google more promotion- (prevention-) focused words and report a higher COVID-19 transmission rate. Using multiple products, Studies 2 and 3 demonstrate that these regulatory mindsets, when activated by pet exposure, carry over to influence downstream consumer judgments, purchase intentions, and behaviors, even in pet-unrelated consumption contexts. Study 4 shows that pet stereotypicality moderates the proposed effect such that the relationship between pet exposure and regulatory orientations persists to the extent consumers are reminded of the stereotypical temperaments and behaviors of the pet species. Studies 5–7 examine the role of regulatory fit and evince that exposure to dogs (cats) leads to more favorable responses toward advertising messages featuring promotion- (prevention-) focused appeals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Filho, Mário dos Santos, Bruna Pereira Gonçalves, Jaíne da Silva, Eduarda de Oliveira Silva Lima Machado, Nathália Marques De Oliveira Lemos, Bruno Ricardo Soares Alberigi, Alexandre Jose Rodrigues Bendas, and Paulo de Tarso Landgraf Botteon. "Unilateral Atrial Ectopic Rhythm in a Golden Retriever." Acta Scientiae Veterinariae 50 (February 21, 2022). http://dx.doi.org/10.22456/1679-9216.118570.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: Atrial ectopic rhythm is a type of supraventricular arrhythmia, originating in two distinct points in the atrial region. In the electrocardiographic (ECG) tracing, it is represented by independent depolarizations of sinus P waves and ectopic P waves. The occurrence of this disorder is rare, and the diagnosis criteria are the presence of the described waves within the basal rhythm. In humans, there have been reports related to severe heart failure with an unfavorable prognosis. The present report aimed to describe the clinical case of a dog with unilateral atrial ectopic rhythm without any underlying cardiac disorder.Case: A 8-year-old male golden retriever was brought to a veterinary clinic for a preoperative evaluation for lipoma removal in the right forelimb. On clinical examination, the owner stated that the patient was active, with no signs of easy fatigue or cough. The canine displayed normophagy, normodipsia, normoquezia, and normouria. On physical examination, he demonstrated a lymphatic temperament with tachypnea. The temperature and capillary filling time were within the normal range, with a normokinetic pulse. Cardiac auscultation revealed a mild grade I/VI murmur in mitral focus and an 80-bpm heart rate. Respiratory auscultation revealed the presence of harshy lung sounds. The cough reflex was positive; the Piparote test, negative. The blood test showed no noticeable changes in blood count and serum biochemistry. Systemic systolic blood pressure was 120 mmHg. On radiographic examination, no evidence of heart or lung abnormalities were identified. After the clinical evaluation, an ECG examination was performed; a unilateral atrial ectopic rhythm was observed with different frequencies between atrial and ventricular rhythm and with P (164°) and P waves (80°). On echocardiographic examination, no morphological abnormalities were seen, though trivial mitral insufficiency was identified in a color Doppler study. Blood was collected to measure electrolytes to check for possible electrolytic abnormalities; the results were within the normal range. The patient was released for and remained stable throughout the procedure, maintaining the rhythm detected during the transoperatory time. Upon reassessment in a new ECG examination, the arrhythmia persisted, suggesting that a primary lesion in the atrial tissue was present.Discussion: An atrial ectopic rhythm diagnosis requires a detailed study with the aim of ruling out heart diseases that may affect the propagation of the cardiac stimulus. However, no morphological or functional abnormalities of note that justified triggering stimuli for the ectopic rhythm were observed. In addition, based on the echocardiographic evaluation, myocardial function was preserved, supporting the canine’s release for the procedure. The suspicion of hydroelectrolytic alteration and hypoxia was present after discarding structural causes, though it was discarded due to normal laboratory results. The presence of ectopic P waves was due to the non-interference in the sinus P’ waves; consequently, they were found in the sinus heart rhythm. Upon assessing the ectopic P wave frequency, the atrial rhythm frequency was higher than the heart rate, juxtaposing the different irregular intervals within the atrial cycle. The rhythm alone may explain that its severity can be linked to the physical findings. This indicated that they did not influence the presentation of ectopia since the cardiac output, controlled by the sinus rhythm part, was responsible for maintaining the rhythm and the demand of the organism; the electrical conduction system, responsible for atrial systole, represents 15-25% of the blood ejection for the ventricular filling. Patients with rhythm disorders as described need guidance and follow-up for the early detection of clinical signs resulting from the destabilization of the condition.Keywords: atrial dissociation, supraventricular arrhythmia, electrocardiogram, dogs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Morag, Talia. "Persons and Their Private Personas: Living with Yourself." M/C Journal 17, no. 3 (June 10, 2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.5204/mcj.829.

Full text
Abstract:
Public life is usually understood to be whatever we do or say in our formal and professional relationships. At the workplace, at the doctor’s office or at the café, we need to make a good impression and we cannot say everything we think or do anything we want. We need to appear a certain way to be liked, get ahead, or simply stay out of trouble. The distinction between private and public presupposes that we invest efforts in maintaining a public “persona” whereas at home we can “be ourselves.” A closer examination, however, reveals that we also have a persona within the circle of our immediate family and close friends. We often censor ourselves with the people closest to us, in order to be seen by our loved-ones as caring partners, devoted children or loyal friends. Can we ever relax and really be ourselves without maintaining a persona? Even in our most private moments, at home alone, we are under the pressure of a private persona, a self-image we want to maintain even when nobody else is looking. We also want to impress ourselves, so to speak, to feel and think about and interact with people in a manner that reflects the way we think we are, or should be, in our social world. On occasion, we explicitly endorse certain values or character traits that we see as guiding our social interactions as well as our private thoughts and emotions. Naturally, most of us probably think that who we are, as reflected from our actions and reactions, thoughts and emotions, matches quite well our private persona. But when we consider those around us, especially those we know well enough to know what they think about themselves, we often notice patterns of behaviour that are in tension with their private persona. We could say that they have a false, or at least a partially blind, self-image. The philosopher and psychoanalyst Jonathan Lear provides a good example to think about these tensions in one’s private persona. In A Case for Irony, he describes a woman who displays prototypical femininity in some of her mannerisms, gestures and her style of clothing, and self-consciously comports herself according to traditional gender roles. Yet, Lear notices that she also exhibits “boyish” behaviour without realising it, a behaviour that further demonstrates that she cares about being “boyish” (42-71). Consider other examples such as: a proud anti-authoritarian whose gestures and reactions reveal that he evaluates people according to their social hierarchy; or a person who fiercely defends her independence in close relationships and yet remains financially dependent; or the delicate flower that cannot hurt a fly with a killer instinct. Around us, many people hold onto a self-image that captures only a part of their social ways of being and ignores other parts that are apparently in tension with it. We ascribe to one another the ability to turn a blind eye to what is there to see. In such cases, it appears that some patterns of social interaction fit well with what we may call one’s endorsed private persona whereas other identifiable patterns do not. Those, in turn, seem to fit a hidden private persona, certain values or character traits that the person does not acknowledge having. In other words, one’s private persona may include an endorsed aspect that is in tension with its hidden aspect. In this paper, I critically examine Jonathan Lear’s suggestion as to how to understand and deal with such tensions. In particular, I examine how one’s private persona may get caught up in such tensions in the first place. Endorsed Private Persona Our roles in our relationships and group-belongings comprise what philosophers call our practical identities, such as being a spouse, a parent, a friend, a child, a teacher or a member of the neighbourhood cat-rescue organisation. These practical identities do not just impose on us duties and obligations and appropriate ways of interaction dictated by our social niche. They are also, as the philosopher Christine Korsgaard says in The Sources of Normativity, “description[s] under which [we] value [ourselves], description[s] under which [we] find [our lives] to be worth living and [our] actions worth undertaking” (101). Our social roles present us sometimes with dilemmas, challenges and moments of choice. When we face an important decision in our lives, such as leaving a boyfriend or getting married, telling or not telling a friend that her husband is cheating on her, we may explicitly ask ourselves: do I want to be the kind of person that pursues this course of action? Am I this kind of partner, this kind of friend? It is not just about what others would think of me, it is about what I will think of me: will I be able to live with myself if I make this choice? These are typical moments in which we encounter our endorsed private persona, and reflect upon certain values that we attempt to cultivate through our choices. We also describe ourselves and identify with certain social styles, character traits, or virtues that guide some of our social gestures and actions. We care about being confident or modest, polite or direct communicators, courageous or risk averse, feminine or masculine, light travellers or collectors of objects. These labels do not just comprise the way people see us or how we want them to see us. They describe how we see and want to see ourselves and thus form a part of our endorsed private persona. We often self-consciously attempt to sustain and cultivate behaviours that would fit our endorsed personal style and qualify us as cool or elegant or nerdy, daring or cautious. We also encounter our endorsed private persona when we assess our spontaneous behaviours, in particular our emotional reactions. Those are moments when we face criticism or self-criticism about our emotions. Emotions may be criticised on the ground that they do not fit the circumstances (e.g. fear of a tiny spider), or that they are exaggerated in intensity (e.g. rage about a minor offence), or that they are ungraceful and reflect badly on us, or that they show we are immoral (e.g. envy of a friend or anger at a child), or excessively touchy (e.g. taking offence by a joke), and so forth. Our practices of criticism demonstrate that although we often forgive or accept emotions as episodes we cannot help but undergo, they nevertheless show something about us, about what kind of a person we are. We take such criticisms to heart when they reveal, on reflection, that our emotion is incompatible with our private persona, with our being someone rational, or moral, or with a hippy’s temperament. At times, we embrace the criticism, make it our own, and use it to control our emotion, with varied degrees of success. Our endorsed private persona includes values, virtues, character traits, and styles of social interactions that cut across practical identities. They are ways of inhabiting our various roles and relationships. Many of our spontaneous behaviours, our gestures and emotional reactions, fit with the way we want to be in the social world. Other people may characterise us similarly to how we characterise ourselves and use the same labels to do so, such as “courageous” or “cautious.” It is the fact that we self-consciously care about fitting those labels that makes them a part of our endorsed private persona. A Hidden Private Persona? Our spontaneous reactions, our passing thoughts and emotions, and our non-reflective actions or gestures, often fit well with the way we see and want to see ourselves. Those are the spontaneous behaviours we notice, endorse, or just accept as forgivable or understandable. They reflect who we think we are and what we self-consciously care about. And yet, many such spontaneous behaviours do not fit so well with the endorsed aspect of our private persona. We do not normally pay much attention to those behaviours. We are quite skilled in ignoring them so they typically do not manage to shake our self-image. They are more like background “noise” for our general self-aware comportment in our social interactions. Even in the privacy of our own mind, of the spontaneous emotions and thoughts that strike us without anyone else knowing, we tend to comply with our own endorsed private persona and ignore those passing thoughts that are incompatible with it. And when such behaviours cannot be easily ignored, such as certain emotional reactions, we may be able to control them, to some extent, in reference to our endorsed cares and concerns. But are the spontaneous emotions and gestures that we ignore or reject nothing more than background noise? Do they follow no other positive rhyme or reason? Do they affirm nothing about us in their own right? Lear says that often, the background noise is not just an aggregate of unacknowledged or un-reflected upon emotions and gestures (46). Reflecting on his experience as a psychoanalyst, he claims that this ignored portion of our social lives is often well unified under another social label or “pretence” that the person does not acknowledge or explicitly identify with, even if certain aspects of that person’s behaviour suggest that she actually cares about fitting that hidden “label” (46-51). Although Lear calls these hidden labels “practical identities,” the example of “boyish” and “feminine” demonstrates that he is talking about personal styles, character traits or virtues, such as “self-sacrificing” or “selfish” or “needy.” When we shut off and ignore what we see as mere background noise, Lear says, we effectively shut off a vibrant and unified part of ourselves (64). Lear notices that subjects who are not aware that a certain label unifies aspects of their patterns of social interaction, self-consciously describe themselves with another counter-label, which is the exact inverse of the hidden label (46-47). Lear’s patient is consciously feminine and also exhibits “boyish” behaviour without realising it. Consider also the married man who also exhibits single-life behaviour, or the self-sacrificing family member who actually also cares about what she sees as her selfish needs. These inversions or tensions occur within larger categories: married life; womanhood; self-concern; adulthood, and so forth. The people in these examples inhabit those social categories in ways they find contradictory. How can I be both married and lead a single’s lifestyle? How can I be both a feminine woman and a boyish woman at the same time? As Lear sees things, once his patient acknowledges that she lives in such a tension, she should reassess her ways of organising her behaviours under such rubrics (59-60). The feminine-boyish woman should examine the behaviours that she classifies under the two conflicting labels and ask: “What does any of this has to do with being a woman?” (59). In other words, Lear expects or hopes his patients ask what are, in effect, philosophical questions: what does it mean to be a woman—for me? How do I fit in this category, “woman?” Such negotiation can help people reach some kind of integration whose general purpose is self-acceptance. Perhaps some apparently conflicting identities may serve to qualify one another into reconciliation. Some may be gradually let go. New identities may arise, through reflection, and regroup, so to speak, the behaviours that until then were grouped separately and in opposition. Alternatively, one may accept both sides of the contrariety as parts of oneself that can be given their own time and place for expression. Persona in a World Full of Clichés In her comments on Lear, Korsgaard remarks that the categories of womanhood that trouble Lear’s patient are “most banal” (“Irony” 81). Indeed, Lear’s example—as well as the examples I suggested so far for such tensions—manifests social clichés. His patients exhibit behaviours describable by two poles of society’s stereotypes, prejudices and unqualified moralisms, as if a woman must be either “feminine” or “boyish,” as if love relationships are either “for life” or “dalliances,” as if one is either “loyal” or a “free-spirit,” or either good and “self-sacrificing,” or bad and “selfish” etc. Society offers us many clichés to label ourselves with and some of them may infect our private persona. How did Lear’s patient get caught in opposing clichés? Lear seems to claim that all such people need is some philosophical therapy that would liberate them from this one glitch of their private persona into a superficial dichotomy, inherited from the social world. Are things that simple? Are all the rejected spontaneous behaviours that do not sit well with our endorsed private persona unified by just one social label that comprises what Lear calls our “core fantasy” (e.g. 46; 57)? Our spontaneous behaviours, whether or not we acknowledge or endorse them, give rise to quite a few identifiable patterns, which in turn organise our emotional life. The tensions Lear speaks about are only one such identifiable structuration. That people’s spontaneous emotions and gestures follow various patterns is familiar from ordinary experience. Although we cannot exactly predict the reactions of those we know well, although they may surprise us, we are usually able to make sense of their reaction in light of their past reactions. Our various mannerisms, gestures and emotional reactions lend themselves to groupings in various patterns of reactions. On the one hand, these patterns do not follow a clear rule (or we would be able to predict each other’s emotions much more easily and reliably). And on the other hand, each identifiable pattern brings to light some common aspect in which the behaviours of a pattern are similar to one another. Some reactions resemble one another straightforwardly, like the irritation I feel often with the same rude waiter. This does not mean that each time I see the same rude waiter I will get irritated but, rather, that when I do get irritated, it is partly because of the similarity of the situation now to certain past irritations. Other reactions, as Freud noticed, resemble one another symbolically, such as the resentment one may feel toward one’s female boss here and now symbolising the resentment he has (secretly) harboured for many years toward his mother. As Freud claims, this symbolic connection is also a causal connection and the current resentment is partly caused by the old resentment. Some reactions may be the inverse of one another as in cases of mixed feelings, such as the joy for and the envy of the same friend who achieved something that we wanted for ourselves. Ambivalence, as Freud repeatedly discovered in his case studies, pervades many of our emotional reactions. In Emotions and the Limits of Reason, I propose that our spontaneous emotional life is stitched together through imaginative connections. That is, every reaction of ours is similar to, or symbolic of, the inverse of, or somehow imaginatively relates to, many other reactions from our past. The emotional imaginative network thus gives rise to many traceable patterns. And for each pattern one could, in principle, articulate the respects in which the reactions that follow it connect with one another imaginatively, through similarities and symbols or inversions etc. When we articulate thematic threads that run through such patterns, we can identify various cares and concerns that emerge from our emotional-imaginative network about people and things, ideas, virtues and styles of social interaction. If we are able to identify patterns of both the reactions we endorse and the reactions we normally ignore, the cares and concerns that emerge from our imaginative-emotional network would include those we ordinarily endorse as well as those that we normally fail to recognise. The similarities and other imaginative connections among our various spontaneous reactions do not come at first instance with “subtitles” or with a list of the respects in which they hold. Yet, given that these respects can be articulated in language, these similarities make implicit use of familiar labels. And some of these labels are clichés; they are prejudiced and stereotyped models for being a woman or a parent or good etc. Sometimes, an imaginative emotional network can also give rise to inversions among various patterns such that one group of patterns falls under one social label and another group of patterns under the contrary social label. Such a person may endorse one label and ignore the counter-label. As Freud remarks, “the unconscious [is] the precise contrary of the conscious”(“Notes” 180). Consciously, we do not like to appear contradictory. But, to paraphrase another Freudian maxim, the unconscious knows no contradiction or negation. Imaginatively speaking, this person occupies two—apparently conflicting—positive prototypes of womanhood or adulthood etc., one through her endorsed private persona and another without acknowledgment. But there is no reason to suppose that inversion is the only kind of unity available nor that overcoming it is a once and for all effort, as Lear suggests. Our private persona may inhabit more than one such clichéd couple of apparently conflicting stereotypes that may or may not cause emotional turmoil at various stages in life. On the picture I propose, we may dig ourselves out of one cliché about our private persona and then find ourselves in another. Alternatively, the same cliché may return to haunt us. The solutions we may find to our personalised Socratic question in Lear’s clinic—such as “what is a woman?” or, “what is a sacrifice?”—do not comprise the final word, not for society and not for oneself. Living with Yourself There is no graduation from therapy or immunisation to clichéd inversions or to the pathologies they may cause at some stage in our lives. Perhaps what one can acquire is an attentive attitude to one’s spontaneous behaviours, including those that are not compatible with one’s endorsed private persona. The skill involves the capacity to “listen” to one’s emotions and passing thoughts, to notice one’s non-reflective gestures and ways of interacting and let them inform one’s endorsed cares and concerns and valued styles, character traits and virtues. The goal is not to unify one’s private persona and reach some ideal peace where one is exactly what one wants to be. The goal is, rather, to attend to the spontaneous interruptions of one’s endorsed private persona and at times be prepared to doubt or negotiate the way one sees and wants to see oneself. References Breuer J. and S. Freud. Studies on Hysteria [1893-1895]. S.E. vol. 2. Freud, Sigmund. The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud [1966]. Trans. James Strachey. London: The Hogarth Press, 1995. Freud, Sigmund. Notes upon a Case of Obsessional Neurosis [1909]. S.E. vol. 10, 155-249. Freud, Sigmund. “The Unconscious [1915].” S.E. vol. 14, 166-215. Greenspan, Patricia. “A Case of Mixed Feelings: Ambivalence and the Logic of Emotion.” Explaining Emotions. Ed. Amélie Rorty. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1980. 223-250. Korsgaard, Christine M. The Sources of Normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996. Korsgaard, Christine M. “Self Constitution and Irony.” A Case for Irony. Ed. J. Lear. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2011. 75-83. Lear, Jonathan. A Case for Irony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2011. Morag, Talia. Emotions and the Limits of Reason: The Role of the Imagination in Explaining Pathological Emotions. PhD Thesis. University of Sydney, 2013. Rorty, Amélie. “Explaining Emotions.” Ed. Amélie Rorty. Explaining Emotions. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1980. 103-126. Sartre, J. P. Being and Nothingness: An Essay on Phenomenological Ontology [1943]. Trans. Hazel E. Barnes. NY: Philosophical Library, 1984.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography