Journal articles on the topic 'Crosscultural Psychology'

To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Crosscultural Psychology.

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 49 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Crosscultural Psychology.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Batten, Cecilia. "Dilemmas of?Crosscultural Psychotherapy Supervision?" British Journal of Psychotherapy 7, no. 2 (December 1990): 129–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0118.1990.tb01327.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kupavskaya, Aleksandra S. "The Teacher and Scientific Mum: Dedicated to the Memory of Tatiana G. Stefanenkо." RUDN Journal of Psychology and Pedagogics 17, no. 1 (December 15, 2020): 189–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.22363/2313-1683-2020-17-1-189-195.

Full text
Abstract:
The article is dedicated to the memory of Tatiana G. Stefanenko, Professor of the Faculty of Psychology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, the teacher and the friend, and talking about the way she influenced and inspired the new generation of crosscultural psychologists on personal and professional levels.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Saraswathi, T. S. "Many Deities, One God: Towards Convergence in Cultural and Crosscultural Psychology." Culture & Psychology 4, no. 2 (June 1998): 147–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1354067x9800400201.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Dixon, Jason M., and John Barletta. "Crosscultural Counselling for Japanese Adolescents Experiencing Acculturative Stress." Australian Journal of Guidance and Counselling 13, no. 2 (December 2003): 167–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1037291100002867.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this article is to define and describe the issues relating to acculturation and acculturative stress. A proposed counselling approach for acculturative stress is presented and has been adapted from interventions for social phobia, which considers of the role of the crosscultural counsellor as well as models of cultural orientation and identity. Suggestions for counsellor preparation for dealing with culturally diverse clients are provided drawing upon resources and methods from the field of cultural anthropology. The intention of presenting these ideas and strategies will propose ways to assist minority groups, like international students, improve and maintain psychological wellbeing and quality of life. Finally, recommendations for further research in the areas of acculturation and acculturative stress are identified.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Hwang, Jin. "The Psychology of Korean·Chinese Swimming Athlete’s Regret : Crosscultural analysis of Counterfactual Thinking." Journal of Sport and Leisure Studies 59 (February 28, 2015): 437–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.51979/kssls.2015.02.59.437.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Hwang, Jin. "The Psychology of Korean·Chinese Swimming Athlete’s Regret : Crosscultural analysis of Counterfactual Thinking." Journal of Sport and Leisure Studies 59, no. 1 (February 28, 2015): 437–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.51979/kssls.2015.02.59.1.437.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

McGurk, Harry. "Real space and represented space: Crosscultural convergences." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12, no. 1 (March 1989): 90–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00024444.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Foa, Uriel G., Barbara Anderson, John Converse, William A. Urbansky, Michael J. Cawley, Solveig M. Muhlhausen, and Kjell Y. Tornblom. "Gender-related sexual attitudes: Some crosscultural similarities and differences." Sex Roles 16, no. 9-10 (May 1987): 511–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00292485.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

van Brakel, J. "Consciousness is not a natural kind." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18, no. 2 (June 1995): 269–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00038437.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractBlocks distinction between “phenomenal feel” consciousness and “thought/cognition” consciousness is a cultural construction. Consciousness (and its “subspecies”) is not a natural kind. Some crosscultural data are presented to support this.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Cheng, Tai Ann. "Symptomatology of minor psychiatric morbidity: a crosscultural comparison." Psychological Medicine 19, no. 3 (August 1989): 697–708. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700024296.

Full text
Abstract:
SynopsisThe basic symptoms of minor psychiatric morbidity (MPM) reported elsewhere were also found in a community survey in Taiwan. However, differences in the patterns of and manifestations of the symptoms were evident. Contrary to most Western surveys, the prevalence of anxiety (24·7 %) was found to be higher than that of depression (8·3 %) in Taiwan. Possible explanations based on sociocultural characteristics of the Chinese family were proposed. The notion of somatization as a predominant symptom in Chinese neurotic patients advocated by some research workers was not supported in this study. As a result of findings in community cases, it is argued that the importance of somatization has been considerably overemphasized as a factor in the illness behaviour of neurotic cases in Chinese and other cultures, and it is therefore not a culturespecific disease phenomenon. It is also suggested that certain culture-specific neurotic syndromes reported in Chinese, such asshen-ching-shuai-jo(neurasthenia) andshen-k'uei(semen loss syndrome), are clinically equivalent to MPM. Implications of the present findings on crosscultural research and management of MPM were discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Barrett, Paula M., Robi Sonderegger, and Noleen L. Sonderegger. "Assessment of Child and Adolescent Migrants to Australia: A Crosscultural Comparison." Behaviour Change 19, no. 4 (December 1, 2002): 220–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/bech.19.4.220.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis study examines whether young migrants, differentiated by cultural background, (a) vary in their experience of cultural adjustment, emotional distress, levels of self-esteem, and coping ability, and (b) how they compare with Australian students on measures of self-esteem and coping ability. One hundred and seventy-three students differentiated by cultural origin (former-Yugoslavian, Chinese, Mixed-culture, and Australian) and school level (primary and high school) were recruited at random from public schools in South East Queensland. Students completed measures of cultural adjustment (Bicultural Involvement Questionnaire), anxiety and trauma (Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale, Trauma Symptom Checklist), self-esteem (Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory, Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale), and coping ability (Coping Scale for Children and Adolescents). The main findings from this study indicate that culturally diverse groups residing in Australia vary in their experience of cultural adaptation, level of self-esteem, and symptoms of emotional distress, illustrating culture-specific strengths and weaknesses among young non-English speaking (NESB) students. This study reveals information on how culturally diverse migrants acculturate, the type and severity of symptoms they experience, and their capacity to cope in stressful situations. The need for culture-specific early intervention and prevention programs is discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Choi, Sook-Hyun, and Patricia M. Keith. "Are “Worlds of Pain” Crosscultural? Korean Working Class Marriages." Journal of Comparative Family Studies 22, no. 3 (October 1, 1991): 293–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.3138/jcfs.22.3.293.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Mangweth, Barbara, Harrison G. Pope, Jr., James I. Hudson, Roberto Olivardia, Johannes Kinzl, and Wilfried Biebl. "Eating Disorders in Austrian Men: An Intracultural and Crosscultural Comparison Study." Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 66, no. 4 (1997): 214–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000289137.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Sonn, Christopher C., Darren C. Garvey, Brian J. Bishop, and Leigh M. Smith. "Incorporating Indigenous and Crosscultural Issues into an Undergraduate Psychology Course: Experience at Curtin University of Technology." Australian Psychologist 35, no. 2 (July 2000): 143–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00050060008260336.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Moscoso, Manolete S., and Charles D. Spielberger. "Cross-cultural assessment of emotions: The expression of anger." Revista de Psicología 29, no. 2 (April 26, 2011): 343–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.18800/psico.201102.007.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this article is to focus on unique issues that are encountered in the crosscultural adaptation of measures of emotions. We take into consideration the cross-cultural equivalence of the concept of emotion, and how cultural differences influence the meaning of words that are utilized to describe these concepts. The critical need to take the state-trait distinction into account in adapting measures of emotional states and personality traits is then discussed. The effects of language and culture in adapting measures of the experience, expression, and control of anger in Latin-America are also reviewed. The construction of the Latin American Multicultural State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory is described.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Matsumoto, Yuki, Kate Sofronoff, and Matthew R. Sanders. "The Efficacy and Acceptability of the Triple P-Positive Parenting Program With Japanese Parents." Behaviour Change 24, no. 4 (November 1, 2007): 205–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/bech.24.4.205.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis study examined the efficacy of the Group Triple P-Positive Parenting Program with a Japanese population to evaluate the feasibility and acceptability of the program and the parenting skills taught in a crosscultural context. The study involved 50 Japanese parents living in Australia and used a randomised group comparison design with two conditions, Triple P group and a waitlist control group. The results revealed significant reductions in parent reported child behaviour problems, parental overreactivity and laxness, and parental conflict as well as increasing parental competence. The acceptability of the program was found to be high. Intervention effects and program acceptability in a Japanese context as well as limitations and future research are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Kwaśniewska, Aneta, Kevin Thomas, and Roger Baker. "Are there cross-cultural differences in emotional processing and social problem-solving?" Polish Psychological Bulletin 45, no. 2 (June 1, 2014): 205–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/ppb-2014-0026.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Emotional processing and social problem-solving are important for mental well-being. For example, impaired emotional processing is linked with depression and psychosomatic problems. However, little is known about crosscultural differences in emotional processing and social problem-solving and whether these constructs are linked. This study examines whether emotional processing and social problem-solving differs between Western (British) and Eastern European (Polish) cultures. Participants (N = 172) completed questionnaires assessing both constructs. Emotional processing did not differ according to culture, but Polish participants reported more effective social problem-solving abilities than British participants. Poorer emotional processing was also found to relate to poorer social problem-solving. Possible societal reasons for the findings and the implications of the findings for culture and clinical practice are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Vrij, Aldert, and FRANS WILLEM WINKEL. "Crosscultural Police-Citizen Interactions: The Influence of Race, Beliefs, and Nonverbal Communication on Impression Formation1." Journal of Applied Social Psychology 22, no. 19 (October 1992): 1546–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00965.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Sibley, Chris G., Maree Hunt, and David N. Harper. "Identifying Crosscultural Differences in the Effectiveness of an Information and Free Child Seat Rental Program." Behaviour Change 18, no. 4 (December 1, 2001): 224–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/bech.18.4.224.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractPorirua (a suburb in Wellington, New Zealand) is a problem area for child restraint use and has been targeted by a variety of government-initiated, and largely ineffective, traffic safety campaigns in recent years (e.g., Gouldsbury, 1999). The present study attempted to increase child restraint use in cars at two Porirua Kindergartens, one predominantly Pacific Nations (Kindergarten A), and the other predominantly New Zealand European (Kindergarten B), by providing parents with information packages and vouchers for free child seat rental. An increase in correct child seat use was not observed at either kindergarten, although all unrestrained children observed during baseline at the predominantly New Zealand European kindergarten changed to wearing seat belts after the intervention. This finding suggests that income limitations per se are not the primary factor maintaining child seat non-use. Discussion focuses on the contradictory findings provided by both previous survey and observational research on the effect of ethnicity and income on child seat use. Potential crosscultural differences in the existence and salience of verbal community effects that may maintain child seat use through the avoidance of social punishment contingencies from other parents within the kindergarten are considered as one possible explanation for the present findings.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Lang, Jessica, and Bernhard Schmitz. "German Translation of the Satisfaction With Life Scale for Children and Adolescents." Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 38, no. 3 (June 11, 2019): 291–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0734282919849361.

Full text
Abstract:
The research in the field of positive psychology is alive for children and adolescents, and especially the effects of intervention studies are promising. While a large number of instruments exist for adults, there is a need for greater attention to basic measurement work of constructs for children. The Satisfaction With Life Scale is one of the most common questionnaires used in the field of positive psychology. Whereas the adult version is translated into many languages, the child version has only been translated into a few languages yet. This is a limitation for countries where young students are not familiar with the English language. The aim of the present study was to translate the Satisfaction With Life Scale–child version (SWLS-C) into German with the use of a backtranslation-proceeding (Study 1) and to validate the questionnaire with a sample of 1,099 students aged 8 to 17 years (Study 2). This included retesting of reliability as well as concurrent and discriminant validity by additionally measuring several established constructs of positive psychology. Model fit indices, internal consistencies, retest reliability as well as concurrent and discriminant validity evidence were satisfying. The German version of the SWLS-C can be used in future studies, but to compare SWLS-C data of different nationalities more translations of the questionnaire as well as crosscultural measurement invariance studies are needed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Yang, Qing, Oscar Ybarra, Kees van den Bos, Yufang Zhao, Lili Guan, and Xiting Huang. "Temporal self-appraisal in a Chinese context: Distancing autobiographical memory following self-uncertainty salience." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 48, no. 2 (February 5, 2020): 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.8632.

Full text
Abstract:
On the premise that individuals are inclined to self-enhance, in temporal self-appraisal (TSA) theory it is suggested that people can motivationally reconstruct subjective distances from their past self to serve that goal. However, given the mixed evidence found in an East Asian cultural context (i.e., Japan), it is important to test the cultural applicability of TSA in a different East Asian culture. Thus we tested the TSA of a Chinese sample, focusing on past-self distance reconstruction. The results supported the prediction suggested in TSA theory, in that participants tended to feel farther away from negative (vs. positive) past experiences. Further, this effect was greater when people were primed with a self-threat (i.e., self-uncertainty salience). These patterns were found independently of whether the past experience was recent (3 months ago) or in the distant past (3 years ago). Implications for crosscultural applicability of TSA theory are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Györkös, Christina, Jurgen Becker, Koorosh Massoudi, Gideon P. de Bruin, and Jérôme Rossier. "The Impact of Personality and Culture on the Job Demands-Control Model of Job Stress." Swiss Journal of Psychology 71, no. 1 (January 2012): 21–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000065.

Full text
Abstract:
Among the various work stress models, one of the most popular has been the job demands-control (JDC) model developed by Karasek (1979 ), which postulates that work-related strain is highest under work conditions characterized by high demands and low autonomy. The absence of social support at work further increases negative outcomes. This model, however, does not apply equally to all individuals and to all cultures. This review demonstrates how various individual characteristics, especially some personality dimensions, influence the JDC model and could thus be considered buffering or moderator factors. Moreover, we review how the cultural context impacts this model as suggested by results obtained in European, American, and Asian contexts. Yet there are almost no data from Africa or South America. More crosscultural studies including populations from these continents would be valuable for a better understanding of the impact of the cultural context on the JDC model.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Maurino, Daniel E. "Crosscultural Perspectives in Human Factors Training: Lessons From the ICAO Human Factors Program." International Journal of Aviation Psychology 4, no. 2 (April 1994): 173–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327108ijap0402_5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Ortlieb, Stefan, Ivan Stojilovic, Danaja Rutar, Uwe Fischer, and Claus-Christian Carbon. "On kitsch and kic: Comparing kitsch concepts from Bavaria, Serbia and Slovenia." Psihologija 50, no. 3 (2017): 357–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/psi1703357o.

Full text
Abstract:
The German word kitsch has been internationally successful. Today, it is commonly used in many modern languages including Serbian and Slovenian (kic)-but does it mean the same? In a pilot study, thirty-six volunteers from Bavaria, Serbia and Slovenia rated two hundred images of kitsch objects in terms of liking, familiarity, determinacy, arousal, perceived threat, and kitschiness. Additionally, art expertise, ambiguity tolerance, and value orientations were assessed. Multilevel regression analysis with crossed random effects was used to explore crosscultural differences: Regardless of cultural background, liking of kitsch objects was positively linked to emotionally arousing items with non-threatening content. Self-transcendence was positively linked to liking, while ambiguity of the parental image was concordantly associated with kitschiness. For participants from Serbia and Slovenia, threatening content was correlated with kitschiness, while participants from Bavaria rated determinate items as kitschier. Results are discussed with regard to literature on kitsch and implications for future research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Shi, Guanfeng, Zhihui Xie, Yulin Niu, Jie Tang, and Huiwei Pang. "Benevolent Leadership and Employee Task Performance: Chain Intermediary Role of Personal Initiative and Work Engagement in Crosscultural Management." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 50, no. 6 (June 1, 2022): 49–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.11528.

Full text
Abstract:
This study explored the antecedent mechanism of task performance. We collected data from teams working on Chinese overseas projects in 13 countries and regions, and conducted a cross-layer analysis through matching the data of 70 direct leaders and 277 employees. The results show that benevolent leadership was positively associated with employees' task performance, personal initiative, and work engagement. In addition, employees' personal initiative and work engagement played a chain intermediary role in the relationship between benevolent leadership and employee task performance. These findings show how benevolent leadership influences employee task performance in cross-cultural management. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Abouchedid, Kamal, and Ramzi Nasser. "ATTRIBUTIONS OF RESPONSIBILITY FOR POVERTY AMONG LEBANESE AND PORTUGUESE UNIVERSITY STUDENTS: A CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISON." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 30, no. 1 (January 1, 2002): 25–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2002.30.1.25.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examines poverty attitudes among Portuguese and Lebanese students (n=372) along Feagin's fatalistic, individualistic, and structuralistic dimensions. Results show that class and nationality are important variables for predicting the causes of poverty in cross-cultural terms. Lebanese students had higher agreements on the fatalistic dimension of poverty than did Portuguese. Significant differences were found between the middle-class Portuguese and Lebanese students on the individualistic and fatalistic dimensions. Middle-class Lebanese students were significantly more fatalistic than their Portuguese counterparts. Furthermore, middle-class Lebanese students documented greater individualistic interpretations of poverty than did Portuguese. MANCOVA test, which used class crossed with nationality on the poverty dimensions, and gender as a covariate did not yield significant differences between means. Wilks' Lambda regression coefficient showed a significant interaction between-class and nationality on the fatalistic dimension. Although the results portray different scores of poverty from those in previous studies, Lebanese students' structuralistic attributions are explained by the present economic and social crises of their country which transcend a strong orientation of system blame. Recommendations are offered for future crosscultural research on poverty.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Davidson, Graham R., Kate E. Murray, and Robert D. Schweitzer. "Review of Refugee Mental Health Assessment: Best Practices and Recommendations." Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology 4, no. 1 (May 1, 2010): 72–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/prp.4.1.72.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis article focuses on mental health assessment of refugees in clinical, educational and administrative-legal settings in order to synthesise research and practice designed to enhance and promote further development of culturally appropriate clinical assessment services during the refugee resettlement process. It specifically surveys research published over the last 25 years into the development, reliability measurement and validity testing of assessment instruments, which have been used with children, adolescents and adults from refugee backgrounds, prior to or following their arrival in a resettlement country, to determine whether the instruments meet established crosscultural standards of conceptual, functional, linguistic, technical and normative equivalence. The findings suggest that, although attempts have been made to develop internally reliable, appropriately normed tests for use with refugees from diverse cultural and linguistic backgrounds, matters of conceptual and linguistic equivalence and test–retest reliability are often overlooked. Implications of these oversights for underreporting refugees' mental health needs are considered. Efforts should also be directed towards development of culturally comparable, valid and reliable measures of refugee children's mental health and of refugee children's and adults' psychoeducational, neuropsychological and applied memory capabilities.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Alessandri, Guido, Michele Vecchione, Gianvittorio Caprara, and Tera D. Letzring. "The Ego Resiliency Scale Revised." European Journal of Psychological Assessment 28, no. 2 (November 1, 2012): 139–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000102.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study examined the crosscultural generalizability of the latent structure of the ER89-R, a brief self-report scale that measures ego-resiliency with subjective self-ratings. First, we investigated the measurement invariance of the scale across three Western cultures, namely, Italy (n = 1,020), Spain (n = 452), and the United States (n = 808). Next, we examined the correlations of the ER89-R scale with several measures of adjustment and maladjustment. Multigroup confirmatory factor analysis provided evidence of partial configural, metric, and scalar invariance across Italy, Spain, and the United States. Overall, the correlation patterns were stable across countries and sex, with some exceptions. As expected, higher levels of ego-resiliency were strongly and consistently associated with the positive poles of the Big Five. Moreover, ego-resiliency showed a positive correlation with psychological well-being in each country, and negative relations with depression in Spain and Italy, but not in the United States. In light of these results, the potential usefulness and applicability of the ER89-R scale are advanced and discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Beinorius, Audrius, Renatas Berniūnas, Vilius Dranseika, Paulius Rimkevičius, Vytis Silius, and Agnė Veisaitė. "Beyond Free Will: Variety in Understanding of Choice, Luck, and Necessity." Vilnius University Proceedings 23 (June 1, 2022): 1–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/beyoundfreewill.2022.

Full text
Abstract:
Contemporary Western discourse on freedom and choice – some of the most championed modern values – is usually anchored in the concept cluster of free will and autonomous choice. In turn, academic research on free will in philosophy (including experimental philosophy) and psychology is largely based on a limited conceptual framework with roots in particular debates in Christianity and European philosophy. This framework is currently challenged by multidisciplinary and interdisciplinary approaches applied in the fields of area and Asian studies, comparative philosophy, and also empirical research in cross-cultural psychology, and anthropology. One reason for this challenge is that the dominant Western academic approach, with its almost exclusive focus on concepts of free will and causal determinism, neglects the multitude of non-Western cultural traditions. In most parts of the world, these traditions continue to shape everyday practices and conceptualizations of free action, choice, and decision. Traditions also provide various strategies for navigating the constraints on human agency.In the present multidisciplinary and interdisciplinary conference, we invited scholars from philosophy, psychology, anthropology, Asian studies, religious studies and other related fields to discuss theoretical alternatives to the dominant framework that are sensitive to cultural differences and local contexts as well as empirical research – especially crosscultural and cross-linguistic – on conceptualizations of free and constrained action and cultural practices in dealing with these constraints.This project “Between choice and determinism: cultural variations in experiencing and conceptualizing free will, luck and randomness” has received funding from European Social Fund (project No. 09.3.3-LMT-K-712-01-0111) under grant agreement with the Research Council of Lithuania (LMTLT).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Skuy, Mervyn, Martene Mentis, Fleur Durbach, Kate Cockcroft, Peter Fridjhon, and Mandia Mentis. "Crosscultural Comparison of Effects of Instrumental Enrichment on Children in a South African Mining Town." School Psychology International 16, no. 3 (August 1995): 265–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0143034395163003.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Mak, Kwok Kei, Kimberly S. Young, Hiroko Watanabe, Milen S. Ramos, and JeeEun Karin Nam. "Crosscultural Measurement Invariance of the Internet Addiction Test-Revised (IAT-R) Among Japanese and Filipino University Students." Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 23, no. 7 (July 1, 2020): 479–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2019.0360.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Bornstein, Marc H., Sharone L. Maital, Joseph Tal, and Rebecca Baras. "Mother and Infant Activity and Interaction in Israel and in the United States: A Comparative Study." International Journal of Behavioral Development 18, no. 1 (March 1995): 63–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/016502549501800104.

Full text
Abstract:
Activities and interactions of Israeli and US mothers and their 5-month-old infants were observed in the natural setting of their homes. This report examines infant visual and tactual exploration and vocalisation as well as maternal stimulation and speech. First, similarities and differences in activities between Israeli and US infants and mothers are assessed. Next, coherence in infant activities and in maternal activities within each society are evaluated, and resultant patterns of coherence between the two societies are compared. Last, correspondences between infant and maternal activities in each society are analysed, and resultant patterns of mother-infant interactions between the two societies are compared. Identification and description of activities, interactions, and developmental processes which are similar and different in comparable segments of Israeli and US society are discussed, and crosscultural tests of developmental issues related to coherence and to correspondence of activity in mother-infant dyads are evaluated. Israeli and US mothers may follow culture-specific paths in striving to meet infants' needs and in achieving socialisation goals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Grygiel, Pawel, Grzegorz Humenny, Slawomir Rebisz, Piotr Świtaj, and Justyna Sikorska. "Validating the Polish Adaptation of the 11-Item De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale." European Journal of Psychological Assessment 29, no. 2 (May 1, 2013): 129–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000130.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this study was to translate into Polish and establish the psychometric properties of the 11-item De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale (DJGLS). The translation process followed the recent guidelines for the crosscultural adaptation of questionnaires and was tested through analysis of differential item functioning (DIF) by the use of the Poly-SIBTEST method and bilingual groups. The essential unidimensionality was checked by bifactor analysis. Reliability was assessed by internal consistency and homogeneity analysis, and external construct validity by correlation with several external scales. The research indicated no differences in item performance between the final Polish and English versions, and confirmed the earlier findings indicating that the DJGLS measures two dimensions of loneliness (social and emotional) which generalize into a higher-order factor of a general sense of loneliness (bifactor structure). Reliability (α = .89) and homogeneity (H = .47) proved to be high. Research showed that the instrument has a satisfactory validity criterion: Correlation with the UCLA Loneliness Scale r = .82; with Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale r = –.56; and with the Beck Depression Inventory r = .46 (all p < .01). The Polish adaptation of the DJGLS thus presents a bifactor structure, with good levels of internal consistency, homogeneity, and construct validity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Kuntsche, Emmanuel, and Florian Labhart. "ICAT: Development of an Internet-Based Data Collection Method for Ecological Momentary Assessment Using Personal Cell Phones." European Journal of Psychological Assessment 29, no. 2 (May 1, 2013): 140–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000137.

Full text
Abstract:
Rapid advances in mobile data-transfer technologies offer new possibilities in the use of cell phones to conduct assessments of a person’s natural environment in real time. This paper describes features of a new Internet-based, cell phone-optimized assessment technique (ICAT), which consists of a retrospective baseline assessment combined with text messages sent to the participants’ personal cell phones providing a hyperlink to an Internet-stored cell phone-optimized questionnaire. Two participation conditions were used to test variations in response burden. Retention rates, completion rates, and response times in different subgroups were tested by means of χ² tests, Cox regression, and logistic regression. Among the 237 initial participants, we observed a retention rate of 90.3% from the baseline assessment to the cell-phone part, and 80.4% repeated participation in the 30 daily assessments. Each day, 40–70% of the questionnaires were returned, a fourth in less than 3 minutes. Qualitative interviews underscored the ease of use of ICAT. This technique appears to be an innovative, convenient, and cost-effective way of collecting data on situational characteristics while minimizing recall bias. Because of its flexibility, ICAT can be applied in various disciplines, whether as part of small pilot studies or large-scale, crosscultural, and multisite research projects.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

BEKEŠ, Andrej. "Foreword." Acta Linguistica Asiatica 2, no. 1 (May 22, 2012): 5–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.4312/ala.2.1.5-6.

Full text
Abstract:
With the present issue of ALA, we are starting the second year in its new incarnation. As the paper is striving to cover the Asian languages in their multiplicity and in the multiplicity of approaches, I am glad to say that this issue offers both, in line with Roman Jakobson’s famous paraphrase “Linguista sum, linguistici nihil a me alienum puto.”Among the six papers in this issue, two papers deal with Japanese, two with Iranian langages, and one each with the language of Ṛgveda and with Arabic. Also, approaches vary from historical phonetics/phonology (paper by John KUPCHIK on the role of rendaku in Eastern Old Japanese poetry) and historical syntax (Tamara DITRICH’s discussion of coordinative particles in Ṛgveda), to typological considerations spannig diachroinc and synchronic views (Yadgar KARIMI’s analysis of the evolution of ergative in Iranian languages). Syncronic approaches comprise pragmatics (Biook BEHNAM and Salam KHALILIAQDAM’s treatment of hedging devices in Kurdish and Robert Michael BIANCHI’s account of the new hybrid language of 3arabizi), and interdisciplinary interpretation of lexis in the light of crosscultural psychology (Márton SZEMEREY’s paper comparing Japanese and Hungarian linguistic resources for expression of emotions).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Gibbons, Pat. "Current research issues in cross-cultural psychiatry." Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine 13, no. 3 (September 1996): 120–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0790966700002718.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe international pilot study of schizophrenia (IPSS) was the first major study to show that the use of standardised assessment and classification instruments allows the reliable comparison of data on the prevalence, psychopathology and prognosis of major psychiatric illness between different cultures. Important questions about the methodology used in cross-cultural research remain to be answered, however These include the inherently ‘Eurocentric’ nature of much of western psychiatric terminology and the absence of directly comparable concepts and language to describe emotional and psychological distress between western and non-western cultures. These difficulties especially arise in relation to illness where organic factors appear to contribute little to aetiology, such as the neurotic and Axis II disorders, and need to be overcome before useful crosscultural research into these disorders can be accomplished.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Segal, Jacqueline. "Therapeutic Process as a Means to Navigate Impasse? Reflections on a Complex Case of Crosscultural Adolescent Family Therapy." Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy (ANZJFT) 31, no. 3 (September 2010): 266–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/anft.31.3.266.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Gullone, Eleonora. "Developmental Psychopathology and Normal Fear." Behaviour Change 13, no. 3 (September 1996): 143–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0813483900004927.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper reviews the extensive research examining developmental patterns in normal fear. Areas of focus include age, gender, and socioeconomic status differences in fear content, prevalence, and intensity. The structure and stability/duration of normal fears are also discussed. Finally, the crosscultural research in this area is reviewed. Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies indicate that fear decreases in prevalence and intensity with age. There are also major changes in the content of normal fear over the course of development. Such changes are characterised by a transition from infant fears which are related to immediate, concrete, and prepotent stimuli, and which are largely noncognitive, to fears of late childhood and adolescence which are related to anticipatory, abstract, and more global stimuli and events. Fears of late childhood and adolescence are also more cognitive. One of the prominent themes during these years is social evaluation. It is noteworthy that fears have been demonstrated to be largely transitory in nature, decreasing significantly in number and intensity over time and with maturation. Future research into normal fear should more closely examine the validity of current assessment techniques. There is also a need for research into the developmental correlates of fear, including individual differences and affective environmental experiences. In particular, research examining the correlates of normal fear within a developmental psychopathology framework is recommended.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Florova, N. B. "Dennis Steve Smith 'The predictive relationship between cultural identity, value orientation, acculturation and the crosscultural student's academic motivation in the international school setting'." Современная зарубежная психология 4, no. 2 (2015): 88–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.17759/jmfp.2015040207.

Full text
Abstract:
The current level of global scientific school of evidence-based prevention helps to assess a student's ability to adapt to a complex society and to prevent the personality disorder. The complexity of the society in the education space is largely connected with multiculturalism. The Southeast Asian countries implement successfully for a long time evidence-based interdisciplinary, transnational projects, focused on management training motivation as a factor of the quality of an educational process. The article discusses the methodological function of cultural identity within the educational process in the contexts of adaptation problems in children with "mixed cultural background" or belonging to the "third culture" in the contemporary world, the phenomenon of their "cultural homelessness" and the specificity of their training motivation. The latest data on teaching quality forecasting resources will be of interest to specialists in educational psychology, preventology and many other domains
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Semenog, Olena. "Professional Training of Future Teacher in Cross-Cultural Dialogue." Comparative Professional Pedagogy 4, no. 3 (September 1, 2014): 38–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/rpp-2014-0033.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract On the example of propaedeutic educational course “Introduction to Slavic Philology” features of future teachers’ professional training of cross-cultural dialogue are considered. Among the main objectives of the course, attention is focused on native language and other languages admirer’s tolerance education, students’ skills formation of making linguistic and cultural comments on folklore, literary texts and discourses; building a strategy and tactics of intercultural communication in accordance to national psychology. The examples of tokens, “ukrainisms” in Slavic languages and phrases are presented; also, it is considered Slavists as tolerant intermediaries towards intercultural cooperation of countries and peoples, the need for skills of translation activity that caused the crosscultural dialogue. Based on scientific and encyclopedic sources analysis, educational course testing is making conclusion that Slavic philology is a powerful tool for cross-cultural dialogue. It contains of big factual material that promotes understanding the process of formation and development of ethno-cultural communities in time and space, formation of human values and national identity of supranational cross-cultural thought, ways of tolerant behavior, language understanding and usage. Also it creates conditions for the realization of individuality as Ukrainian language and other languages, cultures, intellectual and emotional contacts between these languages and cultures.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Fabian, Myroslava. "SEMANTIC SPECIFICITY OF ADJECTIVES DENOTING A SUCCESSFUL PERSON /THING IN ENGLISH AND UKRAINIAN." Naukovy Visnyk of South Ukrainian National Pedagogical University named after K. D. Ushynsky: Linguistic Sciences 2021, no. 32 (2021): 149–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.24195/2616-5317-2021-32-12.

Full text
Abstract:
Comparative studies fascinate scholars working in various branches of human activity. In linguistics, onlycomparison helps find out both common and distinctive features of the languages and trace their interconnections as well as specificity. The present paper deals with the comparative research of two distantly related languages - English and Ukrainian - on the material of adjectives denoting a successful person/thing. The topic in question is relevant and contributes to further studies of lexical and comparative semantics, cross-language and crosscultural communication, lexicography, etc. The adjectives in question occupy significant places in the vocabulary of the languages under study. The concept of success belongs to basic social and cultural values and is studied in philosophy, sociology, psychology, linguistics and other sciences. Having introduced the methodology of formalized analysis of lexical semantics, one of the requirements of which is a formal criterion – belonging of the lexical units to one part of speech – the author of this paper collected, analyzed and classified the obtained language material from lexicographical sources. Depending on the degree of polysemy, three groups in English and two in Ukrainian have been formed. Each of them possesses its own features alongside with their common characteristics. Comparative research of the definite fragment of lexis resulted in in-depth analysis of its system and structural organization, semantic specificity, both common and distinctive featuresas well as its representation in corresponding language and culture bearers’ consciousness.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Lourengo, Orlando M. "Portuguese Children's Judgements of Moral, Prosocial, and Academic Norms: Duty or Aspiration?" International Journal of Behavioral Development 17, no. 2 (June 1994): 367–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/016502549401700208.

Full text
Abstract:
Borrowing from Fuller's distinction between duty and aspiration, we examined Portuguese children's attributions of credit and blame for good and bad deeds in moral, prosocial, and academic norms. In addition, we made crosscultural comparisons with US and Japanese children, and suggested that the frequently reported tendency for children to give more weight to negative outcomes than positive ones may be due to a common belief (i.e. "fundamental-education error") that states that people should not have to be praised for doing what they should do, but should always be blamed or punished for misbehaviour. A total of 81 children aged from 5 to 10 were presented with moral, prosocial, and academic scenarios representing either adherences to, or violations of, norms. Children's own judgements of credit and blame, and children's predictions of what their teachers would do if forced to choose between sanctioning a positive and a negative act were assessed. The results showed that: (1) Portuguese children's attributions of credit and blame for moral and academic norms were similar to those observed for US and Japanese children with regard to a rank order of domains, but not in absolute terms; (2) at an early age, Portuguese children commit the "fundamental-education error"; and (3) the duty-aspiration asymmetry is not as pervasive as has been claimed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Rolando, Dede Mercy, and Hariya Toni. "MULTICULTURAL PARADIGM AND DA'WAH STRATEGY BUILDING RELIGIOUS MODERATION IN REJANG LEBONG." Alfuad: Jurnal Sosial Keagamaan 5, no. 2 (November 26, 2021): 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.31958/jsk.v5i2.4684.

Full text
Abstract:
The research looks at how the multicultural paradigm influences one's experience and what is the da'wah strategy in dealing with a multicultural society? To conduct this research, the researcher uses a mixed method research (qualitative and quantitative) which aims to combine two methods at once in order to obtain more comprehensive, valid, reliable and objective data. The subjects and objects of this research are students from 3 high schools in Rejang Lebong, Curup city, namely SMAN 1, SMAN 2 and SMAN Xaverius. The results of the research show that the portrait of the paradigm of experience High school students in Rejang Lebong have high enough confidence to consistently interact and adapt to differences. In fact, it is not only considered a luxury, but is interpreted as a necessity where they cannot avoid religious differences. This has implications for their multicultural behavior where they are able to be smart in responding to differences. In this context, the experience of students' multicultural behavior has a very good level of religious and cultural moderation. Of course, this is based on the cultivation of a formal and non-formal understanding of multiculturalism. This study found several strategies that were obtained in order to be able to carry out religious and cultural moderation in a multicultural society including: planting an understanding of the value of tolerance through an educational approach (crosscultural education) to develop universal values that can be accepted by different groups of people, cultural approaches and language, as well as approaches through socio-religious psychology.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

WATKINS, FRANCIS. "Poluha, Eva, and Mona Rosendahl (eds.). 2002. Contesting ‘good’ governance. Crosscultural perspectives on representation, accountability and public space. London: RoutledgeCurzon. viii + 276 pp. Hb.: £65.00. ISBN: 1 7007 1494 4." Social Anthropology 12, no. 2 (June 2004): 241–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s096402820426058x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Flere, Sergej, and Miran Lavric. "Predicting civil religion at a cross-cultural level." Psihologija 42, no. 2 (2009): 159–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/psi0902159f.

Full text
Abstract:
The concept of civil religion has caught major attention among scholars studying the junction of religion and politics (J.-J. Rousseau, E. Durkheim, R. Bellah). The notion focuses on the phenomenon of cultural contents sacralizing and ritualizing the ruling political institutions of a society, extending support to the integration of the political and social system at a cultural level. The notion of civil religion has recently been operationalized crossculturally, but light has not been shed upon its predictors. In this paper authoritarianism is tested as a predictor of civil religion cross-culturally. Four student samples of Bosnian, Serbian, Slovenian and US students were analyzed. Very strong, significant associations between authoritarianism, as operationalized by a modified Lane scale, and civil religion were found in all cases. Moreover, upon introducing femininity, anxiety and gender into the analysis, a strong, dominant and significant impact on the part of authoritarianism was still found when civil religion was observed crossculturally. When the same predictors were applied to explaining general religiosity, authoritarianism fell short of being a significant predictor in most of the environments observed. Such results suggest an especially close link between civil religion and authoritarianism.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Jong, Kim, and Philip Spinhoven. "De Nederlandse versie van de Outcome Questionnaire (OQ-45): een crossculturele validatie." Psychologie & gezondheid 36, no. 1 (February 2008): 35–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf03077465.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Tsang, Hin-Yeung, Main-Yoon Chong, and Andrew T. A. Cheng. "Development of the Chinese Version of the Geriatric Mental State Schedule." International Psychogeriatrics 14, no. 2 (June 2002): 219–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1041610202008414.

Full text
Abstract:
Objective: To test the validity and reliability of the Chinese Geriatric Mental State Schedule (CGMS) in Taiwanese elders. Methods: The CGMS has gone through a standardized two-way translation, a pretest phase, and consensus focus group meetings in order to modify relevant culture-related terms of the original English version. The interrater reliability of the CGMS among eight psychiatrists was conducted after a training course was given to them. Diagnoses generated by the CGMS-AGECAT (Automated Geriatric Examination for Computer Assisted Taxonomy) were compared with psychiatric diagnoses according to the DSM-III-R criteria. The sample subjects were aged 65 and over and recruited from a community (n = 36) and an “old age home” (n = 56). Results: Four of the eight diagnostic categories generated by the CGMS-AGECAT had a generalized kappa value of 1.0, and the figures for the remaining four categories were acceptable: .8 for depressive neurosis, .6 for anxiety disorder, .5 for schizophrenia, and .5 for depressive psychosis (generalized kappa = .5). The overall agreement between the CGMS-AGECAT and independent psychiatric diagnosis (based on the DSM-III-R criteria) was satisfactory. Conclusion: The CGMS has been found to be a crossculturally valid and reliable instrument for use in Taiwan.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Kaniuka, Inna, and Liudmyla Serdiuk. ""Language coach" as an instrument for the crosscultural competence formation and development." OPEN EDUCATIONAL E-ENVIRONMENT OF MODERN UNIVERSITY, no. 5 (2018). http://dx.doi.org/10.28925/2414-0325.2018.5.123132.

Full text
Abstract:
The article reveals the description of a scientific and practical project aimed at developing cross-cultural competence among students of the Borys Grinchenko University of Kyiv. The mechanisms of formation and development of cross-cultural competence among students of the youth are revealed, as well as theoretically, the structural components of the above mentioned competence are researched in order to further successful adaptation of the personality in the process of intercultural communication. These components include: sensitivity to intercultural differences and linguistic understanding. An interactive tool for the formation and development of cross-cultural competence, author's personal diary "Language coach" was developed and tested. The principles and levels of this tool are described. Principles include: principles of conscious learning and conscious choice, interaction, positive attitude, creativity, reflexivity, self-motivation and readiness to act, self-management and monitoring, active and productive action. The content of the co-educational, interactive, creative, activity and reflexive levels of the proposed author's interactive tool is revealed. The peculiarities of the linguistic and socio-cultural component of the indicated instrument are revealed. Italso describes the filling and specificity of the tasks, designed for the 21st day of the diary. Project Duration: February 2017-December 2018. Testing of interactive tools took place during the first semester of the academic year 2018. The participants were students of the 2nd year, numbering 20 people, specialty "Psychology", the Institute of Human Rights of the CUBA. The article provides in detail the format of organization and implementation of the project, interaction with its participants. As a result of the quantitative and qualitative indicators of the empirical part, a psychological portrait of users is created. The advantages and disadvantages of this tool are revealed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Callizo-Romero, Carmen, Slavica Tutnjević, Maja Pandza, Marc Ouellet, Alexander Kranjec, Sladjana Ilić, Yan Gu, et al. "Does time extend asymmetrically into the past and the future? A multitask crosscultural study." Language and Cognition, March 31, 2022, 1–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2022.5.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Does temporal thought extend asymmetrically into the past and the future? Do asymmetries depend on cultural differences in temporal focus? Some studies suggest that people in Western (arguably future-focused) cultures perceive the future as being closer, more valued, and deeper than the past (a future asymmetry), while the opposite is shown in East Asian (arguably past-focused) cultures. The proposed explanations of these findings predict a negative relationship between past and future: the more we delve into the future, the less we delve into the past. Here, we report findings that pose a significant challenge to this view. We presented several tasks previously used to measure temporal asymmetry (self-continuity, time discounting, temporal distance, and temporal depth) and two measures of temporal focus to American, Spanish, Serbian, Bosniak, Croatian, Moroccan, Turkish, and Chinese participants (total N = 1,075). There was an overall future asymmetry in all tasks except for temporal distance, but the asymmetry only varied with cultural temporal focus in time discounting. Past and future held a positive (instead of negative) relation in the mind: the more we delve into the future, the more we delve into the past. Finally, the findings suggest that temporal thought has a complex underlying structure.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography