To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Auditory perception.

Journal articles on the topic 'Auditory perception'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Auditory perception.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Deny Nitalia Mindrawati, Grahita Chandrarin, and Harianto Respati. "The Determinant Of Auditor Career Survivability Adopting The Blockchain Technology." Brilliant International Journal Of Management And Tourism 4, no. 1 (January 26, 2024): 151–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.55606/bijmt.v4i1.2758.

Full text
Abstract:
This empirical study examined the influence of the auditor perspective on the supportive factors of blockchain technology adoption and the implication on auditory career survivability. The current research population consisted of all auditors in Indonesia, 6.034 individuals. The researchers used the Slovin formula to take 375 respondents. The researchers analyzed the obtained data with a validity test, reliability test, and path analysis. The results found the perception of the auditor about the positive and significant influencing factors toward the blockchain technology adoption and survivability of the auditor's career. Blockchain technology adoption could not moderate the survivability of an auditor's career. The current research novelty dealt with the examined variables, from the qualitative study. Then, the researchers developed the research into quantitative research to provide significant evidence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Haas, Ellen C. "Auditory Perception." Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting 36, no. 3 (October 1992): 247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/107118192786751817.

Full text
Abstract:
Auditory perception involves the human listener's awareness or apprehension of auditory stimuli in the environment. Auditory stimuli, which include speech communications as well as non-speech signals, occur in the presence and absence of environmental noise. Non-speech auditory signals range from simple pure tones to complex signals found in three-dimensional auditory displays. Special hearing protection device (HPD) designs, as well as additions to conventional protectors, have been developed to improve speech communication and auditory perception capabilities of those exposed to noise. The thoughtful design of auditory stimuli and the proper design, selection, and use of HPDs within the environment can improve human performance and reduce accidents. The purpose of this symposium will be to discuss issues in auditory perception and to describe methods to improve the perception of auditory stimuli in environments with and without noise. The issues of interest include the perception of non-speech auditory signals and the improvement of auditory perception capabilities of persons exposed to noise. The first three papers of this symposium describe the perception of non-speech auditory signals. Ellen Haas defines the extent to which certain signal elements affect the perceived urgency of auditory warning signals. Michael D. Good and Dr. Robert H. Gilkey investigate free-field masking as a function of the spatial separation between signal and masker sounds within the horizontal and median planes. Jeffrey M. Gerth explores the discrimination of complex auditory signal components that differ by sound category, temporal pattern, density, and component manipulation. The fourth paper of this symposium focuses upon the improvement of auditory perception capabilities of persons exposed to hazardous noise, and who must wear hearing protection. Special HPD designs, as well as additions to conventional protectors, have been developed to improve speech communication and auditory perception capabilities of persons exposed to noise. Dr. John G. Casali reviews several new HPD technologies and describes construction features, empirical performance data, and applications of each device. These papers illustrate current research issues in the perception of auditory signals. The issues are all relevant to the human factors engineering of auditory signals and personal protective gear. The perception of auditory stimuli can be improved by the thoughtful human factors design of auditory stimuli and by the proper use of HPDs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

PURWINS, HENDRIK, BENJAMIN BLANKERTZ, and KLAUS OBERMAYER. "Computing auditory perception." Organised Sound 5, no. 3 (December 2000): 159–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1355771800005069.

Full text
Abstract:
In this paper the ingredients of computing auditory perception are reviewed. On the basic level there is neurophysiology, which is abstracted to artificial neural nets (ANNs) and enhanced by statistics to machine learning. There are high-level cognitive models derived from psychoacoustics (especially Gestalt principles). The gap between neuroscience and psychoacoustics has to be filled by numerics, statistics and heuristics. Computerised auditory models have a broad and diverse range of applications: hearing aids and implants, compression in audio codices, automated music analysis, music composition, interactive music installations, and information retrieval from large databases of music samples.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Kiela, Douwe, and Stephen Clark. "Learning Neural Audio Embeddings for Grounding Semantics in Auditory Perception." Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research 60 (December 26, 2017): 1003–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1613/jair.5665.

Full text
Abstract:
Multi-modal semantics, which aims to ground semantic representations in perception, has relied on feature norms or raw image data for perceptual input. In this paper we examine grounding semantic representations in raw auditory data, using standard evaluations for multi-modal semantics. After having shown the quality of such auditorily grounded representations, we show how they can be applied to tasks where auditory perception is relevant, including two unsupervised categorization experiments, and provide further analysis. We find that features transfered from deep neural networks outperform bag of audio words approaches. To our knowledge, this is the first work to construct multi-modal models from a combination of textual information and auditory information extracted from deep neural networks, and the first work to evaluate the performance of tri-modal (textual, visual and auditory) semantic models.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Zahorik, Pavel. "Auditory/visual distance perception." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 137, no. 4 (April 2015): 2374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4920626.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Merchel, Sebastian, and M. Ercan Altinsoy. "Auditory-tactile music perception." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 133, no. 5 (May 2013): 3256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4805254.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Hirsh, Ira J. "Timing in auditory perception." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 81, S1 (May 1987): S90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.2024468.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Hirsh, Ira J., and Charles S. Watson. "AUDITORY PSYCHOPHYSICS AND PERCEPTION." Annual Review of Psychology 47, no. 1 (February 1996): 461–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.461.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Specht, Karsten, C. Paul Stracke, and Jürgen Reul. "Laterality of auditory perception." NeuroImage 13, no. 6 (June 2001): 942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(01)92284-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Munkong, Rungsun, and Biing-Hwang Juang. "Auditory perception and cognition." IEEE Signal Processing Magazine 25, no. 3 (May 2008): 98–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/msp.2008.918418.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Recanzone, Gregg H. "Perception of auditory signals." Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1224, no. 1 (April 2011): 96–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05920.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Sloos, Marjoleine, and Denis McKeown. "Bias in Auditory Perception." i-Perception 6, no. 5 (October 13, 2015): 204166951560715. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2041669515607153.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Blauert, Jens, and Jonas Braasch. "Auditory perception in rooms." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 141, no. 5 (May 2017): 3498. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4987317.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Povel, D. J. "Auditory perception and speech." Acta Psychologica 75, no. 2 (November 1990): 176–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(90)90091-s.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Lotto, Andrew, and Lori Holt. "Psychology of auditory perception." Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2, no. 5 (October 15, 2010): 479–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.123.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Waterlot, Muriel. "Welke aanpak voor de Nederlandse vertaling van Poolse auditieve verba sentiendi?" Neerlandica Wratislaviensia 33 (November 17, 2022): 139–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.19195/0860-0716.33.10.

Full text
Abstract:
When translating verbs from auditory perception, the translator often limits himself to a semantic and syntactic analysis of the predicates in a sentence. However, there is also an enunciative dimension (i.e. the relationship between the speaker and the subject of auditory perception) to be taken into account. Linguists divide the verbs of auditory perception into two groups according to cognitive criteria: verbs of passive and active perception. In Polish, many auditory perception verbs have a prefi x. In this article, we analyse how Polish passive auditory perception verbs and active auditory perception verbs are to be translated into Dutch.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Bernard, Corentin, Richard Kronland-Martinet, Madeline Fery, Sølvi Ystad, and Etienne Thoret. "Tactile perception of auditory roughness." JASA Express Letters 2, no. 12 (December 2022): 123201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/10.0016603.

Full text
Abstract:
Auditory roughness resulting from fast temporal beatings is often studied by summing two pure tones with close frequencies. Interestingly, the tactile counterpart of auditory roughness can be provided through touch with vibrotactile actuators. However, whether auditory roughness could also be perceived through touch and whether it exhibits similar characteristics are unclear. Here, auditory roughness perception and its tactile counterpart were evaluated using pairs of pure tone stimuli. Results revealed similar roughness curves in both modalities, suggesting similar sensory processing. This study attests to the relevance of such a paradigm for investigating auditory and tactile roughness in a multisensory fashion.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Crommett, Lexi E., Alexis Pérez-Bellido, and Jeffrey M. Yau. "Auditory adaptation improves tactile frequency perception." Journal of Neurophysiology 117, no. 3 (March 1, 2017): 1352–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00783.2016.

Full text
Abstract:
Our ability to process temporal frequency information by touch underlies our capacity to perceive and discriminate surface textures. Auditory signals, which also provide extensive temporal frequency information, can systematically alter the perception of vibrations on the hand. How auditory signals shape tactile processing is unclear; perceptual interactions between contemporaneous sounds and vibrations are consistent with multiple neural mechanisms. Here we used a crossmodal adaptation paradigm, which separated auditory and tactile stimulation in time, to test the hypothesis that tactile frequency perception depends on neural circuits that also process auditory frequency. We reasoned that auditory adaptation effects would transfer to touch only if signals from both senses converge on common representations. We found that auditory adaptation can improve tactile frequency discrimination thresholds. This occurred only when adaptor and test frequencies overlapped. In contrast, auditory adaptation did not influence tactile intensity judgments. Thus auditory adaptation enhances touch in a frequency- and feature-specific manner. A simple network model in which tactile frequency information is decoded from sensory neurons that are susceptible to auditory adaptation recapitulates these behavioral results. Our results imply that the neural circuits supporting tactile frequency perception also process auditory signals. This finding is consistent with the notion of supramodal operators performing canonical operations, like temporal frequency processing, regardless of input modality. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Auditory signals can influence the tactile perception of temporal frequency. Multiple neural mechanisms could account for the perceptual interactions between contemporaneous auditory and tactile signals. Using a crossmodal adaptation paradigm, we found that auditory adaptation causes frequency- and feature-specific improvements in tactile perception. This crossmodal transfer of aftereffects between audition and touch implies that tactile frequency perception relies on neural circuits that also process auditory frequency.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Pudov, V. I., and O. V. Zontova. "Hearing perception by cochlear implantation." Сенсорные системы 37, no. 4 (October 1, 2023): 320–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.31857/s0235009223040066.

Full text
Abstract:
Cochlear implantation is a unique development in the field of prosthetics of human sensory systems. Due to the electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve, auditory sensations close to natural occur. Despite significant progress in the engineering design of cochlear implants (CI), the quality of auditory perception when used is significantly limited. CI users experience the greatest difficulties in communication tasks such as understanding speech in noise or in multi-talkers environment. There are many factors, both technical and physiological, to reduce speech intelligibility in CI users. Speech perception in CI users is limited due to low frequency resolution, perceptual distortion of pitch, and compression of dynamic range. Low frequency resolution is the reason a decrease in speech intelligibility and the ability to perceive music. To realize these ability the question about the state of central hearing mechanisms, especially for children with congenital deafness, is crucial Neuroplasticity with ages decreases and the central auditory processing deficiency develops, therefore, it is desirable to carry out cochlear implantation as early as possible after hearing loss identification. Analysis of the auditory perception features in case of the auditory nerve is electrically excited allows not only to propose innovative approaches to improve the auditory abilities of CI users, but also to study auditory processing disorders.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Storms, Russell L., and Michael J. Zyda. "Interactions in Perceived Quality of Auditory-Visual Displays." Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 9, no. 6 (December 2000): 557–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/105474600300040385.

Full text
Abstract:
The quality of realism in virtual environments (VEs) is typically considered to be a function of visual and audio fidelity mutually exclusive of each other. However, the VE participant, being human, is multimodal by nature. Therefore, in order to validate more accurately the levels of auditory and visual fidelity that are required in a virtual environment, a better understanding is needed of the intersensory or crossmodal effects between the auditory and visual sense modalities. To identify whether any pertinent auditory-visual cross-modal perception phenomena exist, 108 subjects participated in three experiments which were completely automated using HTML, Java, and JavaScript programming languages. Visual and auditory display quality perceptions were measured intraand intermodally by manipulating the pixel resolution of the visual display and Gaussian white noise level, and by manipulating the sampling frequency of the auditory display and Gaussian white noise level. Statistically significant results indicate that high-quality auditory displays coupled with highquality visual displays increase the quality perception of the visual displays relative to the evaluation of the visual display alone, and that low-quality auditory displays coupled with high-quality visual displays decrease the quality perception of the auditory displays relative to the evaluation of the auditory display alone. These findings strongly suggest that the quality of realism in VEs must be a function of both auditory and visual display fidelities inclusive of each other.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

de Boer, J. N., M. M. J. Linszen, J. de Vries, M. J. L. Schutte, M. J. H. Begemann, S. M. Heringa, M. M. Bohlken, et al. "Auditory hallucinations, top-down processing and language perception: a general population study." Psychological Medicine 49, no. 16 (January 4, 2019): 2772–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s003329171800380x.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractBackgroundStudies investigating the underlying mechanisms of hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia suggest that an imbalance in top-down expectations v. bottom-up processing underlies these errors in perception. This study evaluates this hypothesis by testing if individuals drawn from the general population who have had auditory hallucinations (AH) have more misperceptions in auditory language perception than those who have never hallucinated.MethodsWe used an online survey to determine the presence of hallucinations. Participants filled out the Questionnaire for Psychotic Experiences and participated in an auditory verbal recognition task to assess both correct perceptions (hits) and misperceptions (false alarms). A hearing test was performed to screen for hearing problems.ResultsA total of 5115 individuals from the general Dutch population participated in this study. Participants who reported AH in the week preceding the test had a higher false alarm rate in their auditory perception compared with those without such (recent) experiences. The more recent the AH were experienced, the more mistakes participants made. While the presence of verbal AH (AVH) was predictive for false alarm rate in auditory language perception, the presence of non-verbal or visual hallucinations were not.ConclusionsThe presence of AVH predicted false alarm rate in auditory language perception, whereas the presence of non-verbal auditory or visual hallucinations was not, suggesting that enhanced top-down processing does not transfer across modalities. More false alarms were observed in participants who reported more recent AVHs. This is in line with models of enhanced influence of top-down expectations in persons who hallucinate.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Kaga, Makiko, Kaori Kon, Akira Uno, Toshihiro Horiguchi, Hitoshi Yoneyama, and Masumi Inagaki. "Auditory perception in auditory neuropathy: Clinical similarity with auditory verbal agnosia." Brain and Development 24, no. 3 (April 2002): 197–202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0387-7604(02)00027-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Leman, Marc. "A Model of Retroactive Tone-Center Perception." Music Perception 12, no. 4 (1995): 439–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285676.

Full text
Abstract:
In this paper, a model for tone-center perception is developed. It is based on an auditory model and principles of schema dynamics such as self-organization and association. The auditory module simulates virtual-pitch perception by transforming musical signals into auditory images. The schema- based module involves data-driven long-term learning for the self-organization of a schema for tone-center perception. The focus of this paper is on a retroactive process (called perceptual interpretation) by which the sense of tone center is adjusted according to a reconsideration of preceding perceptions in view of new contextual evidence within the schema. To this purpose, a metaphor is introduced, in which perceptual interpretation is described as the movement of a snaillike object in an attractor space. Additionally, the mathematical details of the model are presented, and the results of computer simulations are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

ERDENER, DOĞU, and DENIS BURNHAM. "Auditory–visual speech perception in three- and four-year-olds and its relationship to perceptual attunement and receptive vocabulary." Journal of Child Language 45, no. 2 (June 6, 2017): 273–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000917000174.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractDespite the body of research on auditory–visual speech perception in infants and schoolchildren, development in the early childhood period remains relatively uncharted. In this study, English-speaking children between three and four years of age were investigated for: (i) the development of visual speech perception – lip-reading and visual influence in auditory–visual integration; (ii) the development of auditory speech perception and native language perceptual attunement; and (iii) the relationship between these and a language skill relevant at this age, receptive vocabulary. Visual speech perception skills improved even over this relatively short time period. However, regression analyses revealed that vocabulary was predicted by auditory-only speech perception, and native language attunement, but not by visual speech perception ability. The results suggest that, in contrast to infants and schoolchildren, in three- to four-year-olds the relationship between speech perception and language ability is based on auditory and not visual or auditory–visual speech perception ability. Adding these results to existing findings allows elaboration of a more complete account of the developmental course of auditory–visual speech perception.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Williams, Jamal, Yuri Markov, Natalia Tiurina, and Viola Stoermer. "Auditory Context Alters Visual Perception." Journal of Vision 21, no. 9 (September 27, 2021): 2796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.9.2796.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Ottaviani, Laura, and Davide Rocchesso. "Auditory perception of 3D size." ACM Transactions on Applied Perception 1, no. 2 (October 2004): 118–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/1024083.1024086.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

McDermott, Josh H., Michael Schemitsch, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Summary statistics in auditory perception." Nature Neuroscience 16, no. 4 (February 24, 2013): 493–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3347.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Long, Glenis R. "Otoacoustic emissions and auditory perception." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 91, no. 4 (April 1992): 2408–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.403251.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Moore, Brian C. J. "Comparative Studies of Auditory Perception." Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 36, no. 4 (April 1991): 314–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/029630.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Schmuckler, Mark A., and David L. Gilden. "Auditory perception of fractal contours." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 19, no. 3 (1993): 641–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.19.3.641.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Repp, Bruno H., and Günther Knoblich. "Action Can Affect Auditory Perception." Psychological Science 18, no. 1 (January 2007): 6–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01839.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Carlile, Simon, and Johahn Leung. "The Perception of Auditory Motion." Trends in Hearing 20 (April 19, 2016): 233121651664425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2331216516644254.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Pollmann, Stefan, and Marianne Maertens. "Perception modulates auditory cortex activation." NeuroReport 17, no. 17 (November 2006): 1779–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0b013e3280107a98.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Griffiths, Timothy D., Virginia Penhune, Isabelle Peretz, Jenny L. Dean, Roy D. Patterson, and Gary G. R. Green. "Frontal processing and auditory perception." NeuroReport 11, no. 5 (April 2000): 919–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200004070-00004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Houben, Mark, Luuk Franssen, Dik Hermes, Armin Kohlrausch, and Berry Eggen. "Auditory perception of rolling balls." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 105, no. 2 (February 1999): 980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.425353.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Sandvad, Jesper. "Auditory perception of reverberant surroundings." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 105, no. 2 (February 1999): 1193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.425625.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Hariri, M. A., M. J. Connolly, M. V. Lakshmi, and S. Lafner. "Auditory Perception in Stroke Patients." Age and Ageing 22, suppl 3 (January 1, 1993): P23—P24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ageing/22.suppl_3.p23-d.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Binder, Jeffrey. "FUNCTIONAL IMAGING OF AUDITORY PERCEPTION." Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 13, no. 4 (July 1996): 351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004691-199607000-00029.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Ortega, L., E. Guzman-Martinez, M. Grabowecky, and S. Suzuki. "Auditory dominance in time perception." Journal of Vision 9, no. 8 (March 22, 2010): 1086. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/9.8.1086.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Viveiros, Carla Mherlyn, Liliane Desgualdo Pereira, and Gianna Mastroianni Kirsztajn. "Auditory Perception in Alport’s Syndrome." Brazilian Journal of Otorhinolaryngology 72, no. 6 (November 2006): 811–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1808-8694(15)31049-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Russell, Michael K. "Auditory Perception of Unimpeded Passage." Ecological Psychology 11, no. 2 (June 1999): 175–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1102_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

JELICIC, MARKO, and BENNO BONKE. "Auditory Perception During General Anesthesia." Southern Medical Journal 82, no. 10 (October 1989): 1220–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00007611-198910000-00005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Binder, Jeffrey. "Functional imaging of auditory perception." Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 102, no. 1 (January 1997): P5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0013-4694(97)86221-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Lebedev, V. G., and N. G. Zagoruiko. "Auditory perception and speech recognition." Speech Communication 4, no. 1-3 (August 1985): 97–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-6393(85)90038-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Alam, Iftekhar, and Ashok Ghatol. "High resolution auditory perception system." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 117, no. 4 (April 2005): 2483–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4787736.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Nazimek, J. M., M. D. Hunter, and P. W. R. Woodruff. "Auditory hallucinations: Expectation–perception model." Medical Hypotheses 78, no. 6 (June 2012): 802–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2012.03.014.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Bronkhorst, Adelbert W., and Tammo Houtgast. "Auditory distance perception in rooms." Nature 397, no. 6719 (February 1999): 517–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/17374.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Sininger, Yvonne S., and Anjali Bhatara. "Laterality of basic auditory perception." Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition 17, no. 2 (March 2012): 129–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650x.2010.541464.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Clifton, Rachel K., Eve E. Perris, and Andre Bullinger. "Infants' perception of auditory space." Developmental Psychology 27, no. 2 (1991): 187–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.187.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Olsho, Lynne Werner. "Infant auditory perception: Tonal masking." Infant Behavior and Development 8, no. 4 (October 1985): 371–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(85)90002-5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography